Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Sexual imprinting and optimal outbreeding

Abstract

WHEN Konrad Lorenz showed that early experiences can have a dramatic influence on the mating preferences of birds, he proposed that ‘sexual imprinting’, as the process came to be known in English, enables adults to recognise their own species1. Subsequent experimental work2,3 has, however, suggested that, although early experiences can indeed have long-lasting effects, a bird may also show a preference for members of its own species in the absence of experience with any of them except itself. The implication is that a bird may have a predisposing bias for its own species and sexual imprinting merely refines this bias in natural conditions. If birds are able to identify and mate with their own species without prior experience (and seeing or hearing themselves is irrelevant) what is the biological function of sexual imprinting? One possible answer is that sexual imprinting is required for recognition of close kin so that, by selecting mates that are slightly different, the animal is able to strike an optimal balance between inbreeding and outbreeding4. The prediction is that the strongest mating preference of a bird should be for something a little different (but not too different) from the object with which it had been imprinted. I report here that male Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica) mate with slightly unfamiliar females in preference to females to which they were exposed in early life, and that both types of female are preferred to those with a grossly unfamiliar type of plumage.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Lorenz, K. J. Orn. Lpz. 83, 137–213; 289–413 (1935).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Immelmann, K. Z. Tierpsychol. 26, 677–691 (1969).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Schutz, F. Z. Tierpsychol. 22, 50–103 (1965).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Bateson, P. P. G. in Biological Determinants of Sexual Behaviour (ed. Hutchison, J. B.) 29–53 (Wiley, London, 1978).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Gallagher, J. Behaviour 57, 91–114 (1976).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Gallagher, J. J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 91, 72–78 (1977).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Sonnemann, P. & Sjölander, S. Z. Tierpsychol. 45, 337–348 (1977).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Mather, K. Genetical Structure of Populations 1–197 (Chapman and Hall, London, 1973).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Bateson, P. P. G. Anim. Behav. (in the press).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

BATESON, P. Sexual imprinting and optimal outbreeding. Nature 273, 659–660 (1978). https://doi.org/10.1038/273659a0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/273659a0

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing