Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Opinion
  • Published:

The genetics of G in human and mouse

Abstract

The g factor refers to the substantial overlap that exists between individual differences in diverse cognitive processes in humans. In this article, I argue that a mouse model of g could provide a powerful analytic tool for exploring cognitive processes that are linked functionally by genes.

Key Points

Summary

  • Cognitive neuroscience focuses on species-universal processes rather than on differences between individuals. Heredity is based on the naturally occurring genetic variation that underlies individual differences. For complex traits, many genes contribute to heritable variation. Common disorders may be the quantitative extreme of the same genetic factors that contribute to variability throughout the distribution of individual differences.

  • One of the most consistent findings from individual-variability research on human cognitive abilities and disabilities is that very diverse processes such as general reasoning, spatial ability and vocabulary highly interrelate. A technique called factor analysis best captures this overlap and yields a g factor that accounts for about 40% of the variance of diverse cognitive tests. The existence of g does not imply that a single physical, physiological or psychological process is responsible for g.

  • There are more studies addressing the genetics of human g than any other trait. These studies consistently converge on the conclusion that genetic factors contribute substantially to g. Multivariate genetic analysis indicates that what is common between cognitive abilities is genetic in origin whereas what is specific to each cognitive ability is largely environmental.

  • An animal model of g is needed to apply sophisticated neurobiological techniques to understand the brain mechanisms that mediate genetic influences on g. Most of the research on individual differences and genetics in learning and memory has used mice than all other non-human species combined, and the general acceptance of g in man rekindled interest in g in rodents. Evidence for g has emerged from several studies of diverse cognitive tasks in mice.

  • More research is needed to prove that g in mice is the same as g in man. Investigating g in mice requires measures that are reliable at the level of the individual mouse, large samples, and a battery of diverse cognitive measures. The strongest genetic evidence for congruence of g in mouse and man will come from identifying genes that are associated with g in both mouse and man. The strongest neurobiological evidence for congruence will come from showing congruence of brain function across species.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Strain differences for performance on the Morris water maze.
Figure 2: Successful selective rat breeding on the basis of maze-learning performance.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Gazzaniga, M. S. (ed.) Cognitive Neuroscience: A Reader (Blackwell, Oxford, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Thompson, R. F. The Brain: A Neuroscience Primer 3rd edn (Worth, New York, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E. & McGuffin, P. Behavioral Genetics 4th edn (Worth, New York, 2001).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Zhang, Y. et al. Positional cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human homologue . Nature 372, 425–432 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Montague, C. T. et al. Congenital leptin deficiency is associated with severe early-onset obesity in humans. Nature 387, 903– 908 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Guthrie, R. The introduction of newborn screening for phenylketonuria: A personal history . Eur. J. Pediatr. 155, S4– 5 (1996).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Plomin, R., Owen, M. J. & McGuffin, P. The genetic basis of complex human behaviors. Science 264, 1733–1739 ( 1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Gayán, J. et al. Quantitative-trait locus for specific language and reading deficits on chromosome 6p. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 64, 157–164 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Carroll, J. B. Human Cognitive Abilities (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, 1993).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  10. Anderson, M. Intelligence and Development: A Cognitive Theory (Blackwell, Oxford, 1992).

    Google Scholar 

  11. Baddeley, A. & Gathercole, S. in Learning and Individual Differences: Process, Trait, and Content Determinants (eds Ackerman, P. L., Kyllonen, P. C. & Roberts, R. D.) 31–50 (American Psychological Association, Washington DC, 1999).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Deary, I. M. Looking Down on Human Intelligence: From Psychometrics to the Brain (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2000).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  13. Stauffer, J. M., Ree, M. J. & Carretta, T. R. Cognitive-components tests are not much more than g: an extension of Kyllonen's analyses. J. Gen. Psychol. 123, 193–205 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Spearman, C. 'General Intelligence' objectively determined and measured. Am. J. Psychol. 15, 201–293 (1904).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Jensen, A. R. The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability (Praeger, Westport, Connecticut, 1998).

    Google Scholar 

  16. Deary, I. J., Whalley, L. J., Lemmon, H., Crawford, J. R. & Starr, J. M. The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence 28, 49– 55 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Gottfredson, L. S. Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24, 79–132 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Salthouse, T. A. & Czaja, S. J. Structural constraints on process explanations in cognitive aging. Psychol. Aging 15, 44–55 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Brody, N. Intelligence 2nd edn (Academic, New York, 1992).

    Google Scholar 

  20. Carroll, J. B. Reflections on Stephen Jay Gould's The Mismeasure of Man. Intelligence 21, 121–134 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Mackintosh, N. J. IQ and Human Intelligence (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1998).

    Google Scholar 

  22. Gould, S. J. The Mismeasure of Man 2nd edn (W. W. Norton, New York, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  23. Gardner, H. Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences (Basic Books, New York, 1983).

    Google Scholar 

  24. Sternberg, R. J. Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1985).

    Google Scholar 

  25. Sternberg, R. J. & Gardner, M. K. in A Model for Intelligence (ed. Eysenck, J. H.) 231–254 (Springer–Verlag, New York, 1982).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  26. Fodor, J. A. The Modularity of Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1983).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  27. Pinker, S. The Language Instinct (William Morrow, New York, 1994 ).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  28. Bouchard, T. J. Jr & McGue, M. Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212, 1055–1059 (1981).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Fisher, P. J. et al. DNA pooling identifies QTLs for general cognitive ability in children on chromosome 4. Hum. Mol. Genet. 8, 915–922 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Plomin, R. in Molecular Genetics of Human Personality (eds Benjamin, J., Ebstein, R. & Belmaker, R. H.) (American Psychiatric Press, New York, in the press).

  31. Plomin, R. Genetics and general cognitive ability. Nature 402, C25–C29 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Detterman, D. K. in The Nature of Intelligence (eds Bock, G. R., Goode, J. A. & Webb, K.) 136–148 (Wiley, Chichester, Novartis Foundation Symposium 233, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  33. Kosslyn, S. & Plomin, R. in Psychiatric Neuroimaging Research: Contemporary Strategies (eds Dougherty, D., Rauch, S. L. & Rosenbaum, J. F.) 491–515 (American Psychiatric, Washington DC, 2001).

    Google Scholar 

  34. Anderson, B. in The Nature of Intelligence (eds Bock, G. R., Goode, J. A. & Webb, K.) 79–95 (Wiley, Chichester, Novartis Foundation Symposium 233, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  35. Matzel, L. D. & Gandhi, C. C. The tractable contribution of synapses and their component molecules to individual differences in learning . Behav. Brain Res. 110, 53– 66 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Chandra, S. B. C., Hosler, J. S. & Smith, B. H. Heritable variation for latent inhibition and its correlation with reversal learning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J. Comp. Psychol. 114, 86–97 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Sprott, R. L. & Staats, J. Behavioral studies using genetically defined mice — a bibliography. Behav. Genet. 5, 27–82 (1975).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Bovet, D. in Genetics, Environment and Intelligence (ed. Oliverio, A.) 79– 92 (North–Holland, Amsterdam, 1977).

    Google Scholar 

  39. Owen, E. H., Logue, S. F., Rasmussen, D. L. & Wehner, J. M. Assessment of learning by the Morris water task and fear conditioning in inbred mouse strains and F1 hybrids: implications of genetic background for single gene mutations and quantitative trait loci analyses. Neuroscience 80, 1087–1099 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Coren, S. The Intelligence of Dogs (Bantam Books, New York, 1994 ).

    Google Scholar 

  41. Tolman, E. in 39th Yearbook of the National Society for Studies of Education 111–119 (Public School Publishing Co., Bloomington, Illinois, 1924).

    Google Scholar 

  42. Tryon, R. C. The inheritance of maze-learning ability in rats. J. Comp. Psychol. 4, 1–8 (1940 ).

    Google Scholar 

  43. Heron, W. T. The inheritance of maze learning ability in rats. J. Comp. Psychol. 19, 77–89 ( 1935).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Heron, W. T. The inheritance of brightness and dullness in maze learning ability in the rat. J. Genet. Psychol. 59, 41– 49 (1941).

    Google Scholar 

  45. Thompson, W. R. The inheritance and development of intelligence. Proc. Assoc. Res. Nervous Mental Disorders 33, 209–231 (1954).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Thorndike, R. L. Organization of behavior in the albino rat. Genet. Psychol. Monographs 17, 1–70 (1935 ).

    Google Scholar 

  47. Tryon, R. C. in Comparative Psychology (ed. Moss, F. A.) 330– 365 (Prentice Hall, New York, 1946).

    Google Scholar 

  48. Anderson, B. Evidence from the rat for a general factor that underlies cognitive performance and that relates to brain size: intelligence? Neurosci. Lett. 153, 98–102 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Locurto, C. & Durkin, J. Problem-solving and individual differences in mice (Mus musculus) using water reinforcement. J. Comp. Psychol. (in the press).

  50. Locurto, C. & Scanlon, C. Individual differences and a spatial learning factor in two strains of mice (Mus musculus). J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 344–352 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Paya-Cano, J. L., Galsworthy, M., Stephenson, J., Plomin, R. Developing a mouse model for the functional investigation of cognitive abilities and disabilities. Am. J. Med. Genet. 96, 560 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Spearman, C. The Abilities of Man: Their Nature and Measurement 403 (Macmillan, New York, 1927).

    Google Scholar 

  53. Martin, N. G. & Eaves, L. J. The genetical analysis of covariance structure. Heredity 38, 79– 95 (1977).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Petrill, S. A. Molarity versus modularity of cognitive functioning? A behavioral genetic perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 6, 96–99 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Talbot, C. J. et al. High-resolution mapping of quantitative trait loci in outbred mice. Nature Genet. 21, 305– 308 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

M. Galsworthy, J. L. Paya-Cano and S. Monleon contributed to this essay and are collaborators in our ongoing research on g in HS mice funded in part by a grant from the US National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Related links

Related links

FURTHER INFORMATION

Robert Plomin's lab

Glossary

QUANTITATIVE TRAIT LOCI

(QTLs) Genes (loci) in multiple-gene systems in which each QTL contributes quantitatively to a continuous distribution, thus creating a quantitative trait.

PRINCIPAL COMPONENTS ANALYSIS

A statistical technique that weights tests to create a dimension that best represents the intercorrelations of the items.

MULTIVARIATE GENETIC ANALYSIS

A quantitative genetic technique that analyses the covariance between traits rather than the variance of each trait considered on its own.

GENETIC CORRELATION

A statistic from multivariate genetic analysis that indicates the extent to which genetic effects on one trait are the same as genetic effects on another trait.

HERITABILITY

A statistic that describes the extent to which individual differences in a population can be ascribed to genetic differences between individuals.

INBRED STRAINS

Animals produced by mating brother to sister for at least 20 generations, which results in homozygous animals with two copies of the same allele (form of gene) at all loci.

HEBB-WILLIAMS MAZE

Rectangular field with a start box a a goal box at opposite ends of the apparatus. Different configurations are obtained by placing barriers at different points of the field.

OUTBRED LINES

Animals produced by crossing inbred strains, which increases heterozygous animals with different alleles at all loci.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Plomin, R. The genetics of G in human and mouse. Nat Rev Neurosci 2, 136–141 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1038/35053584

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/35053584

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing