Abstract
The impairing effects on memory functioning after acute alcohol intoxication in healthy volunteers and after chronic use in alcoholics are well established. However, research determining the next-morning effects of a single episode of binge drinking on memory functioning is scarce. A total of 48 healthy volunteers participated in a single-blind study comprising an evening (baseline) session, followed by a treatment administration (ethanol 1.4 g/kg or placebo), and a morning session. Memory was tested with a word-learning test (including immediate and delayed recall, and recognition). Further, a 45-min Mackworth clock test for measuring vigilance was included (parameters: number of hits and false alarms) and subjective alertness was assessed, to infer whether word-learning test findings reflect sedation or specific memory impairments. Delayed recall in the morning session was significantly worse in the alcohol group when compared to the placebo group (F1,42=6.0, p<0.02). In contrast, immediate recall and recognition were unimpaired in the alcohol group. In the morning session, relative to the placebo group, subjective alertness was significantly reduced in the alcohol group before and after the tests (F1,44=8.7, p<0.005; F1,44=13.3, p<0.001, respectively). However, in the Mackworth clock test, the alcohol group and placebo group did not differ significantly in the morning session. The specific findings of impaired delayed recall show that memory retrieval processes are significantly impaired during alcohol hangover. Vigilance performance was not significantly affected, indicating that this memory impairment does not reflect sedation.
INTRODUCTION
Alcohol hangovers are next-morning adverse effects that occur after an evening of binge drinking, that is, drinking more than four (women) or five (men) drinks on one occasion. When hangover symptoms start to occur, blood alcohol concentration (BAC) is normally (close to) zero. The hangover state continues up to 24 h after awakening (Swift and Davidson, 1998), and is characterized by a variety of symptoms, including sleepiness, drowsiness, concentration problems, lightheadedness, dry mouth, and nausea. The severity of these symptoms varies between and within subjects, for example depending on the type of alcoholic beverage, the presence or absence of congeners (Pawan, 1973), the amount of alcohol consumed, drinking pattern, and drinking history. The actual cause(s) of hangover are yet unknown, but a relation with the toxic effects of one of ethanol's major metabolic products, that is, acetaldehyde (Swift and Davidson, 1998; Wiese et al, 2000), has been suggested, since acetaldehyde produces effects similar to adverse effects observed in the alcohol hangover state. In addition, several other factors have been suggested to contribute to alcohol hangover, including dehydration, electrolyte imbalance, gastrointestinal disturbances, low blood sugar levels (hypoglycemia), and sleep and biological rhythm disturbances (Swift and Davidson, 1998).
Alcohol hangover effects cause serious economic consequences. Hangover-related absenteeism and poor job performance costs the US economy about $148 billion a year (Stockwell, 1998). Workers with an alcohol hangover reported significantly more conflicts with supervisors and co-workers, and felt miserable or fell asleep on the job (Ames et al, 1997). Also, physical performance of healthy volunteers (Karvinen et al, 1962) and athletes (O'Brien and Lyons, 2000) is significantly reduced during hangover. Binge drinking is especially prevalent among college students. Since three out of five college students are regularly involved in binge drinking, it is the most serious public health problem of American colleges (Wechsler et al, 1998,2000).
In general, but especially for college students, proper memory functioning is essential to perform daily activities requiring the ability of learning and remembering. Memory can be distinguished in declarative and procedural memory. Procedural memory refers to learned procedures, expressed in acquired skills. Declarative memory comprises acquisition, retention, and retrieval of events (episodic memory) or facts (semantic memory) that can be measured by direct or indirect tests of memory, such as free recall, cued recall, or recognition (Gabrieli, 1998). Declarative memory can be further divided into short-term memory processes (generally tested within 10 min after learning) and long-term memory processes (generally tested 30 min up to days after learning). For example, in the word-learning test, after learning a list of words (acquisition), the learned words may be transferred to a long-term storage (consolidation). Retrieval of learned words from this long-term storage can be without cues (free delayed recall) or by means of cued recall (recognition).
Memory and psychomotor impairment during alcohol intoxication has been consistently reported (Krüger, 1992; Ferrara et al, 1994; Holloway, 1995; Koelega, 1995; Moskowitz and Fiorentino, 2000). Studies performed with alcoholics showed that frequent binge drinking also produces serious memory deficits in the long run (eg Tapert et al, 2001). Moreover, a recent study performed in rats showed that even after a 4-day period of continuous binge drinking, significant corticolimbic neural cell loss was observed (Obernier et al, 2002). During hangover, these rats performed significantly worse on spatial and reversal memory tests, when compared to predrinking assessments.
Surprisingly, alcohol hangover studies in men failed to find significant impairment on memory functioning (Myrsten et al, 1980; Chait and Perry, 1994; Finnigan et al, 1998). Confusingly, some studies reported significant psychomotor performance impairment (Takala, 1958; Kelly et al, 1970; Seppälä et al, 1976; Bonte and Volck, 1978; Myrsten et al, 1980; Laurell and Törnros, 1983; Yesavage and Von Leirer, 1986; Roehrs et al, 1991; Yesavage et al, 1994; Anderson and Dawson, 1999), whereas other studies did not (Carroll et al, 1964; Ideström and Cadenius, 1968; Dowd et al, 1973; Collins and Chiles, 1980; Collins, 1980; Morrow et al, 1990; Törnros and Laurell, 1991; Lemon et al, 1993; Chait and Perry, 1994; Streufert et al, 1995; Taylor et al, 1996; Finnigan et al, 1998). Taking into account that the majority of participants in these studies did report hangover symptoms, the inconclusive results are rather unexpected.
Methodological shortcomings possibly account for these contradictory results. Some of these studies measured alcohol hangover effects while BAC had not yet reached zero (Takala, 1958; Dowd et al, 1973), did not report the peak BAC after drinking (Carroll et al, 1964; Finnigan et al, 1998; Anderson and Dawson, 1999), or BAC at testing times (Carroll et al, 1964; Anderson and Dawson, 1999). Interpretation of results from studies with unknown BAC levels at testing time or in the case that BAC was not zero is difficult, since it remains unclear as to whether impairment is caused by hangover effects or the ongoing alcohol intoxication. Further, peak BAC after drinking gives an impression of the severity of hangover, and the sensitivity of the subjects to the effects of alcohol. Another problem includes the absence of controlled intake (Carroll et al, 1964; Finnigan et al, 1998), since in such instances one cannot be sure that the beverage was actually consumed.
Other studies did not perform blinded treatment administration (Laurell and Törnros, 1983; Törnros and Laurell, 1991; Chait and Perry, 1994). As a result, participants were aware of the alcoholic content of the beverages they consumed. It is likely that this knowledge has affected their test performance, since it is acceptable to assume that expectations and motivational aspects change human behavior when subjects are informed about treatments or dosages.
Some studies examined hangover effects after drinking during daytime, without sleeping opportunities (Yesavage et al, 1994; Taylor et al, 1996). Results from these studies are difficult to interpret, since normally binge drinking takes place during the evening or night. Finally, in some studies participants received less than 6 h of sleeping time before next-morning tests started (Collins and Chiles, 1980; Collins, 1980). In these studies, the effects of sleep restriction probably interfere with the effects caused by binge drinking itself.
Taking into account the factors mentioned above, the present study examines the effects of alcohol hangover on memory functioning. To investigate if possible effects were caused by alcohol hangover-related sedation, a Mackworth clock test was also performed. This vigilance test measures readiness to react (ie vigilance) during prolonged visual search (Mackworth, 1948), and gives an insight into performance requiring arousal and sustained attention. Under these circumstances, it is hypothesized that alcohol hangover symptoms will significantly impair memory functioning.
METHODS
Subjects
Healthy volunteers were recruited by newspaper advertisement. Subjects were physically and mentally healthy, free from medication, and used no drugs of abuse. Written informed consent was obtained before the start of the study, and subjects were paid for their participation. All participants were treated in accordance with the guidelines for Good Clinical Practice and the Declaration of Helsinki. Subjects were moderate drinkers, consuming between 10 and 35 alcoholic beverages per week, and were experienced with binge drinking (drinking>5 alcoholic drinks on a single occasion). However, subjects were excluded if they had past or present alcohol or drug dependency (DSM IV, 1994). To be included, weight had to be within 20% of the normal range according to gender, height and stature, and a body mass index (BMI) below 28 kg/m2. Also, caffeine consumption (<5 cups/day) and nicotine use (<10 cigarettes/day) were restricted, and if the experimenter or a subject suspected that the abstinence of coffee and cigarettes during the morning session would affect their test performance or behavior they were excluded from participation. Finally, Dutch had to be the native language of the participants.
Four subjects were excluded at screening, and replaced. Subjects were also replaced if they vomited within 1 h after treatment administration (N=6), or when they reported sleeping less than 6 h (N=2). A total of 48 subjects (24 men and 24 women) completed the study. The mean±SEM age was 21.9±2.5 years (men: 21.0±1.7; women: 22.8±3.0) and mean±SEM weight was 68.3±7.9 kg (men: 74.8±9.8; women: 61.8±6.0). The mean±SEM number of alcoholic drinks/day was 3.42±0.3 (placebo group: 3.0±0.3; alcohol group: 3.8±0.3), that is, ap-proximately 24 standard alcoholic drinks per week (in the Netherlands a standardized alcoholic drink contains 12 g of alcohol).
Procedure
On each test session two subjects participated in the study. For the evening session, subjects were transported to the institute, and familiarized with the test procedures. Thereafter, a standardized dinner (macaroni) was served, followed by an evening session of all tests to establish baseline performance. During dinner and beverage consumption, subjects were seated in separate rooms in a relaxing atmosphere. Laboratory tests were performed in a soundproof and dimly illuminated test room. Subjects were then randomly assigned to either the alcohol group or the placebo group. Treatment of the alcohol group consisted of pure ethanol (1.4 g/kg body weight) mixed with orange juice (a total volume of 500 ml), representing the equivalence of 8–9 standard alcoholic drinks. The placebo group received 500 ml orange juice. Beverages were consumed in a gradual fashion, within 30 min, starting at 22.30 h (subject 1) or 23.00 h (subject 2). Subjects used a nose-clip while drinking and the beverages were flavored with Grand Marnier essence, both in order to blind treatment. In addition, up to 1 h after administration subjects of both groups performed breath alcohol tests approximately every 10 min. Subjects remained unaware of their BAC levels. Thereafter, subjects were transported home, and instructed to get a normal night rest (±7 h of sleep). During the night, it was possible to consult the study physician and/or the experimental staff, if needed. The following morning the subjects were transported to the institute. Eating, smoking, and coffee drinking were not allowed. In addition, excessive water consumption was not allowed. BAC measurements were performed every 5–10 min. Tests started as soon as BAC was zero.
Subjective Assessments
Before the start of the morning session, the Sleep Quality Questionnaire (Mulder-Hajonides van der Meulen, 1981) was completed. The questionnaire contains 14 statements regarding sleep quality, and scores ranged from 0 (no sleeping problems) up to 14 (severely disturbed sleep). Also, total hours of sleeping and number of awakenings were recorded. In addition, alcohol-hangover related adverse events were reported, including sleepiness, drowsiness, concentration problems, headache, dizziness, lightheadedness, coordination problems, diplopia, palpitations, tinnitus, dry mouth, thirst, agitation, gastrointestinal disturbances, and nausea. From these data, hangover intensity, a composite score of the number of reported adverse events (moderate intensity=1 point, severe intensity=2 points), was computed. Finally, before and after the test battery, subjects rated their alertness on a 41-point equal interval scale, ranging from ‘maximal alert’ (41) to ‘very sleepy’ (0).
Test Performance
Word-learning test
The word-learning test (Dutch language version) is a computerized and standardized test with high internal consistency and high test–retest reliability. The test consists of 12 parallel lists of 15 monosyllabic meaningful nouns (immediate and delayed recall), complemented with 15 distracter words in the recognition test. During training, evening and morning sessions, different word lists were used. All presented stimuli are frequently used Dutch words, with high imageability (Loon-Vervoorn, 1985), and having low association with each other.
In all, 15 words were presented five times on a computer display. After each presentation, subjects had to write down as many words they could remember. The highest trial score was a measure of immediate recall. Then after 60 min, delayed recall was recorded. Finally, the original list along with 15 distracter words was presented. The subjects had to indicate by button-press whether a presented word was a member of the original list or not. Recognition was expressed in recognition time (ms) and recognition score (number of correct recognized words).
Mackworth clock test
Subjects were instructed to attend a running clock for 45 min, in which 12 dots were subsequently illuminated. Occasionally, nine times during the entire test, a dot was skipped from being illuminated, that is, the pointer made a ‘double-jump’, and subjects had to push a button as fast as possible. A response was considered as missing if the reaction time was longer than 1800 ms. The numbers of hits and false alarms were recorded.
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS statistical program (SPSS 10.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The factors Group (two levels: alcohol group and placebo group), Time (two levels: baseline (evening) and morning session), OldNew (two levels: learned (old) and distracter (new) words) and Gender, and their interactions were tested for significance (p<0.05) by using ANOVA for repeated measures. To examine the effects on learning, a separate analysis of immediate recall data was performed, which also included the factor Trial (five levels: trials 1–5). Additionally, analyses of the alertness scale included the factor Prepost (two levels: before and after the test battery).
RESULTS
BAC
Figure 1 shows that BAC increased rapidly after treatment administration and reached a peak BAC of 0.155% within 60 min after treatment consumption.
BAC reached zero approximately 10.5 h after treatment administration. Although not significant, alcohol elimination was faster in women (±10 h) than in men (±11 h).
Subjective Assessments
Sleep quality
All subjects slept approximately 7 h, without treatment-related awakenings (mean±SEM number of awakenings: 0.8±0.2 in the placebo group and 0.5±0.2 in the alcohol group). Sleep quality scores did not differ significantly between the alcohol and placebo group. Sleep quality scores differed significantly (F1, 47=5.3, p<0.03) between women (score=2.25) and men (score=3.75). However, this difference has no clinical relevance, since both test scores indicate good sleep quality.
Hangover intensity
Relative to the placebo group, hangover intensity was significantly (F1, 44=23.2, p<0.0001) higher in the alcohol group. Also, the main effect of Gender was significant (F1, 44=7.4, p<0.01), reflecting that in the alcohol group the hangover intensity of women was significantly (F1, 22=5.8, p<0.03) higher than that of men. In contrast, this effect was not significant in the placebo group.
Alertness
The overall effect of Group was not significant (F1, 44=2.7, p<0.1). It is evident from Figure 2 that subjects were more alert during the morning session, when compared to evening baseline (Time: F1, 44=5.4, p<0.03). Also, the interaction between Time and Group was significant (F1, 44=24.7, p<0.0001). Relative to baseline, alertness in the morning session was significantly (F1, 22=27.8, p<0.0001) increased in the placebo group. In contrast, in the alcohol group, this effect did not reach significance (F1, 22=3.3, p<0.08). In the morning session, relative to the placebo group, subjective alertness was significantly reduced in the alcohol group before and after the tests (F1,44=8.7, p<0.005; F1,44=13.3, p<0.001, respectively). At baseline, alertness did not differ significantly between the two groups. Overall, women were less alert than men (Gender: F1, 44=5.4, p<0.03).
As expected, alertness decreased during performance of the laboratory test battery (Prepost: F1, 44=59.1, p<0.0001). The interaction between Time and Prepost was also significant (F1, 44=4.7, p<0.04), indicating that the decrease in alertness was of greater magnitude at baseline than in the morning session. The interaction between Prepost, Gender, and Group was also significant (F1, 44=6.6, p<0.02), indicating that the Prepost reduction in alertness is greater in women in the alcohol group, whereas the Prepost difference in alertness in the placebo group is greater among men.
Test Performance
Table 1 presents mean±SEM scores on the tests of the evening session (baseline performance) and the morning session, and the outcome from the statistical analyses, both within and between the alcohol group and the placebo group. All 48 subjects completed the Mackworth clock test. Due to technical problems, the word-learning test data of two subjects was lost and not included in the statistical analyses.
Word-learning test
Results from the word-learning test are shown in Figure 3 and Table 2.
Immediate recall: The effects and their interactions for immediate recall (highest trial score) were not significant.
Learning: Statistical analyses revealed a significant overall learning effect (Trials: F4, 39=170.5, p<0.0001). The overall effects of Group, Gender, and Time were not significant (F<1).
The interactions between the factors Trial, Time, Group, and Gender were not significant, except for the interaction between Trials, Time, and Group (F4, 39=4.0, p<0.01).
Further analysis showed that in Trial 4 of the morning session of the alcohol group immediate recall was significantly (F1, 42=7.9, p<0.01) lower than in trial 4 of the placebo group. However, as is evident from Table 2, the clinical relevance of this finding is questionable, since after completion of the learning sessions (trial 5), as well as all previous sessions during the learning process (first trial score, trial 2 and trial 3), the differences between the conditions are very small and not significant.
Delayed recall: ANOVA revealed a trend towards significance (F1, 42=3.4, p<0.07) for the effect of Group. The effect of Time was significant (F1, 42=27.3, p<0.0001), as well as the interaction between Group and Time (F1, 42=7.0, p<0.01). Further analysis showed that, relative to baseline, in the morning session delayed recall was significantly poorer in both the placebo (F1, 22=5.3, p<0.03) and the alcohol group (F1, 20=21.5, p<0.0001). However, in the morning session, delayed recall of the alcohol group was significantly (F1, 44=6.0, p<0.02) poorer when compared to the placebo group. The other effects and their interactions were not significant.
Recognition score: The overall effects of Group, Time, and Gender were not significant. A significant (F1, 42=10.2, p<0.003) effect of OldNew was found, indicating that distracter words (New, 14.2±0.10 words) were more easily recognized than learned words (Old, 14.6±0.07 words). The interaction between Time and OldNew was also significant (F1, 42=7.8, p<0.008), indicating that the difference in recognized learned and distracter words was greater during the morning session (Δ=0.7 words) when compared to the evening session (Δ=0.1 words). Other interactions were not significant.
Recognition time: The overall effects of Group, Gender, and OldNew were not significant. However, a significant (F1, 42=7.7, p<0.008) overall effect of Time was found. The interaction between Time and OldNew was also significant (F1, 42=10.5, p<0.002), indicating that relative to the distracter words, learned (Old) words were recognized faster in the evening session (−26 ms) but slower in the morning session (+23 ms). The other interactions were not significant.
Mackworth clock test
Number of hits: The effect of Group was not significant. ANOVA revealed a significant (F1, 44=7.7, p<0.01) effect of Time, indicating that performance in the morning session was worse when compared to baseline. The interaction between Time and Group was not significant (F1,44=2.9, p<0.10).
False alarms: The effects of Group and Time were not significant. However, the interaction between Group and Time was significant (F1, 44=5.0, p<0.03). Relative to baseline, in the morning session the number of false alarms was significantly (F1, 22=4.8, p<0.04) increased in the placebo group, whereas the number of false alarms in the alcohol group was nonsignificantly decreased. The number of false alarms in the morning session did not differ significantly between the two groups.
DISCUSSION
Memory Functioning and Vigilance Test Performance
At baseline, the alcohol and the placebo group did not significantly differ on any memory test parameter, indicating that the groups matched well. In the morning session, however, delayed recall differed significantly between the two groups. In contrast, no significant impairment was observed for immediate recall and recognition parameters, indicating that encoding and consolidation processes seem to be intact. The specific finding of impaired delayed recall, however, indicates that retrieval processes were impaired during alcohol hangover. Nevertheless, retrieval was unaffected in the recognition test, which is understandable, since retrieval is much easier in the recognition test (cued recall) than during (free) delayed recall. That is, during delayed recall the subjects are required to explicitly write down the words, whereas in the recognition test they must simply identify which words (from a larger group of words) were presented on the original list. Finally, the present results are in line with those of other alcohol hangover studies that failed to find significant impairment on tests measuring immediate free recall (Chait and Perry, 1994) and probed memory recall (Finnigan et al, 1998).
In contrast to memory functioning, vigilance performance was not significantly affected during alcohol hangover. In line with a previous study that also applied the Mackworth clock test (Lemon et al, 1993), the difference between the two groups was not significant in the morning session.
Subjective Assessments
Figure 2 illustrates that alertness in the placebo condition was significantly increased in the morning session. This finding is consistent with other studies revealing that alertness is maximal after awakening and gradually decreases as the day continues (Monk et al, 1989; Johnson et al, 1992). In contrast, performance measures were generally worse in the morning session, when compared to baseline. Increased alertness possibly weakened the expected effects of alcohol hangover on the Mackworth clock test in the alcohol group, indicated by the nonsignificantly decreased number of hits and nonsignificantly increased number of false alarms in the morning session.
The number of hangover symptoms varied between the participants, but none of the subjects of the alcohol group reported the absence of hangover-related adverse events. Also, women reported a significantly greater number of adverse events than men. Further, in line with previous studies (Sutker et al, 1983; Mishra et al, 1989; Mumenthaler et al, 1999), women eliminated ethanol faster from the blood than men, corresponding to gender differences in bodily fat/water ratio. It was unlikely that this would evoke gender differences on test performance, since the difference in elimination time was not significant and tests started only when BAC was zero.
In contrast to the subjective assessments, no significant gender differences were found on objective performance parameters. Other researchers also reported that hangover intensity and number of adverse events do not correlate well with performance impairment during hangover (Seppälä et al, 1976; Laurell and Törnros, 1983; Chait and Perry, 1994).
Concluding Comments
The results clearly show the specific effects of alcohol hangover on explicit memory, which cannot be explained by increased sedation, since vigilance performance on the Mackworth clock test was not significantly impaired. The impairing effects on memory retrieval processes are in line with those observed after acute alcohol intoxication and those found in alcoholics. Unfortunately, alcohol hangover effects are not always taken seriously. Our results show that there are reasons to change this opinion.
References
Ames GM, Grobe JN, Moore RS (1997). The relationship of drinking and hangovers to workplace problems: an empirical study. J Stud Alcohol 58: 37–47.
Anderson S, Dawson J (1999). Neuropsychological correlates of alcohol hangover. S Afr J Sci 95: 145–147.
Bonte W, Volck J (1978). Untersuchungen zum Problem der Alkoholnachwirkungen. Blutalkohol 15: 35–46.
Carroll JR, Ashe WF, Roberts BE (1964). Influence of the aftereffects of alcohol combined with hypoxia on psychomotor performance. Aerosp Med 35: 990–993.
Chait LD, Perry JL (1994). Acute and residual effects of alcohol and marijuana, alone and in combination, on mood and performance. Psychopharmacology 115: 340–349.
Collins WE (1980). Performance effects of alcohol intoxication and hangover at ground level and simulated altitude. Aviat Space Environ Med 51: 327–335.
Collins WE, Chiles WD (1980). Laboratory performance during acute alcohol intoxication and hangover. Hum Factors 22: 445–462.
Dowd PJ, Wolfe JW, Cramer RL (1973). Aftereffects of alcohol on the perception and control of pitch attitude during centripetal acceleration. Aerosp Med 44: 928–930.
DSM IV (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.
Ferrara SD, Zancaner S, Giorgetti R (1994). Low blood alcohol concentrations and driving impairment. Int J Legal Med 106: 169–177.
Finnigan F, Hammersley R, Cooper T (1998). An examination of next-day hangover effects after a 100 mg/100 ml dose of alcohol in heavy social drinkers. Addiction 93: 1829–1838.
Gabrieli JDE (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annu Rev Psychol 49: 87–115.
Holloway FA (1995). Low-dose alcohol effects on human behavior and performance. Alcohol, Drugs Driving 11: 39–56.
Ideström CM, Cadenius B (1968). Time relations of the effects of alcohol compared to placebo. Psychopharmacologia 13: 189–200.
Johnson MP, Duffy JF, Dijk DJ, Ronda JM, Dyal CM, Czeisler CA (1992). Short-term memory, alertness and performance: a reappraisal of their relationship to body temperature. J Sleep Res 1: 24–29.
Karvinen E, Miettinen M, Ahlman K (1962). Physical performance during hangover. Q J Stud Alcohol 23: 208–215.
Kelly M, Myrsten A, Neri A, Rydberg U (1970). Effects and after-effects of alcohol on physiological and psychological functions in man—a controlled study. Blutalkohol 7: 422–436.
Koelega HS (1995). Alcohol and vigilance performance: a review. Psychopharmacology 118: 233–249.
Krüger HP (1992). Effects of low alcohol dosages. A review of the literature. Alcohol, Drugs Traffic Safety, Proceedings of the 1992 Meeting, Band 2. pp 763–778.
Laurell H, Törnros J (1983). Investigation of alcoholic hangover effects on driving performance. Blutalkohol 20: 489–499.
Lemon J, Chesher G, Fox A, Greeley J, Nabke C (1993). Investigation of the ‘hangover’ effects of an acute dose of alcohol on psychomotor performance. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 17: 665–668.
Loon-Vervoorn WA van (1985). Voorstelbaarheidswaarden van Nederlandse woorden: 4600 substantieven, 1000 verba en 500 adjectieven. Swets & Zeitlinger, ISBN 90-265-0658-9.
Mackworth NH (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Q J Exp Psychol 1: 6–21.
Mishra L, Sharma S, Potter JJ, Mezey E (1989). More rapid elimination of alcohol in women as compared to their male siblings. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 13: 752–754.
Monk TH, Moline ML, Fookson JE, Peetz SM (1989). Circadian determinants of subjective alertness. J Biol Rhythms 4: 393–404.
Morrow D, Leirer V, Yesavage J (1990). The influence of alcohol and aging on radio communication during flight. Aviat Space Environ Med 61: 12–20.
Moskowitz H, Fiorentino D (2000). A review of the scientific literature regarding the effects of alcohol on driving-related behavior at blood alcohol concentrations of 80 mg/dl and lower (Report HS-809-028). US Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration: Washington, DC.
Mulder-Hajonides van der Meulen WREH (1981). Measurement of subjective sleep quality. Proceedings of the International European Sleep Conference. Amsterdam: Netherlands.
Mumenthaler MS, Taylor JL, O'Hara R, Yesavage JA (1999). Gender differences in moderate drinking effects. Alcohol Res Health 23: 55–64.
Myrsten AL, Rydberg U, Ideström CM, Lamble R (1980). Alcohol intoxication and hangover: modification of hangover by chlormethiazole. Psychopharmacology 69: 117–125.
Obernier JA, White AM, Swartzwelder HS, Crews FT (2002). Cognitive deficits and CNS damage after a 4-day binge ethanol exposure in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 72: 1–12.
O'Brien CP, Lyons F (2000). Alcohol and the athlete. Sports Med 29: 295–300.
Pawan GL (1973). Alcoholic drinks and hangover effects. Proc Nutr Soc 32: 15A.
Roehrs T, Yoon J, Roth T (1991). Nocturnal and next-day effects of ethanol and basal level of sleepiness. Hum Psychopharmacol 6: 307–311.
Seppälä T, Leino T, Linnoila M, Huttunen M, Ylikahri R (1976). Effects of hangover on psychomotor skills related to driving: modification by fructose and glucose. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol 38: 209–218.
Stockwell T (1998). Towards guidelines for low-risk drinking: quantifying the short- and long-term costs of hazardous alcohol consumption. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22(Suppl 2): 63S–69S.
Streufert S, Pogash R, Braig D, Gingrich D, Kantner A, Landis R et al (1995). Alcohol hangover and managerial effectiveness. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 19: 1141–1146.
Sutker PB, Tabakoff B, Goist KC, Randall CL (1983). Acute alcohol intoxication, mood states and alcohol metabolism in women and men. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 18(Suppl 1): 349–354.
Swift R, Davidson D (1998). Alcohol hangover. Mechanisms and mediators. Alcohol Health Res World 22: 54–60.
Takala M (1958). Intellectual functions and dexterity during hangover. Q J Stud Alcohol 19: 1–29.
Tapert SF, Brown GG, Cheung EH, Frank LR, Brown SA (2001). fMRI measurement of brain dysfunction in alcohol-dependent young women. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25: 236–245.
Taylor JL, Dolhert N, Friedman L, Mumenthaler M, Yesavage JA (1996). Alcohol elimination and simulator performance of male and female aviators: a preliminary report. Aviat Space Environ Med 67: 407–413.
Törnros J, Laurell H (1991). Acute and hang-over effects of alcohol on simulated driving performance. Blutalkohol 28: 24–30.
Wechsler H, Dowdall GW, Maenner G, Gledhill-Hoyt J, Lee H (1998). Changes in binge drinking and related problems among American college students between 1993 and 1997. J Am Coll Health 9: 57–68.
Wechsler H, Lee JE, Kuo M, Lee H (2000). College binge drinking in the 1990s: a continuing problem. J Am Coll Health 48: 199–210.
Wiese JG, Shlipak MG, Browner WS (2000). The alcohol hangover. Ann Intern Med 132: 897–902.
Yesavage JA, Dolhert N, Taylor JL (1994). Flight simulator performance of younger and older aircraft pilots: effects of age and alcohol. J Am Geriatr Soc 42: 577–582.
Yesavage JA, Von Leirer O (1986). Hangover effects of aircraft pilots 14 h after alcohol ingestion: a preliminary report. Am J Psychiatry 143: 1546–1550.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by internal funds only.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Verster, J., van Duin, D., Volkerts, E. et al. Alcohol Hangover Effects on Memory Functioning and Vigilance Performance after an Evening of Binge Drinking. Neuropsychopharmacol 28, 740–746 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300090
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300090
Keywords
- alcohol
- hangover
- memory
- vigilance
- binge drinking.
This article is cited by
-
A comprehensive review of attention tests: can we assess what we exactly do not understand?
The Egyptian Journal of Neurology, Psychiatry and Neurosurgery (2023)
-
Effects of alcohol hangover on attentional resources during a verbal memory/psychomotor tracking dual attention task
Psychopharmacology (2022)
-
Alcohol intoxication, but not hangover, differentially impairs learning and automatization of complex motor response sequences
Scientific Reports (2021)
-
Behavioral and Biochemical Effects of N-Acetylcysteine in Zebrafish Acutely Exposed to Ethanol
Neurochemical Research (2018)
-
A critical analysis of alcohol hangover research methodology for surveys or studies of effects on cognition
Psychopharmacology (2014)