INTRODUCTION

Binge eating is abnormal feeding behavior characterized by discrete episodes of rapid, excessive food consumption. Binge episodes, diagnostic features of several eating disorders, typically involve palatable foods rich in sugar and fat and a ‘loss of control’ (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Corwin and Buda-Levin, 2004; Yanovski, 2003). Binge eating is more prevalent in obese individuals and, conversely, binge eaters are frequently obese (Pike et al, 2001; Smith et al, 1998). Accordingly, binge eating is a hypothesized etiologic risk factor for obesity (Hudson et al, 2007). A high comorbidity between binge eating and dysphoria has been observed and the causal nature of this relationship remains uncertain (Blazer et al, 1994; Gluck, 2006).

Current binge eating models emphasize a role for dietary restraint in promoting binge behavior (Howard and Porzelius, 1999), with many animal models positing that a history of quantitative food restriction, modeled by limiting the daily caloric ration (eg 66% of daily intake) (Hagan et al, 2003) or the duration of daily food access (eg 2 h) (Inoue et al, 2004), is central to bingeing. However, an alternative conceptualization might emphasize the qualitative aspect of dietary restraint, namely the attempted abstinence of binge eaters from ‘forbidden,’ palatable foods (Kales, 1990; Knight and Boland, 1989; Fletcher et al, 2007; Mitchell and Brunstrom, 2005; Gonzalez and Vitousek, 2004; Stirling and Yeomans 2004; Corwin, 2006; Corwin and Buda-Levin, 2004). Binge eaters restrict their intake of ‘forbidden’ foods to the point that ‘relapse’ intake is limited to very brief, often ritualized, binge episodes, perhaps precipitated by mild energy restriction.

Reciprocal to the binge hyperphagia of ‘forbidden’ foods, the prospect of (predictable experience with) palatable food, leads to refusal of otherwise acceptable alternatives in humans, described by some as finickiness (Pliner et al, 1990). Such a learned change in food acceptance may increase risk for body weight dysregulation and eating disorders because of the increased role placed on sensory–hedonic, rather than nutritional, properties of food for controlling intake (Wardle et al, 2001). Perhaps an analog of this learned change in food acceptance, negative contrast in rodents refers to the hypophagia of an otherwise acceptable tastant that results from having predictably received access to a more preferred substance either immediately before (successive negative contrast) or subsequent to (anticipatory negative contrast) that substance (Flaherty and Checke, 1982; Flaherty and Rowan, 1986; Flaherty et al, 1995). Contrast effects have been studied previously using limitedly available (3–5 min) sweet solutions in weight-restricted rats, but have not yet been well-studied vis-à-vis day-to-day food acceptance/intake in subjects' self-determining body weight.

Thus, both binge eating and refusal of less preferred, but perhaps healthier, foods may represent learned behavioral adaptations to contrasting sensory–hedonic experience with food across time. Previously, these phenomena have been segregated to different fields of study and not considered for their possible relatedness. The present study sought to test the hypothesis that rats with highly limited access to preferred food would jointly develop binge-like eating of the preferred diet and anticipatory negative contrast, or hypophagia, of the otherwise acceptable, preceding chow diet. Thus, the main aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that binge eating and anticipatory negative contrast are joint phenomena with shared etiologic roots, as exemplified following intermittent access to palatable food.

A second aim of the study was to test the hypothesis that the preferential μ/κ opioid-receptor antagonist nalmefene would jointly reduce not only binge-like intake of the highly preferred food, but also differentially modulate intake of the less preferred chow according to diet history. Nalmefene was predicted to normalize successive intake of the otherwise differently preferred diets, reducing chow intake in subjects that had not experienced more preferred food options, but increasing intake of less-preferred chow in palatable food-experienced subjects. This novel, second prediction is based on findings that (1) binge-like eating and anticipatory negative contrast are palatability-induced adaptations of eating behavior (Corwin, 2006; Flaherty et al, 1995) and that (2) opioid-receptor antagonists dull palatability-related processes (Cooper, 2004; Yeomans and Gray, 2002).

A third set of descriptive aims was to define the anxiety-related behavior and metabolic outcomes of intermittent, highly limited access to sugary, palatable diet under the current experimental conditions. To determine whether rats receiving highly limited access to preferred food developed increased anxiety-like behavior, subjects were tested in the elevated plus-maze. To determine whether rats with such a diet history were susceptible to becoming obese, changes in feed efficiency, body weight, adiposity, and circulating leptin, ‘active’ ghrelin, and growth hormone (GH) levels were measured.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Adolescent female Wistar rats (n=23 126–150 g, 41–47 days old; Charles River, Raleigh, NC) were single-housed on arrival in wire-topped, plastic cages (19 × 10.5 × 8 inches) in a 12:12 h lit (reverse cycle, 0800 h lights off), humidity- (60%), and temperature-controlled (22°C) vivarium. Rats had access to corn-based rodent chow (Harlan-Teklad LM-485 diet 7012: 65% (kcal) carbohydrate, 13% fat, 21% protein, 3.41 kcal/g; Harlan, Indianapolis, IN) and water ad libitum for 1 week before experiments. Procedures adhered to the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH Publication number 85–23, revised 1996) and the ‘Principles of laboratory animal care’ (http://www.nap.edu/readingroom/bookslabrats) and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of The Scripps Research Institute.

Drug

Nalmefene hydrochloride, or 17-(cyclopropylmethyl)-4,5α-epoxy-6-methylenemorphinan-3, 14-diol hydrochloride (Mallinckrodt, St Louis, MO) was freshly dissolved in isotonic saline. Nalmefene binds potently to κ (Ki=0.083 nM) and μ (Ki=0.24 nM) opioid receptor subtypes, but 2 orders less potently to δ, than to μ or κ, receptors (Ki=16.1 nM). Accordingly, nalmefene has high antagonist potency at κ and μ (IC50=18.5 and 13 nM, respectively), but less so δ, subtypes (Bart et al, 2005; Culpepper-Morgan et al, 1995; Emmerson et al, 1994; Michel et al, 1985).

Diet Preference

To determine relative diet preference, acclimated rats (n=8) were provided concurrent access to chow diet and to a nutritionally complete, chocolate-flavored, high-sucrose (50% kcal), AIN-76A-based diet comparable in macronutrient composition and energy density to the chow diet (chocolate-flavored formula PJPPP: 69.1% (kcal) carbohydrate, 11.8% fat, 19.1% protein, metabolizable energy 3.70 kcal/g; formulated as 45-mg precision food pellets to increase its preferredness, Cooper and Francis, 1979; Research Diets Inc., New Brunswick, NJ) (see Table 1). After food intake and preference were stabilized, food preference was calculated as the % of total 24-h (kcal) intake consumed in the form of the chocolate-flavored, high-sucrose diet, which was found to be strongly preferred by all subjects (see Results) and which hereafter is referred to as ‘preferred’ diet.

Hypothesis 1:

  • Anticipatory negative contrast and binge-like eating develop jointly.

Table 1 Diet Composition and Energy Density

Feeding Procedure

Baseline

A separate group of subjects (n=15) was acclimated to the following daily testing schedule: 15 min before dark cycle onset, animals were transferred to a room, weighed, and individually placed in wire mesh cages (20 × 25 × 36 cm). Each test session then consisted of four contiguous periods in the following order: (a) 1-h chow access, (b) 2-h food deprivation, (c) 10-min access to a chow feeder, and (d) 10-min access to a different chow feeder. Rats were then rapidly returned to the home cage and vivarium with chow available ad libitum. During baseline and testing, water was always available ad libitum. White noise (70 dB) was present in the test room. The 2-h food deprivation sought to (1) make recent intake uniform, (2) increase slightly the motivation to eat, (3) prompt reliable baseline 10-min intake with subsequent satiation, and (4) help signal the imminent availability of preferred food. Intake was weighed with 0.01 g precision. Feed efficiency was calculated as body weight gain (mg) per unit (kcal) energy intake. Over 2 weeks, intake from the 10-min feeders stabilized, with first feeder intake increasing per an associative learning acquisition curve across days (four-parameter logistic regression r=0.97, p<0.01) (Hartz et al, 2001). This baseline period separated the time course of procedural acquisition/acclimation from that of preferred diet schedule-induced feeding adaptations.

Testing

For testing, rats, matched for body weight, daily food intake, feed efficiency, and food intake within each test session period, were assigned to a ‘chow/chow’ control group, which received chow access from both 10-min feeders or a ‘chow/preferred’ group, which also received chow in the first 10-min feeder, but instead received the preferred diet in the second 10-min feeder. Rats were tested daily until day 52, except when otherwise specified.

Hypothesis 2:

  • Rats with intermittent, highly limited access to sugary, preferred diet will show increased anxiety-like behavior.

Elevated plus-maze

To determine whether rats with highly limited access to the preferred diet developed increased anxiety-like behavior, elevated plus-maze testing was performed under dim illumination as described previously (Zorrilla et al, 2002). The primary measures were the percent of total arm time and entries directed toward the open arms, validated indices of anxiety-related behavior (Fernandes and File, 1996) and the number of closed arm entries, an index of locomotor activity (Cruz et al, 1994). The rats described in hypothesis 1 were tested 3–5 h into the dark cycle (24–26 h after prior preferred diet access) on test day 16. The regular feeding session was not performed on this day.

Hypothesis 3:

  • Nalmefene treatment will differentially affect food intake according to diet history.

    To determine the role of opioid receptor activity in palatability-induced feeding adaptations, rats received nalmefene (0, 0.01, 0.03, 0.1, 0.3, 1 mg/kg, subcutaneously (s.c.)) 20 min before the first 10-min feeder. This pretreatment interval was chosen to ensure full antagonist activity throughout the presentation of both feeders. Previous reports indicate that it takes 15–20 min for the maximum effect of subcutaneous nalmefene to be observed in the rat, with comparable functional activity and ex vivo receptor occupancy maintained for at least 1 h (June et al, 1998; Unterwald et al, 1997; Landymore et al, 1992). Treatments were given to the rats described in hypothesis 1 using a full Latin square design with 1 to 3 intervening treatment-free test days from days 24 to 37 following three daily acclimating saline injections.

Hypothesis 4:

  • Rats with intermittent, highly limited access to sugary, preferred diet will become obese.

Circulating leptin, GH, and ‘active’ ghrelin

To determine whether rats with highly limited access to the preferred diet developed endocrine and fat mass changes seen in obesity, rats were overnight fasted (18 h) and decapitated 2–5 h into the dark cycle following 53 days on the diet schedules. Rats were killed after a uniform 18-h fast to reduce potentially confounding acute feeding effects, including diet history-related differences in intake during the experimental feeder period or in the home cage. Spanning the rats’ inactive cycle, the magnitude of the fast was modest on a caloric basis, comparable with those used previously to study these endocrine factors in rats (Proulx et al, 2005; Drazen et al, 2006) and analogous to the clinical procedure of overnight fasting used to measure these hormones in humans (Falorni et al, 1998; Sherwin et al, 1977). Trunk blood (5 ml) was collected in chilled tubes containing 500 μl of 0.5 M ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid and 50 μl of a commercial protease inhibitor cocktail (Sigma catalog P8340). Plasma was isolated by centrifugation (4°C, 3000 g, 15 min) and stored at −80°C until duplicate analysis with immunoassays for rat leptin (LincoPLEX), GH and Ser3-n-octanoylated ghrelin (acyl-ghrelin) enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (Linco, St Charles, MO). Sensitivity limits were 12, 500, and 33 pg/ml, respectively. Typical intra-assay coefficients of variation are <5, <4, and 3.5–5.5%, respectively.

Adiposity

Carcasses were thawed (room temperature) and weighed to determine freezing-related water loss. Gastrointestinal tracts were removed to determine eviscerated weight. Inguinal (subcutaneous) and gonadal (intra-abdominal/visceral) fat pads were dissected, weighed, and returned to the carcass for chemical composition analysis. Total body water, fat mass, and fat-free dry mass (FFDM) were determined using the method of Harris and Martin (1984).

Statistical Analysis

Changes in test session food intake and cumulative feed efficiency were analyzed using two-way analyses of covariance, with baseline as a covariate. Incremental daily food intake and body weight gain were analyzed using two-way repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs). In both models, diet history was a between-subjects factor and day a within-subjects factor. Student's t-test was used to interpret significant group differences and to identify cumulative changes in food intake and body weight gain.

To determine whether the ‘acquisition’ of binge-like feeding (Hagan and Moss, 1997) and of anticipatory negative contrast (first feeder chow hypophagia) resembled an associative learning process, the following sigmoidal four-parameter logistic regression function was fit to feeder intake (Hartz et al, 2001):

The min and max parameters model intake before and asymptotic intake after diet history-induced behavioral adaptation (‘learning’). The Hillslope describes the rate and valence of intake adaptation. The EC50 describes the number of days that passed until 50% of maximal behavioral adaptation occurred.

To determine whether rats stably differed in their individual vulnerability to anticipatory negative contrast or binge eating, two-way, random effect intraclass correlations of absolute agreement (Shrout and Fleiss, 1979) were performed on first and second 10-min feeder intakes (kcal) of days 7 to 15.

Linear regression was used to determine whether rats with limited access to preferred diet showed an altered regularity (r2, the proportion of variance explained) or nature (slope) of the relation between first and second feeder intake. Regression was also used to test the relation between typical ‘binge’ magnitude (mean feeder 2 intake from days 7–15) and anxiety-like behavior, as inversely measured by % open arm entries.

Nalmefene effects on intake were analyzed using two-way repeated measures ANOVAs. Diet history and dose were between- and within-subjects factors, respectively. Linear contrasts determined whether dose effects were log-linearly dependent, and nalmefene's ED50 to reduce second feeder intake (binge-like eating) was estimated using the above sigmoidal dose–response function. Pair-wise dose effects were interpreted using within-subject Dunnett's tests and diet history effects by Student's t-tests. Dunnett's test was used to determine whether nalmefene normalized total intake of chow/preferred-fed rats to vehicle-treated chow/chow-fed levels.

To assess the explanation that hypophagia at the first feeder might reflect a compensatory response to excess weight gain, Pearson correlations were calculated on both cross-sectional and cross-lagged bases for days 7–15. These analyses determined whether an inverse correlation existed between differences in body weight gain on the one hand with concurrent (same day) or subsequent (lagged by 1 or 2 days) differences in first feeder hypophagia (relative to baseline) intake on the other. Correlations were performed on a day-wise basis, averaged following Fisher's Z transformations and backtransformed to obtain an average r. Because of the possible instability of day-wise correlations, analyses also were performed correlating the 3-day moving average of body weight gain with the concurrent 3-day moving average of chow hypophagia.

Adiposity, plasma metabolite/hormone levels, and plus-maze measures were analyzed using unpaired or paired Student's t-test for between- or within-subjects comparisons, respectively. The software packages were Systat 11.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL), SigmaPlot 9.0 (Systat Software, Inc., Point Richmond, CA), InStat 3.0 (GraphPad, San Diego, CA), and Prism 3.0 (GraphPad).

RESULTS

Diet Preference

The mean (±SEM) 24-h intake (kcal) preference ratio for the uniformly preferred chocolate-favored, high-sucrose diet over the chow diet was 92.2±1.1% (range: 88.8–97.9%).

Hypothesis 1:

  • Anticipatory negative contrast and binge-like eating develop jointly.

Sequential 10-min Feeders

As predicted, rats receiving highly limited access to preferred diet (chow/preferred) developed hypophagia of chow from the first 10-min feeder (Figure 1a; diet history: F(1,12)=14.48, p<0.005; diet history × day: F(14,168)= 2.29, p<0.01) and hyperphagia of preferred diet from the second 10-min feeder (Figure 1b; diet history: F(1,12)= 53.96, p<0.001; diet history × day: F(14,168)= 8.98, p<0.001). Feeding adaptations were experience-dependent, as indicated by the diet history × day interactions and, especially by the excellent fits of intake from each feeder to the sigmoidal associative learning function (Figure 1c, Table 2). The two processes had not only different valences, but also different magnitudes and time courses. Acquisition of binge-like intake (feeder 2 intake) exceeded and preceded that of chow hypophagia (feeder 1 intake). Diet history groups differed reliably from one another by day 2 for feeder 2 intake (preferred diet hyperphagia), but not until day 9 for feeder 1 intake (chow hypophagia). The EC50 for binge-like hyperphagia preceded that for ‘anticipatory’ chow hypophagia by 4–5 days (Table 2).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of daily, 10-min access to a feeder containing a highly preferred diet on food intake, body weight gain, and feed efficiency in female Wistar rats. Following 2 h of food deprivation, rats were daily provided 10-min access to a first feeder containing chow followed sequentially by equally brief access to a second feeder containing either chow diet again (chow/chow; n=7) or a highly preferred, chocolate-flavored sugary diet (chow/preferred; n=8). Chow was otherwise freely available. Panels represent (mean±SEM). (a) First feeder food intake in 10 min. (b) Second feeder food intake in 10 min. (c) Sigmoidal regression of first and second feeder food intake in chow/preferred rats. (d) Predeprivation chow intake in 1 h. (e) Home-cage chow intake in 20 h and 40 min. (f) Linear regression of first feeder intake (x-axis) vs second feeder intake (y-axis). Values represent the averaged intakes from test days 7–15. (g) Cumulative food intake. *Differs from chow/chow p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 (Student's t-test).

Table 2 Acquisition of Anticipatory Chow Hypophagia and Binge-Like Eating in Rats as Modeled by Sigmoidal Regression Analysis

Although chow/chow-fed rats were almost fully satiated by first feeder intake of 6 kcal, exhibiting little intake (1 kcal) from the second feeder (Figure 1b), chow/preferred-fed rats escalated second feeder intake to a max value of 34.4 kcal. Thus, 42.9±2.0% of their daily intake (or 45.6±2.7% of chow/chow daily intake) was consumed within only 10 min at a rate of one 45 mg food pellet every 2.9±0.1 s (range: 151–259 pellets/10 min). Conversely, first feeder chow intake decreased to 3.4 kcal (61% of baseline first feeder intake).

Body weight gain and first feeder hypophagia

In chow/preferred rats, greater body weight gain did not concurrently or prospectively predict greater hypophagia at the first feeder from days 7–15, as indicated by a lack of significant day-wise inverse cross-sectional (average r=0.102) or cross-lagged correlations between these measures (average r's=0.022 and −0.040 for 1- and 2-day lags, respectively). Similarly, a rat's average weight gain during this period was unrelated to its average magnitude of first feeder hypophagia (average r=−0.025).

Individual difference and correlation analysis

Unlike the lack of relation of body weight gain to first feeder hypophagia, intraclass correlation analysis showed strong, stable individual differences in intake of chow/preferred-fed rats from both the first (ICC[2,9]=0.86, r2=0.74; F(7,56)= 7.93, p<0.00001) and second feeder (ICC[2,9]=0.89, r2= 0.79; F(7,56)=9.42, p<0.00001). In contrast, chow/chow-fed rats showed no individual differences in first feeder intake (ICC[2,9]=0.37, r2=0.14; F(6,48)=1.61, p=0.17) and smaller individual differences in second feeder intake (ICC[2,9]=0.64, r2=0.41; F(6,48)=2.78, p<0.05). Thus, after mild deprivation, chow/chow-fed rats refed in a manner relatively independent of individual-specific characteristics. In contrast, chow/preferred-fed rats showed trait-like, individual-specific magnitudes of ‘anticipatory’ chow hypophagia and preferred diet hyperphagia following mild energy restriction.

Although intake of individual chow/chow-fed rats was highly and positively correlated across feeders (slope=0.78, reliably greater than 0, F(1,5)=11.67, p<0.02), consistent with sustained refeeding, there was no reliable correlation between the intake of individual chow/preferred-fed rats from the chow to preferred diet feeder (slope indistinguishable from 0, F(1,6)=1.07, p=0.34) (see Figure 1f, difference in r2, z=2.43, p=0.01]). Thus, intakes of individual chow/preferred-fed rats did not exhibit positive or inverse ‘energy homeostatic/compensatory’ short-term correlations across diets/feeders. Those subjects that showed the strongest anticipatory chow hypophagia were not necessarily those that showed the greatest binge-like hyperphagia.

Predeprivation 1-h chow intake

Predeprivation 1-h chow intake of chow/preferred-fed rats was also significantly suppressed starting from test day 11 (Figure 1d; diet history × day interaction F(14,182)=2.35, p<0.01). However, the decrease in predeprivation 1-h chow intake was later in onset (day 11 vs day 9) and significantly smaller both on an absolute (t(7)=−5.59, p<0.001) and proportional basis (t(7)=−3.00, p<0.01) than the average reduction of first feeder intake observed in chow/preferred-fed rats. Importantly, these findings are consistent with an ‘anticipatory’ interpretation of first feeder hypophagia and, like the absence of correlations with body weight gain, inconsistent with the alternative interpretation that first feeder hypophagia resulted from residual energy homeostatic responses to prior binge feeding or excess weight gain.

Home-cage chow intake

Home-cage chow intake of chow/preferred-fed rats progressively decreased in an experience-dependent manner (Figure 1e; diet history: F(1,12)=100.64, p<0.001; diet history × day: F(14,168)=12.06, p<0.001), significantly so by day 3 of testing.

Total daily intake

Despite the substantial changes in intake of particular diets within the day, ANOVA did not reveal significant effects of diet history or diet history × day on incremental total daily energy intake. However, Student's t-tests showed that cumulative energy intake of chow/preferred-fed rats slightly, but significantly, exceeded that of chow/chow-fed rats beginning from test day 9 (Figure 1g).

Hypothesis 2:

  • Rats with intermittent, highly limited access to sugary, preferred diet will show increased anxiety-like behavior.

    Chow/preferred-fed rats showed significantly decreased relative time spent within (Figure 2a, left) and entries into (mean±SEM: 21.5±4.6 vs 34.7±4.7%; t(13)=2.14, p<0.05) the open arms of the elevated plus-maze as compared with chow/chow-fed rats, an anxiogenic-like effect. The number of closed arm entries, a control measure of locomotor activity (Cruz et al, 1994), was unchanged by diet history (Figure 2a, right). A rat's typical ‘binge’ size strongly correlated with its degree of anxiety-like behavior, indicated in a substantial inverse correlation between average feeder 2 intake and % open arm time in chow/preferred-fed rats (Figure 2b). This relationship accounted for most of the variance in these measures (77.4%) and was not significant in regression analysis of chow/chow-fed rats regardless of whether an outlier was included (36.5%) or excluded (9.2%).

    Figure 2
    figure 2

    Effects of daily, 10-min access to a feeder containing a highly preferred diet on elevated plus-maze behavior in female Wistar rats. Following 2 h of food deprivation, rats were daily provided 10-min access to a first feeder containing chow followed sequentially by equally brief access to a second feeder containing either chow diet again (chow/chow; n=7) or a highly preferred, chocolate-flavored sugary diet (chow/preferred; n=8). Chow was otherwise freely available. Panels represent mean±SEM. (a) Elevated plus-maze behavior reflecting (left) anxiety-like behavior, as indicated by the percent of total arm time directed towards the open arms (less % open arm time signifies more anxiogenic-like behavior), and (right) locomotor activity, as indicated by the number of closed arm entries. Rats were tested 3–5 h into the dark cycle (24–26 h after their last preferred diet access) after 16 test days (Student's t-tests). (b) Linear regression of second feeder intake (x-axis) vs % open arm time (y-axis). Values represent the averaged intakes from test days 7–15. **Differs from chow/chow p<0.01.

Hypothesis 3:

  • Nalmefene treatment will differentially affect food intake according to diet history.

    Under vehicle-treated conditions, chow/preferred-fed rats showed ‘anticipatory’ chow hypophagia (feeder 1) and preferred diet hyperphagia (feeder 2) (Figure 3). As predicted, nalmefene treatment differentially affected 10-min chow intake from the first feeder according to diet history (diet history × dose: F(5,65)=3.60, p<0.01; dose: F(5,65)=3.06, p<0.05). Specifically, nalmefene decreased intake of chow/chow-fed rats in a log-linear, dose-dependent fashion (F(1,30)=13.35, p<0.001), with significant pair-wise reductions from vehicle observed at the 0.03 and 1 mg/kg doses. In contrast, nalmefene significantly increased chow intake of chow/preferred-fed rats at the 0.03 mg/kg dose (Figure 3, left). Consequently, low-dose nalmefene pretreatment (s.c., 0.03 mg/kg) blocked ‘anticipatory’ chow hypophagia.

    Figure 3
    figure 3

    Effects of daily, 10-min access to a feeder containing a highly preferred diet on feeding responses to nalmefene, a μ/κ opioid-receptor antagonist in female Wistar rats. Following 2 h of food deprivation, rats were daily provided 10-min access to a first feeder containing chow followed sequentially by equally brief access to a second feeder containing either chow diet again (chow/chow; n=7) or a highly preferred, chocolate-flavored sugary diet (chow/preferred; n=8). Chow was otherwise freely available. Panels represent mean±SEM. Effects of pretreatment (20 min before first feeder) with nalmefene (0, 0.01, 0.03, 0.1, 0.3, 1 mg/kg, s.c.) on (left) first feeder food intake in 10 min, (middle) second feeder food intake in 10 min, and (right) total 20-min food intake (Dunnett's tests). *Differs from chow/chow p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, #differ from chow/chow vehicle condition p<0.05, ##p<0.01, $differs from chow/preferred vehicle condition p<0.05, $$p<0.01.

    Nalmefene also differentially suppressed intake from the second feeder according to diet history (diet history × dose: F(5,65)=6.60, p<0.001; dose: F(5,65)=6.28, p<0.001). Specifically, nalmefene potently (ED50=0.025 mg/kg, r2=0.97) and substantially reduced intake of the preferred diet by chow/preferred-fed rats in log-linear, dose-dependent fashion (F(1,30)=35.37, p<0.0001), without reliably altering chow intake of chow/chow-fed rats (Figure 3, middle).

    Consistent with these findings, nalmefene treatment also differentially reduced total 20-min intake of the two groups, indicated by overall (F(5,65)=5.31, p<0.0001) and log-linear contrast (F(1,13)=44.68, p<0.0001) diet history × dose interaction effects. Nalmefene more efficaciously reduced intake of chow/preferred and chow/chow rats (log-linear: slope: −4.05±0.94 vs −0.69±0.32 kcal/dose increment, respectively). Nalmefene also more potently reduced total 20-min intake in chow/preferred rats (dose: F(5,35)=8.48, p<0.0001), reducing intake significantly at 0.1, 0.3, and 1 mg/kg doses, whereas only the highest dose (1 mg/kg) was effective in chow/chow rats (dose: F(5,30)=2.70, p<0.05). Overall, the highest nalmefene dose (1 mg/kg) reduced total 20-min intake of chow/preferred-fed rats to a level that no longer reliably exceeded that of vehicle-treated chow/chow-fed controls (Figure 3, right). Nalmefene treatment did not exhibit carryover effects on first or second feeder intake of intervening post-treatment days.

Hypothesis 4:

  • Rats with intermittent, highly limited access to sugary, preferred diet will become obese.

Body-weight gain and feed efficiency

Chow/preferred-fed rats gained more body weight than chow/chow-fed rats (diet history: F(1,13)=10.79, p<0.01; diet history × day: F(14,182)=5.96, p<0.001). Significant group differences were evident by day 5 (4 days before differences in cumulative energy intake), increasing through day 15 (Figure 4a). By day 15, chow/preferred-fed rats had gained 14.3 g more than chow/chow-fed rats despite having only ingested 92 kcal more and received only 2.5 h access to the sugary diet. This weight gain in excess of energy intake reflected an increase in cumulative feed efficiency (Figure 4b), which differed significantly per diet history (F(1,12)=10.14, p<0.01) by day 5. By day 24, chow/preferred-fed rats weighed more on an absolute basis than chow/chow-fed rats (Figure 5a), and were 11% heavier by day 53 (despite having only received 9 h total access to the preferred diet).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of daily, 10-min access to a feeder containing a highly preferred diet on cumulative body weight and cumulative feed efficiency in female Wistar rats. Following 2 h of food deprivation, rats were daily provided 10-min access to a first feeder containing chow followed sequentially by equally brief access to a second feeder containing either chow diet again (chow/chow; n=7) or a highly preferred, chocolate-flavored sugary diet (chow/preferred; n=8). Chow was otherwise freely available. Panels represent (mean±SEM). (a) Cumulative body weight gain. (b) Cumulative feed efficiency. *Differs from chow/chow p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 (Student's t-test).

Figure 5
figure 5

Long-term effects of daily, 10-min access to a feeder containing a highly preferred diet in female Wistar rats. Following 2 h of food deprivation, rats were daily provided 10-min access to a first feeder containing chow followed sequentially by equally brief access to a second feeder containing either chow diet again (chow/chow; n=7) or a highly preferred, chocolate-flavored sugary diet (chow/preferred; n=8). Chow was otherwise freely available. Panels show mean±SEM. (a) Body weight. (b) Adiposity. (Left) Absolute (bars) and proportional (embedded numerals) fat mass and FFDM and (right) inguinal (subcutaneous) and gonadal (visceral) fat pad weights. *Differs from chow/chow p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 (Student's t-test).

Adiposity and endocrine status

Excess body weight was largely because of a 57% increase in fat mass (Figure 5b, left). Accordingly, chow/preferred-fed rats were fattier, as defined by a significant increase in % body fat, with no change in the proportion of body weight accounted for by FFDM (Figure 5b, left) and a reduced % water mass (mean±SEM: 71.9±0.8 vs 74.3±0.7%, p<0.05). Fat pad analysis showed significant expansion of subcutaneous (inguinal; 41% increase) and, especially, visceral (gonadal; 76% increase) fat depots (Figure 5, right).

In plasma, chow/preferred-fed rats also showed 60% higher leptin-immunoreactivity, 47% lower GH-immunoreactivity, and 59% lower acylated ghrelin-immunoreactivity, as compared with chow/chow-fed rats (Figure 6a–c). As expected, plasma leptin-immunoreactivity correlated strongly with total fat mass within (rs=0.82 and 0.86 for chow/preferred- and chow/chow-fed groups, ps<0.05) and across groups (r=0.91, p<0.001) (Figure 6d) as well as with gonadal (r=0.85, p<0.001) and inguinal fat pad masses (r=0.78, p<0.01).

Figure 6
figure 6

Long-term effects of daily, 10-min access to a feeder containing a highly preferred diet in female Wistar rats. Following 2 h of food deprivation, rats were daily provided 10-min access to a first feeder containing chow followed sequentially by equally brief access to a second feeder containing either chow diet again (chow/chow; n=7) or a highly preferred, chocolate-flavored sugary diet (chow/preferred; n=8). Chow was otherwise freely available. Panels show mean±SEM. (a) Plasma levels of leptin immunoreactivity and (b) correlation of leptin with total fat mass. (c) Plasma levels of GH-immunoreactivity and (d) Ser3-n-octanoylated ghrelin-immunoreactivity. *Differs from chow/chow p<0.05 (Student's t-test).

DISCUSSION

Female rats with highly restricted access to a highly preferred, high sucrose diet developed experience-dependent, binge-like hyperphagia of the diet and anticipatory hypophagia of a less preferred alternative. The adaptations in feeding behavior were dissociable from one another in time, across individuals, and in their pharmacological dose–response to an opioid-receptor antagonist, suggesting that they represent distinct palatability-motivated processes with shared etiologies. Rats with restricted access to the highly preferred food also spontaneously showed increased anxiety-like behavior and rapidly became viscerally obese.

Binge-like eating developed quickly (EC50=3.2 days) per logistic growth function, consistent with a learned, associative adaptation (Hartz et al, 2001). ‘Binges’ were substantial, accounting for almost half of daily caloric intake and were 7-fold greater than the caloric intake that was able to satiate chow-maintained control rats exposed to the same brief (2 h) restriction period. During the first 8 days of testing (when anticipatory chow hypophagia had not yet developed), ‘binges’ occurred even though rats had just eaten otherwise satiating quantities of the chow from the first feeder. On subsequent days, the degree of anticipatory chow hypophagia never approach the degree of overeating. ‘Binges’ were attenuated dose-dependently by very low doses of nalmefene (relative to those often used in the literature), a preferential μ/κ opioid-receptor antagonist which blunts binge ethanol drinking by alcoholics (Mason et al, 1994, 1999) and which lowers the consumption and subjective ‘pleasantness’ of palatable food in humans (Yeomans et al, 1990; Yeomans and Gray, 2002; Yeomans and Wright, 1991). Rats ate very quickly within binges (2.9 s/45 mg pellet without discounting non-eating time), faster than rats maintained ad libitum on the same preferred diet (unpublished observations). The findings collectively suggest a hedonic component to the binge-like feeding adaptations. Interestingly, the propensity to develop binge-like eating was a highly stable, individual-specific trait, with the subject's identity accounting for 79.7% of the variance in the typical size of acquired ‘binge’ episodes.

Although binge-like eating developed for the restricted, preferred diet, chow/preferred-fed rats decreased their intake of less-preferred chow in their home-cages and also in the predeprivation and postdeprivation (‘first feeder’) portions of the test session. The onset of these hypophagias differed, with chow hypophagia in the home-cage (day 3) preceding those from the first feeder (day 9) and predeprivation (day 11) test periods by about 1 week. Studies were not designed to distinguish the mechanisms responsible for reductions in home-cage or predeprivation chow intake. However, several findings support the interpretation that chow hypophagia at the first feeder was a form of anticipatory negative contrast (Flaherty and Checke, 1982; Flaherty and Rowan, 1986; Flaherty et al, 1995) and not energy homeostatic compensation for accruing weight gain, lasting satiety, or successive negative contrast. First, there was no concurrent or prospective relation between differences in weight gain and the magnitude of chow hypophagia (unlike strong individual differences seen in chow hypophagia). Second, first feeder chow hypophagia began 2 days earlier and was greater in magnitude than hypophagia during the earlier predeprivation hour. An energy homeostatic explanation would predict similar onset and magnitude (if not faster onset and greater magnitude) of anorexia during the earlier predeprivation hour, if compensatory hypophagia was ostensibly sustained through the subsequent first feeder presentation. Third, there was no inverse correlation between the degree of first feeder chow hypophagia and the magnitude of second feeder binges. Fourth, first feeder hypophagia (EC50=7.5 days) developed 4–5 days after binge-like eating. Consistent with the present results, anticipatory negative contrast for an otherwise acceptable sweet solution occurs independent of body weight or caloric intake changes, when the solution is historically succeeded by a more preferred saccharin solution (Flaherty and Rowan, 1986). Still, a possible contribution of energy homeostatic mechanisms in altering the reward threshold for food acceptance in the present study cannot be excluded. For example, levels of leptin and ghrelin, appetite-regulatory homeostatic hormones that directly modulate reward neurocircuitry (Hommel et al, 2006; Abizaid et al, 2006; Hao et al, 2006; Shizgal et al, 2001) differed as a result of diet history and perhaps changed before onset of body weight differences. Longitudinal analysis of leptin and ghrelin levels or pharmacological manipulation of their action could help clarify any contribution of these energy homeostatic regulatory hormones to anticipatory chow hypophagia in the present study.

As with bingeing, individual rats also stably differed in the degree to which they developed anticipatory negative contrast, with identity accounting for 74.3% of the variance in first feeder intake. Importantly, however, this trait was statistically unrelated to and later developing than binge-like eating. Also, whereas nalmefene log-linearly and monophasically reduced binge-like eating with an ED50 of 0.025 mg/kg and near normalization of total food intake at the 1 mg/kg dose, the opioid antagonist blocked anticipatory negative contrast only at a single intermediate dose (0.03 mg/kg).

Anticipatory chow hypophagia from the first feeder developed per a sigmoidal logistic growth function, consistent with a learned, associative process (Hartz et al, 2001). This evidence for an associative, learned adaptation is inconsistent with several potential alternative explanations, including that there was no adaptation across time, that learning was non-associative in nature (eg sensitization or habituation to the diets or test environments) or several non-learned adaptations (eg change in stomach size, non-Pavlovian change in energy homeostatic hormone action). Several cues might have served as conditioned stimuli predictive of imminent preferred food availability, including the experimenter, test environment, deprivation period, or even the preceding first feeder (chow) presentation. Indeed, hyperphagia was significantly reduced (10.9 kcal, or 34%, less, p<0.001) if the first feeder was not presented, consistent with an acquired role for this stimulus in preparing or facilitating preferred food intake (data not shown). Manipulating such conditioned stimuli will be useful for distinguishing between unconditioned and conditioned components of preferred food hyperphagia and their neurochemical substrates.

That nalmefene, a μ/κ opioid-receptor antagonist, differentially reduced binge-like preferred food hyperphagia is consistent with previous reports that the endogenous opioid system subserves the control of hedonic, rather than nutritionally, motivated intake in both humans and animals (Olszewski and Levine, 2007). Several previous findings support the hypothesis that mesolimbic opioid receptors modulate behavioral responses to rewarding stimuli, including hedonically driven consumption of preferred foods (Kelley et al, 2002). Nalmefene may have blunted binge-like eating by blocking opioid receptors in the ventral tegmental area, thereby disinhibiting GABAergic inhibitory interneurons that synapse on dopamine neurons and decreasing dopamine release in the shell of the nucleus accumbens (Taber et al, 1998; MacDonald et al, 2003, 2004). Nalmefene also might have acted by blocking μ-opioid receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell or ventral pallidum (Woolley et al, 2006; Ward et al, 2006), cooperative components of a neurocircuit for amplifying appetitive responses to preferred foods, substances of abuse, and other rewards (Smith and Berridge, 2007; Kelley et al, 2005).

Anticipatory negative contrast has been interpreted alternatively as: devaluation (whereby the hedonic value of the first tastant is decreased as a result of historical or representational comparisons to a more preferred tastant), inhibition (whereby rats learn that a more preferred tastant is imminent and correspondingly inhibit intake of a less preferred, predictive, tastant), or behavioral competition (whereby conditioned anticipatory behavior interferes with ingestion of the first tastant) (Flaherty et al, 1995). Although the present data do not clearly differentiate between these interpretations, they suggest a hedonic, non-energy homeostatic account of anticipatory negative contrast. First, anticipatory chow hypophagia occurred despite the preceding 2 h of food deprivation, after which one might expect animals to accept energy-containing food. This finding is consistent with observations that food deprivation paradoxically increases sensitivity to palatability (Hunt et al, 1988; Kauffman et al, 1995).

Second, a single low dose of nalmefene (0.03 mg/kg) blocked anticipatory chow hypophagia by increasing acceptance of the less preferred chow, whereas nalmefene monophasically decreased chow intake of rats that had never experienced the preferred diet. The differential actions of nalmefene on chow intake according to diet history support the hypothesis that opioids also participate in learned associative, appetitive processes that underlie food acceptance and selection (Barbano and Cador, 2006; Jarosz et al, 2006; Kas et al, 2004). This conclusion differs from prevailing views that opioid-receptor antagonists simply are anorectic per se (especially for palatable food) or modulate putative ‘intrinsic’ hedonic properties of foods (Cooper, 2004; de Zwaan and Mitchell, 1992).

Rats that received highly limited access to the sugary, highly preferred diet spontaneously showed increased anxiety-like behavior 1 day after their most recent access to preferred food. A rat's typical binge size was strongly correlated with its subsequent degree of anxiety-like behavior. Whether anxiogenic-like behavior was due to the long-term diet history or acute withdrawal (Cooper, 2004) from the preferred diet is unclear. Scheduled food access per se and recent hypophagia are unlikely to account for the increased anxiety-like behavior because scheduled food deprivation increased relative open arm exploration in the elevated plus-maze, an anxiolytic-like effect (Inoue et al, 2004). Similarly, obesity is unlikely to account for the increased anxiety-like behavior because Zucker lean and obese rats do not differ in plus-maze behavior (Chaouloff, 1994) and because selectively bred diet-induced obese and diet-induced resistant rats do not spontaneously differ in open field emotionality (Levin et al, 2000). An important question for future study is whether the anxiety-like behavior shown by chow/preferred-fed rats results from receiving the preferred diet per se, as opposed to the highly limited or intermittent nature of access. Overall though, the results show that rats with highly restricted access to a preferred food showed not only binge-like eating, but also correspondingly greater behavioral anxiety, findings consistent with the comorbidity of binge-eating disorders and obesity on the one hand with pathological anxiety on the other (Gluck, 2006; Kessler et al, 1994; Specker et al, 1994).

Rats with limited access to the preferred diet gained body weight and fat disproportionate to the total amount of energy consumed, findings which cannot be accounted for by the total duration of access they received to the preferred diet (9 h). Diets had similar fat, protein, and carbohydrate proportions, so differences in macronutrient composition do not explain the effects. Despite only consuming 8.3% more energy, rats with highly limited access to the preferred diet gained 71.3% more body weight over 15 days. By the end of the study, chow/preferred-fed rats had gained 88% more body weight, owing to preferential accretion of visceral body fat, which increases the risk for cardiovascular disease and metabolic disease (Despres, 1993; Wajchenberg, 2000). The greater feed efficiency of chow/preferred-fed rats may result from the sucrose content of the preferred diet (Kanarek et al, 1987; Kanarek and Orthen-Gambill, 1982) as well as the acquired habit of consuming large energy loads following self-imposed periods of relative hypophagia (Batista et al, 1997). This self-determined ‘meal-fed’-like pattern of sustained dietary restraint interrupted predictably by a single large meal/binge models eating patterns of some dieters and patients with eating disorders and, via greater prandial insulin responses (Calderon et al, 2004; Taylor et al, 1999), may promote lipogenesis.

Rats that received highly limited access to the preferred diet also developed endocrine changes seen in human obesity, including increased circulating leptin (Considine et al, 1996) and decreased circulating Ser3-n-octanoylated ghrelin levels. Leptin, the ob gene product, is a 16-kDa, primarily white adipose tissue-derived circulating hormone (Bates and Myers, 2003; Guha et al, 2003; Pico et al, 2003) that acts as a lipostatic negative feedback signal to regulate energy balance. With increasing fat stores, circulating leptin levels increase to curb appetite and facilitate energy utilization (Bates and Myers, 2003). Accordingly, in the present study, leptin levels increased and correlated strongly with accrued total fat mass (Considine et al, 1996; Maffei et al, 1995). Unlike leptin, ghrelin, a 28-residue, post-translationally acylated, endogenous ligand of the GH secretagogue receptor (GHSR1a), is a predominantly stomach-derived, anabolic hormone whose circulating levels are homeostatically increased by energy insufficiency to signal the central nervous system. Given pharmacologically, the Ser3-n-octanoylated form of ghrelin is orexigenic and decreases energy expenditure and utilization of fat as an energy substrate, leading to weight gain and adiposity with chronic central administration (Druce et al, 2006; Tschop et al, 2000; Wortley et al, 2005). Because leptin and ghrelin respectively increased and decreased in chow/preferred-fed rats, obesity and feeding adaptations probably developed despite energy balance-appropriate, homeostatic accommodations to levels of both hormones (similar to diet-induced human obesity), rather than because of dysregulated release.

Human obesity also has been associated with decreased serum GH concentrations, reflecting reduced half-life, frequency of secretory episodes, and daily production (Scacchi et al, 1999). Chow/preferred-fed rats similarly showed 47% reductions in circulating GH. Of potential pathophysiologic relevance, conditions with decreased GH levels (eg aging, GH deficiency syndrome, and obesity) carry increased risk for cardiovascular disease (Gola et al, 2005; Hoffman, 2005).

In sum, rats with highly limited access to a high-sucrose, highly preferred diet jointly developed binge-like eating and anticipatory negative contrast, a possible determinant of food acceptability, in female Wistar rats. These learned, palatability-motivated feeding adaptations were opioid-dependent and dissociable in time and individual from one another. A single low dose of nalmefene blocked anticipatory negative contrast and potently and progressively reduced ‘bingeing,’ almost normalizing total caloric intake. Rats with highly limited access to a sugary preferred diet showed increased anxiety-like behavior and morphometric and hormonal signs of visceral obesity. The results support the hypothesis that highly restricted access to ‘forbidden’ palatable foods may have an etiologic role in the development of binge eating, food preferences, obesity, and associated disorders.