INTRODUCTION

Anxiety is one of the symptoms that the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders of the American Psychiatric Association lists for the nicotine withdrawal syndrome in humans (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Self-report studies have generally indicated increased anxiety after cessation of cigarette smoking (Hughes et al, 1991; Zvolensky et al, 2005). Further, increased anxiety during withdrawal has been found to predict an inability to abstain from tobacco smoking (Pomerleau et al, 2005), highlighting the importance of understanding the neuropsychological basis of increased anxiety during cigarette and nicotine withdrawal.

In behavioral rodent experiments, anxiety often is assessed in tests that are based on the conflicting drives to explore novel, potentially rewarding stimuli and environments, and avoid potentially threatening conditions. Using such tests, it has been shown that withdrawal from chronic nicotine administration leads to anxiogenic-like effects in the light–dark box and elevated plus maze tests in DBA/2J and C57BL/6J mice (Costall et al, 1989; Jonkman et al, 2005; Damaj et al, 2003) and the elevated plus maze and social interaction test in rats (Irvine et al, 2001; Pandey et al, 2001; Cheeta et al, 2001). Thus, nicotine withdrawal is associated with an anxiogenic-like effect in exploration-based tests in rodents, at least in relatively high stress environments (eg novelty, bright light, open spaces).

In addition to conflict-based models of anxiety, there is a class of anxiety tests that assesses passive reflex reactivity and does not involve measures of exploratory behavior or a conflict situation. These tests rely on the phenomenon that the acoustic startle response is augmented during threat in mammals (Brown et al, 1951; Davis, 1998). Startle reactivity has been shown to be increased during presentation of conditioned and unconditioned aversive stimuli, such as a shock-paired cue light and foot shock (Davis et al, 1989; Davis, 1989), and after presentation of more ethologically threatening stimuli, such as bright illumination and predator odor (Walker and Davis, 1997; Hebb et al, 2003). With such startle-based tests, it has been shown that defensive behavior can be dissociated phenomenologically and anatomically into a fast onset, fast offset response system to specific threats, and a slow onset, slow offset response system to potential and unpredictable threats (Walker et al, 2003). The former system, referred to as the fear system, is critically dependent on the integrity of the central nucleus of the amygdala, and can be modeled with cue-enhanced startle. The latter system, referred to as the anxiety system, is critically dependent on the integrity of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), and can be modeled with light-enhanced startle (LES) and corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) administration (Walker and Davis, 1997; de Jongh et al, 2003). Because startle reactivity is a cross-species defensive behavior that has translational value for understanding the mechanisms of anxiety (Risbrough and Stein, 2006), this test could provide important information on how nicotine withdrawal affects anxiety systems.

Several studies have examined the effect of nicotine withdrawal on baseline startle amplitudes. Unfortunately, the results of these studies have been inconsistent. It has been reported that startle amplitudes were unchanged during nicotine withdrawal in DBA/2J and C57BL/6J mice (Semenova et al, 2003, Jonkman et al, 2005) and Sprague–Dawley, Wistar, and Long–Evans rats (Acri et al, 1991, 1995), while increases in startle amplitude have been reported in Long–Evans rats (Helton et al, 1993, 1997; Rasmussen et al, 1996, 1997, 2000). Interestingly, however, the studies that showed increases in startle amplitudes during nicotine withdrawal tested subjects for startle reactivity in ambient lighting conditions (Kurt Rasmussen, personal communication), suggesting that perhaps the presence of anxiogenic illumination may be a critical factor in the effect of nicotine withdrawal on startle reactivity. Importantly, the effects of nicotine withdrawal on baseline startle reactivity cannot be ascribed to any specific psychological process such as anxiety because a wide variety of manipulations, some of them putatively orthogonal to anxiety mechanisms (eg strychnine-induced startle), may increase the brainstem acoustic startle response (Koch, 1999).

In contrast, the LES test allows the investigation of the effects of nicotine withdrawal on anxiety-related processes. LES involves the measurement of a reactive response in a stressful situation. In this procedure, startle responses are measured in successive sessions during which the startle chambers are either dark or brightly lit. The startle response is potentiated by the aversive bright light in a slow onset, slow offset manner, and this response is selectively reduced by anxiolytic compounds (de Jongh et al, 2002; Walker and Davis, 2002). Hence, the degree to which light enhances startle reactivity is used as an operational measure of anxiety.

In the present study, we investigated the potential anxiogenic effects of withdrawal from chronic nicotine administration in the LES test in Wistar rats. We hypothesized that both spontaneous and precipitated nicotine withdrawal would specifically enhance the aversive effect of light-potentiating startle reactivity in light, but not dark, testing conditions.

METHODS

Animals

Male Wistar rats (Charles River, Raleigh, NC) were housed in groups of two. Food and water were available ad libitum in the home cages. All rats were kept on a 12-h reverse light/dark cycle (lights off from 0700 to 1900) in a colony room with temperature and humidity remaining constant. On testing days, animals were transferred from the animal facility in transparent individual holding cages and placed in a dark and quiet room for at least 60 min prior to the beginning of testing.

Apparatus

Rats were placed in cylindrical Plexiglas chambers mounted on a Plexiglas platform inside a ventilated, sound-attenuated chamber (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA). A high-frequency loudspeaker was located directly above the cylindrical Plexiglas chamber housing the rat. Measurement of the startle response is described in detail elsewhere (Mansbach et al, 1988). Briefly, the flinch response of the animal was measured using a piezoelectric unit mounted at the bottom of the platform, which sends a digitized signal to the computer. Monthly calibration was performed on the chambers to ensure the accuracy of the sound levels and measurements. Each startle apparatus was equipped with a compact fluorescent light bulb (Commercial Electric, Model no. EDXO-23) inside the sound-attenuating chamber that produced light intensities between 2700 and 3600 lx measured inside the Plexiglas cylinder using a Minolta Auto meter IV F luxmeter.

Drugs

Nicotine hydrogen tartrate salt and mecamylamine hydrochloride were purchased from Sigma Chemical Co. (St Louis, MO) and dihydro-β-erythroidine hydrobromide (DHβE) was purchased from Research Biochemicals (Natick, MA). All drugs were dissolved in physiological saline (0.9% sodium chloride). Nicotine was delivered subcutaneously (s.c.) through implantation of an osmotic minipump, whereas mecamylamine and DHβE were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) in a volume of 1 ml/kg body weight. Nicotine doses are reported as base, and nicotinic receptor antagonist doses are reported as salt.

Surgeries

Osmotic minipumps (Alzet model 2ML4 28 day pumps, Alza Corporation, Palo Alto, CA) filled with either saline or nicotine hydrogen tartrate dissolved in saline were implanted s.c. under isoflurane/oxygen (1–3% isoflurane) anesthesia. The nicotine concentration was adjusted based on the body weight of animals to deliver an average dose of 3.16 mg/kg per day nicotine base (9 mg/kg per day nicotine tartrate).

Light-Enhanced Startle

The LES procedures were based on those of de Jongh et al (2005). One LES measurement was based on four startle tests (termed blocks), which were conducted in two separate sessions of two blocks each. At the beginning of the startle tests (block 1 of the session), the rats were placed in the startle chamber and left undisturbed in the dark for 5 min. Thereafter, the rats were presented with 30 startle stimuli, 10 each at 90, 95, and 105 dB, with an average interstimulus interval of 30 s, presented in a pseudorandom order and in dark conditions. These 30 stimuli constituted the first startle block. After the first block, the rats were removed from the chamber and placed in their holding cage in an adjacent dark and quiet room for 5 min (spontaneous withdrawal experiment) or 15 min (precipitated withdrawal experiments). After this interval, rats were placed back into the startle chambers for the second block of the session, which was exactly the same as the first block, except that it was presented either in the dark (dark → dark session) or bright light (dark → light session) condition. Thus, one full test of LES consisted of two separate test sessions, one dark → dark to measure habituation of the startle reflex, and one dark → light to measure the startle-enhancing effects of the light. The rats were tested with at least 48 h between these two sessions for the precipitated withdrawal experiments, or on the same day at least 6 h apart for the spontaneous withdrawal experiment. The order of sessions (block 2 in either the dark or light condition) was counterbalanced across animals within groups. That is, half of the rats started the experiment with a dark → dark session, and the other half began with a dark → light session. For precipitated withdrawal experiments, nicotinic receptor antagonists were injected immediately after the first block because of concerns that injection before the first block may have resulted in a reduced precipitated nicotine withdrawal state by the time the critical second block of the session started, due to dissipation of the drug. To this end, the interval between block 1 and block 2 in precipitated nicotine withdrawal experiments was extended to 15 min to allow distribution of the administered nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) antagonists in the central nervous system before initiation of the second block.

Experimental Design

Prior to implantation of minipumps, all rats were given one startle session in which the light was on in the second block. Rats were then assigned to nicotine and saline pump conditions so that they were counterbalanced for both average peak startle value in the first block (baseline startle in the dark) and percentage increase of startle value in the second block (LES). The minipumps were implanted, and the rats were left undisturbed for at least 7 days before the first precipitated nicotine withdrawal test to allow nicotine ‘dependence’ to develop. Previous studies indicate that 6 days of nicotine exposure is sufficient to allow the detection of precipitated nicotine withdrawal effects on intracranial self-stimulation thresholds upon administration of nAChR antagonists (eg Bruijnzeel and Markou, 2004; Skjei and Markou, 2003). The minipumps were surgically removed 28 days after implantation, and spontaneous nicotine/saline withdrawal was assessed in the LES test 24 h after minipump removal.

The results reported here are derived from three experiments that were conducted sequentially. Naive rats were used in each of the three experiments. In the first experiment, saline- (n=5) and nicotine-treated (n=5) rats were left undisturbed for 28 days until the pumps were removed, and spontaneous nicotine withdrawal was assessed 20–28 h later with one dark → dark and one dark → light session conducted at least 6 h apart.

In the second experiment, a separate group of naive rats was treated with saline (n=10) or nicotine (n=10) for 28 days and tested for DHβE-precipitated nicotine withdrawal in the LES test, while the rats were treated with nicotine or saline via the subcutaneous osmotic minipumps that were removed on day 28 of pump exposure after the completion of DHβE testing. DHβE was injected immediately after the first block of the session, and the rats were left undisturbed for 15 min before the second block commenced. The effects of DHβE on LES in saline- and nicotine-treated rats were assessed using a within-subjects pseudorandom design, where each rat received i.p. injections of saline, 1.5, 3, and 6 mg/kg DHβE with a washout period of at least 48 h between each injection. This procedure resulted in a total of eight injections, administered in four dark → dark and four dark → light sessions, all conducted at least 48 h apart. The DHβE injections were administered between days 7 and 24 of saline/nicotine exposure. Previous work has indicated that these conditions result in reliable precipitated nicotine withdrawal, as assessed by elevations in intracranial self-stimulation thresholds (Epping-Jordan et al, 1998; Skjei and Markou, 2003; Watkins et al, 2000). The minipumps were removed 28 days after implantation, and spontaneous nicotine withdrawal was assessed 20–28 h later with one dark → dark and one dark → light session conducted at least 6 h apart. Previous work has indicated that such nicotine exposure results in reliable and robust spontaneous nicotine withdrawal as assessed by both elevations in intracranial self-stimulation thresholds and somatic signs of withdrawal (Skjei and Markou, 2003).

In the third experiment, a separate set of naive rats was treated with saline (n=12) or nicotine (n=10) for 28 days and tested for mecamylamine-precipitated nicotine withdrawal in the LES test, while the rats were treated with nicotine or saline via the subcutaneous osmotic minipumps that were removed on day 28 of pump exposure after the completion of mecamylamine testing. Mecamylamine was injected immediately after the first block of the session, and the rats were left undisturbed for 15 min before the second block commenced. The effects of mecamylamine on LES in saline- and nicotine-treated rats were assessed using a within-subjects pseudorandom design, where each rat received i.p. injections of saline, 1, 2, and 4 mg/kg mecamylamine with a washout period of at least 48 h between each injection. This procedure resulted in a total of eight injections, administered in four dark → dark and four dark → light sessions, all conducted at least 48 h apart. The mecamylamine injections were administered between days 7 and 24 of saline/nicotine exposure. The pumps were removed 28 days after implantation, and spontaneous nicotine withdrawal was assessed 20–28 h later with one dark → dark and one dark → light session conducted at least 6 h apart.

The spontaneous nicotine withdrawal data from these three experiments were pooled. Rats that showed mild signs of unhealed or open post-pump removal incisions before spontaneous withdrawal testing were excluded from the spontaneous withdrawal analyses, yielding a total of 25 saline-treated and 24 nicotine-treated rats.

Statistical Analyses

The effect of spontaneous nicotine withdrawal on baseline startle values was analyzed with an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Nicotine (nicotine- or saline-containing pump) as a between-subjects factor, and Session (Dark → Dark or Dark → Light) and Block (first or second) as within-subjects factors. Due to consistent interactions of pulse intensity with the other factors, data for the three startle pulse intensities were subsequently analyzed separately. To assess the effect of spontaneous nicotine withdrawal on LES, a difference score of the peak startle value was calculated for each session (Dark → Dark and Dark → Light). This difference score was calculated by subtracting the startle value of the first block from that of the second block. The difference scores were analyzed with an ANOVA with Nicotine/Saline as the between-subject factor and Session as the within-subjects factor.

The effects of nicotinic receptor antagonist-precipitated nicotine withdrawal on baseline startle values were assessed by analyzing data from the second block only because the nicotinic antagonists were injected after the first block, before assessment in the second startle block. The data were analyzed with an ANOVA with Nicotine/Saline treatment (ie pump content) as the between-subjects factor, and Dose (separate analyses for DHβE and mecamylamine experiments) and Session as the within-subjects factors. Statistically significant interactions were further analyzed with Tukey's post hoc comparisons. The criterion for significance was set at p<0.05.

RESULTS

Spontaneous Nicotine Withdrawal

Twenty-four hours after pump removal, baseline startle levels were not altered (Nicotine/Saline: F1,47<1, NS). There was a significant Session × Block interaction (F1,47=17.04, p<0.001), indicating that startle was indeed significantly potentiated during the light session across all groups. However, there was also a significant Session × Block × Pulse intensity interaction (F2,94=13.57, p<0.001). Subsequent separate analyses of the three pulse intensity startle values revealed that LES was only significant at the highest startle intensity (105 dB) across both treatment groups (Session × Block: 90 dB: F1,47=3.49, NS; 95 dB: F1,47=3.97, NS; 105 dB: F1,47=17.02, p<0.001; Table 1). Thus, subsequent analyses of the effect of Nicotine/Saline withdrawal on LES were conducted using only the 105 dB startle data. Analysis of the 105 dB pulse data revealed no significant effect of Nicotine/Saline withdrawal on baseline startle values (F1,47 <1, ns; Figure 1). However, Nicotine/Saline treatment did interact with the light/dark condition (Nicotine × Session × Block: F1,47=6.06, p<0.025, Figure 1), indicating that nicotine withdrawal significantly potentiated LES. Hence, there was a light-potentiated startle effect in the 105 dB pulse data, and this effect was significantly increased by nicotine withdrawal.

Table 1 Light-Enhanced Startle in Wistar Rats Across the Three Startle Pulse Intensities Used in the Present Studies
Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of spontaneous nicotine withdrawal on baseline peak startle and LES in Wistar rats for 105 dB startle pulse data. Data are mean±SEM peak startle response. Rats were administered saline or 3.16 mg/kg per day of nicotine (base) for 28 days, and tested in the LES 24 h after removal of the nicotine/saline-containing minipump. The LES test consists of one session with two consecutive startle blocks in the dark (Dark 1.1 and Dark 1.2) and a second session with two consecutive startle blocks with a bright light turned on in the second block (Dark 2.1 and Light 2.2). There was no effect of nicotine withdrawal on baseline startle. There was a significant LES effect on startle reactivity to the 105 dB pulse, and nicotine withdrawal significantly potentiated this LES effect. Asterisk (*p<0.025) indicates a significant effect of nicotine withdrawal on LES. Hash (#p<0.001) indicates a significant light-enhanced startle effect. Group sizes were nicotine (n=24) and saline (n=25).

Analysis of the difference scores for the 105 dB startle values subsequently confirmed that there was a significant overall effect of the light condition on startle reactivity, which was significantly enhanced in the nicotine-withdrawing group (Session: F1,47=17.02, p<0.001; Session × Nicotine: F1,47=6.06, p<0.025; Figure 2). Posthoc analysis with Tukey's test revealed that this interaction reflected a significant increase in difference scores during nicotine withdrawal in the Dark → Light (Nicotine/Saline: F1,47=8.86, p<0.01), but not the Dark → Dark session (Nicotine: F1,47<1, NS). Thus, nicotine withdrawal selectively increased startle during the light sessions.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effect of spontaneous nicotine withdrawal on difference scores of peak startle response to a 105 dB stimulus between the dark and the light session in Wistar rats. Rats were administered saline or 3.16 mg/kg per day nicotine (base) for 28 days and tested 24 h after removal of the minipump. The difference scores were calculated by subtracting the average peak startle response in the first block from that in the second block for both sessions, yielding average Dark 1.2–Dark 1.1 (session 1) and Light 2.2–Dark 2.1 (session 2) difference scores (±SEM). There was a main effect of Session (reflecting LES) and an interaction between Nicotine/Saline withdrawal and Session that was due to a specific increase in the light–dark (but not dark–dark) difference scores by nicotine withdrawal. Asterisks (**p<0.01) indicate a significant increase in light–dark difference scores associated with nicotine withdrawal. Hash (#p<0.001) indicates a significant light-enhanced startle effect. Group sizes were nicotine (n=24) and saline (n=25).

There was no main effect of order of session presentation (dark–dark vs dark–light) on LES, nor was there an interaction of that factor with Nicotine/Saline (F1,45=2.11, NS). We did observe a main effect of rat cohort (data were pooled from three different startle experiments) on overall startle reactivity (F2,37=6.27, p<0.01), due to the initial nicotine group from the first experiment exhibiting lower baseline startle than the DHβE- or mecamylamine-treated cohorts (p<0.05, Tukey's test). There was no interaction, however, of rat cohort with Session, Phase, or Nicotine/Saline (data not shown), supporting the combination of the spontaneous withdrawal data from the three rat cohorts to examine the effects of nicotine withdrawal on LES.

Precipitated Nicotine Withdrawal

DHβE

Startle values and difference scores were only analyzed for the 105 dB pulse data, because, as mentioned above, the 90 and 95 dB pulse intensities did not consistently produce LES. The 105 dB peak startle values were neither affected by chronic nicotine administration (F1,18<1, NS, Figure 3) nor DHβE injections (F3,54=1.23, NS). Further, there was no interaction of the factors Nicotine/Saline and DHβE dose (F3,54 <1, NS). There was a significant effect of Session, reflecting LES (F1,18=40.95, p<0.001), but this effect did not interact significantly with chronic Nicotine/Saline administration (F1,18=1.91, NS) or DHβE injections (F3,54 <1, NS). Further, there was no three-way interaction between the factors Session, Nicotine/Saline, and DHβE dose (F3,54 <1, NS). Finally, there was no effect of order of testing (F3,54<1, NS), and no interaction of order of testing with Nicotine/Saline exposure or DHβE/saline injections (F3,54<1, NS). Thus, DHβE-precipitated nicotine withdrawal did not increase LES.

Figure 3
figure 3

Effect of dihydro-β-erythroidine hydrobromide (DHβE)-precipitated nicotine withdrawal on peak startle reactivity to a 105 dB startle pulse in Wistar rats exposed through osmotic minipumps to saline or 3.16 mg/kg per day nicotine (base). DHβE (within-subjects Latin square, saline, 1.5, 3, and 6 mg/kg) was injected intraperitoneally immediately after the first block of the session and the second block commenced 15 min later. Subcutaneous minipumps delivering saline or nicotine were present throughout the DHβE experiment (see the ‘Methods’ section for details). The startle values depicted are averages of the second (post-injection) block (±SEM). DHβE did not affect baseline startle or LES differentially between nicotine- and saline-treated rats. Group sizes were nicotine (n=10) and saline (n=10).

Mecamylamine

As previously stated, startle values and difference scores were only analyzed for the 105 dB pulse data. The 105 dB peak startle values were significantly affected by mecamylamine injections (F3,60=3.01, p<0.05), but this effect did not interact with Session (F3,60=2.73, NS), reflecting a general suppressive effect of mecamylamine on startle. Startle reactivity was reduced during both the dark and light blocks (Figure 4). The effect of mecamylamine injections did not interact with chronic Nicotine/Saline administration (F3,60<1, NS). There was a significant effect of Session, reflecting LES (F1,20=28.66, p<0.001), but this effect did not interact significantly with chronic Nicotine/Saline administration and Mecamylamine injections (Session × Nicotine × Dose: F3,60=1.44, NS). Finally, there was no effect of order of testing (F3,60<1, NS), and no interaction of order of testing with Nicotine/Saline exposure and Mecamylamine/Saline injections (F3,60=1.10, NS). Thus, mecamylamine-precipitated nicotine withdrawal did not increase LES. Mecamylamine had a general suppressive effect on startle magnitudes that did not interact with Nicotine/Saline treatment or LES.

Figure 4
figure 4

Effect of mecamylamine-precipitated nicotine withdrawal on peak startle reactivity to a 105 dB startle pulse in Wistar rats exposed through osmotic minipumps to saline or 3.16 mg/kg per day nicotine (base). Mecamylamine (within-subjects Latin square, saline, 1, 2, and 4 mg/kg) was injected intraperitoneally immediately after the first block of a session, and the second block commenced 15 min later. Subcutaneous minipumps delivering saline or nicotine were present throughtout the mecanylamine experiment (see the ‘Methods’ section for details). The startle values depicted are averages of the second (post-injection) block (±SEM). Mecamylamine did not affect baseline startle or LES differentially between nicotine- and saline-treated rats. Mecamylamine induced a significant decrease in startle values that was independent of session (Dark or Light) and Nicotine/Saline withdrawal. Group sizes were nicotine (n=10) and saline (n=12).

DISCUSSION

The present results show that spontaneous nicotine withdrawal significantly increases startle reactivity during light stress, but has no effect on baseline startle responding measured in dark conditions. This result suggests that nicotine withdrawal does not induce anxiety levels similar to those induced by the presence of aversive bright light or intracerebroventricular administration of CRF (Walker et al, 2003), conditions that increase baseline startle responsivity. Instead, nicotine withdrawal appears to selectively increase anxiety-like reactivity in an aversive environment (ie brightly lit environment) in rats.

An important distinction of the presently used procedure of light-potentiated startle compared to previously used conflict tests is that the LES paradigm provides an internal baseline (startle reactivity during the dark block) to determine the specificity of the experimental manipulation to anxiety-like responding. The present data indicate that nicotine withdrawal affected anxiety responding only in the presence of another stressor, which corresponds well to smoking cessation findings in humans. Specifically, nicotine-withdrawing smokers report increased craving, negative affect symptoms, and somatic symptoms when presented with stressful stimuli (Beckham et al, 1996). Most importantly, low-state anxiety levels are poor predictors of craving and withdrawal in smokers, while high-state anxiety levels are closely linked to these measures (Pomerleau et al, 1990; Perkins and Grobe, 1992; for review see al’Absi, 2006). These data support the notion that behavioral and physiological signs of stress that show exacerbation with nicotine withdrawal may provide predictive screens for smoking cessation treatments.

The finding that nicotine withdrawal does not increase startle responsivity when assessed in the dark is in agreement with reports of unchanged startle during spontaneous nicotine withdrawal in DBA/2J and C57BL/6J mice (Semenova et al, 2003; Jonkman et al, 2005), and Sprague–Dawley, Wistar, and Long–Evans rats (Acri et al, 1991, 1995), though increases have been reported in Long–Evans rats (Helton et al, 1993, 1997; Rasmussen et al, 1996, 1997, 2000). Interestingly, however, the studies that reported increases in baseline startle reactivity were conducted in transparent startle testing boxes that allowed ample white room light to enter (Kurt Rasmussen, personal communication). Thus, the conditions in the previously published experiments that reported increases in startle reactivity during spontaneous nicotine withdrawal are more similar to our LES conditions, where we did observe increases in startle amplitude, than our standard startle testing in the dark where we did not observe increases in startle during spontaneous nicotine withdrawal. Consequently, the present set of results may have resolved a long-standing discrepancy in the literature about increases or lack of changes in startle reactivity during nicotine withdrawal in rodents. Together with reports of increases in anxiety-like behavior during nicotine withdrawal in various conflict-based models of anxiety (Costall et al, 1989; Jonkman et al, 2005; Damaj et al, 2003; Irvine et al, 2001; Pandey et al, 2001; Cheeta et al, 2001), these results indicate that nicotine withdrawal increases anxiety-like behavior during stress (eg novelty, bright light). The present results are one of the first demonstrations that nicotine withdrawal increases anxiety-like reactivity in a nonlocomotor-based measure.

The lack of effect of nAChR antagonists on LES was surprising. It has been well documented that the administration of these nicotinic receptor antagonists to nicotine-treated animals precipitates a depression-like anhedonic effect (ie elevations in intracranial self-stimulation thresholds; Epping-Jordan et al, 1998; Watkins et al, 2000; Skjei and Markou, 2003), and somatic signs of nicotine withdrawal in the case of mecamylamine (Watkins et al, 2000; Malin et al, 1992, 1994) and hyperalgesia (Damaj et al, 2003), similar to that observed during spontaneous nicotine withdrawal. One possible explanation for these findings is that the doses of mecamylamine and DHβE administered were not high enough to precipitate the increase in anxiety-like reactivity seen in spontaneous withdrawal. However, this explanation is unlikely. Both the depression-like and somatic behavioral effects of spontaneous nicotine withdrawal are precipitated in rats with mecamylamine or DHβE doses that are lower than the highest doses used in the present study (Epping-Jordan et al, 1998; Watkins et al, 2000; Hildebrand et al, 1997; Malin et al, 1994). It is also possible that antagonist precipitated withdrawal was not observed because of the multiple repeated nAchR antagonist-precipitated withdrawals experienced and the shorter exposure to nicotine than in the spontaneous withdrawal experiment; these conditions may not have allowed for neuroadaptive changes to nicotine exposure to occur that may mediate the increased sensitivity of nicotine-exposed animals to the stress of light. Another interpretation is that the increase in anxiety-like behavior observed after spontaneous withdrawal may be mediated by nicotine acetylcholine receptor subtypes other than the α4β2 nAChR subtype for which DHβE has the highest selectivity (Harvey and Luetje, 1996), and β4-containing nicotinic receptors for which mecamylamine shows some limited selectivity (Chavez-Noriega et al, 1997; Papke et al, 2001; although it should be emphasized that generally mecamylamine is nonselective) such as the α7 homomeric nAChR subtype.

Alternatively, the increase in anxiety-like reactivity that we observed may not have arisen immediately after cessation of nicotine administration. Spontaneous and precipitated nicotine withdrawal both induce reduced activation of nAChRs, but they differ markedly in the time course that elapses between the initial decrease in nAChR activation and behavioral testing. In nicotinic receptor antagonist-precipitated nicotine withdrawal, receptor blockade is immediate. Due to drug metabolism, this blockade may dissipate before downstream effects of such antagonism take effect. Thus, the observed increases in anxiety-like behavior only during spontaneous, but not precipitated, nicotine withdrawal may be attributable to downstream consequences of reduced nicotinic activation that affect anxiety-like behavior. Indeed, it was recently reported that the intracranial self-stimulation threshold elevations observed during immediate precipitated withdrawal were reversible by a CRF receptor antagonist, while the same threshold elevations observed during later spontaneous withdrawal were not reversible by the same CRF receptor antagonist (Bruijnzeel et al, 2007). These findings suggest that the induction of nicotine withdrawal by blockade of nAChRs depends on CRF receptor activation, while the more prolonged spontaneous nicotine withdrawal phase is sustained by downstream consequences that are no longer immediately dependent on CRF receptor activation.

Because the LES effect depends critically on the integrity of the BNST (Walker and Davis, 1997), and given the receptor antagonist-precipitated, but not spontaneous, nicotine withdrawal dependence on CRF mechanisms (Bruijnzeel et al, 2007), enhanced LES may be a downstream consequence of CRF receptor activation. Interestingly, increased norepinephrine release after CRF administration is observed after substantial delays (Palamarchouk et al, 2002), and the administration of clonidine, which reduces norepinephrine release by acting as an agonist at presynaptic inhibitory α2 receptor (Pudovkina et al, 2001) and is used as an aid for smoking cessation (Frishman, 2007), into the BNST abolishes the LES effect under baseline conditions (ie no nicotine withdrawal; Schweimer et al, 2005). Therefore, we speculate that CRF may be released during early nicotine withdrawal, which could lead to a delayed norepinephrine effect, which may in turn potentiate LES. These hypotheses deserve further study.

In summary, we show here that nicotine withdrawal modulates the effectiveness of an anxiogenic stimulus, without affecting baseline startle reactivity in Wistar rats. These results suggest that smokers who plan to quit smoking should minimize anxiogenic circumstances during early withdrawal because the effects of these stressors may be potentiated during nicotine withdrawal.