Abstract
Rauscher et al.1 reported that listening to ten minutes of Mozart's music increased the abstract reasoning ability of college students, as measured by IQ scores, by 8 or 9 points compared with listening to relaxation instructions or silence, respectively1. This startling finding became known as the ‘Mozart effect’, and has since been explored by several research groups. Here I use a meta-analysis to demonstrate that any cognitive enhancement is small and does not reflect any change in IQ or reasoning ability in general, but instead derives entirely from performance on one specific type of cognitive task and has a simple neuropsychological explanation.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 51 print issues and online access
$199.00 per year
only $3.90 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
References
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L. & Ky, K. N. Nature 365, 611 (1993).
Rosenthal, R. Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research (Sage, Newbury Park, 1991).
Rauscher, F. H. & Shaw, G. L. Percept. Motor Skills 86, 835–841 (1998).
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L. & Ky, K. N. Neurosci. Lett. 185, 44–47 (1995).
Ditunno, P. L. & Mann, V. A. Cortex 26, 177–188 (1990).
Heller, W. & Nitschke, J. B. Cogn. Emot. 11, 637–661 (1997).
Robertson, I. H., Mattingley, J. B., Rorden, C. & Driver, J. Nature 395, 169–172 (1998).
Nantais, K. M. & Schellenbergg, E. G. Psychol. Sci. 10, 370–373 (1999).
Hallam, S. Psychol. Music (submitted).
Steele, K. M., Bass, K. E. & Crook, M. D. Psychol. Sci. 10, 366–369 (1999).
Newman, J. et al. Percept. Motor Skills 81, 1379–1387 (1995).
Stough, C., Kerkin, B., Bates, T. & Mangan, G. Personal. Individ. Diff. 17, 695 (1994).
Kenealy, P. & Monsef, A. Psychologist 7, 346 (1994).
Steele, K. M. et al. Nature 400, 827 (1999).
Steele, K. M., Ball, T. N. & Runk, R. Percept. Motor Skills 84, 1179–1184 (1997).
Steele, K. M., Brown, J. D. & Stoecker, J. A. Percept. Motor Skills 88, 843–848 (1999).
Carstens, C. B. & De Fruscio, D. M. Percept. Motor Skills (submitted).
Carstens, C. B., Huskins, E. H. & Hounshell, G. W. Psychol. Rep. 77, 111–114 (1995).
Wilson, T. L. & Brown, T. L. J. Psychol. 131, 365–370 (1997).
Rideout, B. E. & Laubach, C. M. Percept. Motor Skills 82, 427–432 (1996).
Rideout, B. E. & Taylor, J. Percept. Motor Skills 85, 112–114 (1997).
Rideout, B. E., Dougherty, S. & Wernert, L. Percept. Motor Skills 86, 512–514 (1998).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Chabris, C. Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart effect’?. Nature 400, 826–827 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1038/23608
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/23608
This article is cited by
-
Music that is used while studying and music that is used for sleep share similar musical features, genres and subgroups
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Musical works are mind-independent artifacts
Synthese (2023)
-
The effects of music on spatial reasoning
Psychological Research (2020)
-
Is working memory training in older adults sensitive to music?
Psychological Research (2019)
-
The Sound of Silence: Can Imagining Music Improve Spatial Rotation Performance?
Current Psychology (2014)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.