INTRODUCTION

The sedating and performance-impairing effects of ethanol are well documented (Roehrs and Roth, 2001). Even low to moderate doses of ethanol (0.2–0.6 g/kg) can produce significant performance decrements such as slowed reaction time, decreased attention, and impaired driving (Moskowitz et al, 1985; Roehrs et al, 1989,1992; Roehrs et al, 1994a; Arnedt et al, 2000,2001; Weiler et al, 2000). The Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT; Carskadon et al, 1986) is a commonly used measure for the assessment of physiological sleepiness and it has been used in a number of studies to assess the sedative effects of ethanol. Sleepiness on the MSLT is determined by averaging the latency to electroencephalographically defined sleep on a series of 4–5 standardized nap opportunities equally spaced throughout the day (Carskadon et al, 1986). The increased sensitivity, rigorous standardization, and objectivity of the MSLT provide advantages over subjective measures of sleepiness (Carskadon and Dement, 1987; Drake et al, 2001). Ethanol has been shown to increase objectively measured sleepiness (antonym to alertness) on the MSLT and it does so in a dose-dependent manner (Williams et al, 1983; Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos et al, 1988; Arnedt et al, 2001). The temporal pattern of the sedative effects of ethanol has also been studied (Nicholson et al, 1992). Specifically, during the first 1.5 h postconsumption (on the rising phase of BrECs) ethanol produces physiological alertness (Papineau et al, 1998). However, 2–6 h postconsumption ethanol's sedative effects become apparent with significant reductions in physiological alertness even after BrECs are near zero (Papineau et al, 1998; Roehrs et al, 1992, 1994b). In addition, ethanol-related decrements in alertness and performance can carry over into the following day even after moderate doses and in the absence of hangover effects (Roehrs et al, 1994b). The sedative effects of ethanol are thought to result, in part, from the stimulation of inhibitory GABA receptors as well as the antagonism of glutamate's NMDA receptor subtype (Roehrs and Roth, 2001).

The effects of caffeine on sleepiness and performance have also been widely studied. In contrast to ethanol, low-dose caffeine (75–250 mg) has been shown to have alerting and performance-enhancing effects in normal and sleep-deprived individuals (Johnson et al, 1990a,1990b; Walsh et al, 1990; Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos et al, 1990; Kerr et al, 1991; Rosenthal et al, 1991; Hasenfratz et al, 1993; Penetar et al, 1993; Bonnet and Arand, 1994a,1994b; Bonnet et al, 1995; Wright et al, 1997a,1997b; De Valck and Cluydts, 2001; Kruk et al, 2001; Van Dongen et al, 2001). Specifically, caffeine can improve auditory vigilance (Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos et al, 1990), and has been shown to increase MSLT scores substantially (Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos et al, 1990; Rosenthal et al, 1991). While the specific mechanism(s) of caffeine's effects on alertness and performance are still being investigated, increased plasma catecholamines (Nehlig et al, 1992; Kamimori et al, 2000), adenosine A(2A) receptor blockade (Basheer et al, 2000; El Yacoubi et al, 2000; Van Dongen et al, 2001) and effects on circadian rhythms (Pelissier et al, 1999; Wright et al, 2000; Antle et al, 2001) have all been implicated. Studies have shown that caffeine can facilitate performance and improve subjective and physiological measures of alertness during prolonged sleep deprivation (Penetar et al, 1993; Bonnet and Arand, 1994a,1994b; Bonnet et al, 1995; Horne and Reyner, 1996; Sicard et al, 1996; Reyner and Horne, 1997; Wright et al, 1997a,1997b; Kamimori et al, 2000; De Valck and Cluydts, 2001; Van Dongen et al, 2001). In addition, several studies have assessed the ability of caffeine to reverse the performance and alertness-impairing effects of benzodiazepines (Mattila and Nuotto, 1983; Roache and Griffiths, 1987; Johnson et al, 1990a,1990b).

As both caffeine and ethanol are among the most widely used drugs in modern society, studies have investigated the combined effects of these substances on several physiological and behavioral measures (Franks et al, 1975; Nuotto et al, 1982; Oborne and Rogers, 1983; Kerr et al, 1991; Hasenfratz et al, 1993; Marsden and Leach, 2000). Several studies have found that caffeine can reverse many of the performance-impairing effects of ethanol (Franks et al, 1975; Kerr et al, 1991; Hasenfratz et al, 1993; Azcona et al, 1995; Liguori and Robinson, 2001). Although some studies of the effects of caffeine on alcohol-induced impairment have not found caffeine to reverse performance deficits (Nuotto et al, 1982; Oborne and Rogers, 1983), differences in basal caffeine intake, dosages, sample size and the specific tasks measured are likely to be responsible for the discrepancies. In a recent study of caffeine antagonism of ethanol effects on driving, caffeine attenuated the impairing effects of ethanol on ‘brake latency’, but performance impairment on a choice reaction time task was unaffected. Moreover, while impairment was attenuated, ‘brake latency’ deficits remained in comparison to placebo, indicating that caffeine up to 400 mg was unable to fully counteract driving impairment induced by ethanol (0.6 g/kg) (Liguori and Robinson, 2001). Another study found that a high dose of caffeine (400 mg) reversed the impairment of simple reaction time induced by a moderate dose of ethanol (Azcona et al, 1995). However, in that study the subjective feeling of ‘tiredness’ and ‘drunkenness’ remained despite the performance reversal. It is not known whether daytime sleepiness was present along with these subjective feelings of tiredness as no standardized measures of sleepiness were administered. It could be argued that subjective measures of alertness may be contaminated by feelings of ‘drunkenness’ in such studies. Therefore, the use of objective measures of sleepiness such as the MSLT are necessary to determine if physiological sleepiness induced by ethanol can be reversed with caffeine.

Many of the ethanol effects that are reversed with caffeine such as reductions in psychomotor vigilance, slowed reaction time and memory impairment are also influenced by manipulations of sleepiness (Drake et al, 2001). In contrast, other effects of ethanol such as dizziness/drunkenness (Azcona et al, 1995), body sway/steadiness (Franks et al, 1975; Nuotto et al, 1982; Liguori and Robinson, 2001) and increased risk-taking (Greenwald et al, 1999) that are not reversed by caffeine may be unrelated to sleepiness. This pattern of results along with caffeine's known alerting effects suggests that caffeine may reverse ethanol–induced impairment in performance through its effects on sleepiness. At present, the degree to which caffeine reverses ethanol effects through a reduction in sleepiness is unknown, as previous studies of ethanol-caffeine interactions have not employed standardized measures of physiological sleepiness such as the MSLT. We hypothesized that caffeine would be able to reverse the physiological sleepiness induced by a moderate dose of ethanol and that it will do so in a dose-dependent manner.

METHOD

In all, 13 healthy individuals (seven females, six males) aged 21–35 years (mean 27.5±5.4 years) were studied. Participants were required to be free from medical or psychiatric disease as determined by physical exam and clinical evaluation, body mass index between 19.8 and 27.5, with a total caffeine consumption of <150 mg/day, alcohol consumption of <14 drinks per week, and no history of drug or alcohol abuse. Participants were also required to pass a urine drug screen and a medical evaluation (tests included: liver function, electrolytes, cholesterol, glucose, triglycerides, kidney, urinalysis, hematology, blood chemistries and a pregnancy test) prior to experimental procedures. Individuals using central nervous system acting drugs or scoring outside the normal range (T score >70) on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (clinical scales excluding M-F) or the Cornell Medical Index were excluded. In addition, participants were required to have nocturnal sleep times of 7–8 h per night, sleep latencies of <30 min, consistent bedtimes and rise times (not varying night to night by >2 h over a 1-week period), and no habitual napping. Participants were instructed to maintain their regular bedtimes and rise times for 1 week before and throughout the study period (including between laboratory intervals). All had normal sleep on an 8-h nocturnal polysomnogram (see the screening section below) and an average daily sleep latency on the MSLT of 8–14 min. The hospital's Institutional Review Board approved study procedures. All participants provided written informed consent and were paid for their participation.

Participants reported to the laboratory at 2200 h and were screened within 1 month of beginning the study using an 8-h nocturnal polysomnogram that was scheduled close to self-reported bedtimes and waketimes (2300–0700). Recordings included electroencephalograms (C3, C4, and OZ referenced to mastoid), two electro-oculograms (EOG; bilateral horizontal), submental electromyogram (EMG) and electrocardiogram (V5 lead), and were scored in 30-s epochs according to standard procedures (Rechtschaffen and Kales, 1968). In addition, leg movements were monitored with a tibialis EMG and respiration with a nasal/oral thermistor. Recordings were made using Grass model 78-D or Nihon Kohden (models 4312 and 4212) polygraphs. All electrode impedances were <10 000 Ω and the paper speed was 10 mm/s. There was no evidence of clinically significant apnea/hypopnea (apnea hypopnea index >10) or periodic leg movements during sleep (>10 h−1) for any of the participants.

At screening, participants were tested using a five-trial (0930, 1130, 1330, 1530, and 1730 h) MSLT that was administered and scored according to standard criteria (Carskadon et al, 1986). In accordance with standard procedures, sleep latency was scored as the time from the start of the MSLT to the first epoch of any stage of sleep. Each sleep latency test was discontinued following three consecutive epochs of stage 1 sleep or one epoch of any other stage of sleep. Each participant was required to have a sleep efficiency >85% at screening and an average daily MSLT sleep latency of 8–14 min. The MSLT range was used to exclude individuals in the upper and lower quartiles of MSLT latency in order to maintain a more representative study sample with respect to physiological sleepiness (Drake et al, 2002). All participants were practiced on each of the performance tasks twice at screening in order to minimize the possibility of practice effects. A total of 12 individuals were excluded from participation due to polysomnographic exclusionary criteria.

Protocol

Participants arrived 1 h prior to their scheduled bedtime (2300 h) for the attachment of electrodes and preparation for polysomnographic recording. Bedtime was set at 2300 h and wake time was set at 0700 h. Recordings proceeded according to standardized screening procedures as above. On each study day after awakening, participants were given a breakfast consisting of a roll and orange juice to standardize stomach content and minimize differences in ethanol absorption rate. Participants underwent each of four conditions presented in a Latin Square Design: placebo–placebo, ethanol (0.5 g/kg)–placebo, ethanol (0.5 g/kg)–caffeine 150 mg, and ethanol (0.5 g/kg)–caffeine 300 mg. The study was performed in a double-blinded manner. Figure 1 provides a graphical representation of the study timeline during each experimental day. On each of four mornings following an 8-h time-in-bed, placebo or ethanol was administered at 0900–0930 h and at 0900 h a caffeine or placebo capsule was given (identical capsules prepared by the hospital pharmacy). The ethanol used was 80-proof vodka, mixed at a 1:4 ratio with carbonated tonic water, and served with 1 oz of either lemon or lime juice per participant preference. Placebo and ethanol beverage volumes were identical. The placebo beverage was masked with 4–5 drops of ethanol. The ethanol dose was equivalent to three 12-oz US beers for a 150-lb person. Generally, participants took the entire 30 min to consume the study beverage. During the study, participants were instructed to avoid all caffeinated or alcoholic beverages other than the study beverage provided for them in the laboratory as described above. BrECs were measured at 0945, 1145, 1345, and 1545 h using an Alco-Sensor III (Intoxometers, Inc., Richmond) calibrated weekly. MSLT trials were performed at 1000, 1200, 1400, and 1600 during each study day. Each MSLT was performed according to standardized criteria (see the screening section).

Figure 1
figure 1

Timeline of experimental manipulations and daytime testing during each condition.

Performance Assessments

During the probed-recall memory (PRM) task, participants were exposed to a written list of four word-pairs for 30 s. After 15 min participants were provided with one of the words in each pair and asked to recall the paired words within a 30s time limit. Performance was measured as number correct per trial. A previous study has found this task to be sensitive to the effects of sleep deprivation (Dinges et al, 1997).

Participants completed a divided attention task at 1030 and 1430 h on each testing day. This 15-min task required participants to track a moving target across a video screen using a joystick while simultaneously responding with a button press (non dominant index finger) to the appearance of stimuli in the center of the target or periphery of the screen. A total of 52 stimuli were presented at random intervals throughout each task period. Dependent measures for this task were reaction time (sec) to central and peripheral stimuli and tracking deviations measured in pixels.

Subjective Measures

The Profile of Mood States (POMS) was administered at 1030 and 1430 h each day in order to assess subjective sleepiness, and mood effects. The POMS is a validated measure that has been used in numerous studies evaluating the effects of sleep loss and pharmacological challenges on mood and performance (Bishop et al, 1997; Dinges et al, 1997; Wright et al, 1997a,1997b; Roehrs et al, 1999). Mean T-scores for each standard POMS scale (fatigue/inertia, vigor/activity, confusion/bewilderment, tension/anxiety, anger/hostility, depression/dejection) were calculated for each study day. A visual analog scale was also used to assess the subjective effects of ethanol and caffeine (ie, dizziness, energy, stimulated, high, jittery). Finally, the Stanford Sleepiness Scale was used as an additional measure of subjective sleepiness (Hoddes et al, 1972).

Statistical Analyses

Repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) using the General Linear Model function of Systat, version 9.0 for PC (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, III) was used to test differences between conditions. Planned comparisons using contrasts for repeated measures designs were performed where significant omnibus-F values were found. The examination of contrasts was made contingent upon a significant omnibus-F value in order to decrease the probability of Type I error given the multiple measures assessed. Specifically, three comparisons were performed for measures with a significant omnibus F: (1) a comparison between placebo and ethanol to determine the presence of ethanol effects at the dosage used, (2) a comparison of placebo to ethanol+caffeine (150-mg) and (3) a comparison of placebo to ethanol+caffeine (300-mg). Post hoc trend analyses were performed on measures where significant ethanol effects were detected in order to examine the consistency of patterns of change across measures with increased caffeine doses. Log-base 10 or reciprocal transformations were performed on data when frequency distributions differed from normality (MSLT and tracking deviations). Raw data are presented in the table and figures. Morning and afternoon data were analyzed separately for performance, memory and subjective measures. ‘Reversal of ethanol effects,’ was operationally defined as a comparison where ethanol effects that were previously significantly different from placebo were not significantly different from placebo with the addition of a given caffeine dose.

RESULTS

MSLT Alertness and BrEC

Mean BrEC±SD was 0.043±0.0197% at 0945 h, declined to 0.024±0.0135% at 1145 h, and reached 0.0 by 1345 h. BrEC did not differ in the three caffeine treatments. Means and SDs for each dependent measure across the four conditions are presented in Table 1 (objective measures) and Figure 2. As expected, ethanol alone reduced mean latency on the MSLT when compared to the placebo condition (p<0.05). There was no significant difference between the placebo and ethanol+caffeine (150 mg) conditions, indicating that the lowest caffeine dose reversed the effect of ethanol on the MSLT. The combination of ethanol and the highest dose (300 mg) increased mean latency (reduced sleepiness) significantly beyond placebo levels (p<0.05) (Table 1 and Figure 2). Caffeine also reversed the effects of ethanol on the MSLT in a dose-related manner as evidenced by a significant primary linear polynomial trend (p<0.05). Thus, each dose of caffeine incrementally and linearly improved physiological alertness above levels induced by ethanol.

Table 1
Figure 2
figure 2

Means±SE for placebo (PLAC), ethanol (ETOH) and ethanol+caffeine (ETOH Caf 150; ETOH Caf 300) conditions. Daily mean is presented for the MSLT, and am data are presented for central reaction time (CRT), PRM (Memory), tracking deviations (Tracking), Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS), and subjective ratings of dizziness (Dizzy); asterisks indicate p<0.05 compared to placebo condition (repeated measures contrast, pairwise comparison).

Performance Measures

Means and SDs for each performance measure across the four conditions are presented in Table 1. Ethanol alone slowed down peripheral and central reaction time and impaired tracking on the divided attention task during the morning session when compared to the placebo condition (p<0.05 for all). No significant difference in reaction times (PRT or CRT) was found for the comparisons between the placebo and ethanol+caffeine (150 mg), or between placebo and ethanol+caffeine (300 mg), indicating that both caffeine doses eliminated the detrimental effects of ethanol on reaction time (Table 1 and Figure 2). Similar to the MSLT results, caffeine reversed the effects of ethanol on reaction time in a dose-related manner as evidenced by significant primary linear trends on both peripheral and central reaction time measures (p<0.05 for both).

Memory

Means and SDs for the PRM task across the four conditions are presented in Table 1. Accuracy on the memory task was impaired by ethanol alone during the morning testing session when compared to the placebo condition (p<0.05). The comparison between placebo and ethanol+caffeine (150 mg) was also significant (p<0.05), indicating that the lowest caffeine dose was unable to reverse the detrimental effect of ethanol on the recall memory task. No significant difference was found for the comparison between placebo and ethanol+caffeine (300 mg), indicating that the detrimental effect of ethanol on the recall task was eliminated by the highest caffeine dose (Figure 2). In addition, caffeine reduced the effects of ethanol in a dose-related manner as indicated by a significant primary linear trend across each of the three ethanol conditions for the morning trial of the PRM task (p<0.05). No effect of ethanol on memory was found during the 1430 h trial.

Subjective Measures

Significant omnibus-F values were obtained for five of the morning rating scales: Fatigue, F(3,36)=4.80, p<0.05; Dizzy, F(3,36)=3.87, p<0.05; Energy, F(3,36)=2.94, p<0.05; High, F(3,36)=3.15, p<0.05; and the Stanford Sleepiness Scale, F(3,36)=4.01, p<0.05. Comparisons revealed that ratings for each scale were significantly impaired by ethanol alone when compared to the placebo condition (p<0.05). The comparisons between placebo and ethanol+caffeine (150 or 300 mg) were not significant (p>0.05), indicating that both caffeine doses reversed the effects of ethanol on ratings of fatigue. Impairments on ratings of energy and subjective sleepiness on the Stanford Sleepiness Scale were also reversed by each caffeine dose (p>0.05). Caffeine reversed ethanol's effects on subjective sleepiness in a dose-related manner as evidenced by a significant linear (p=0.05) trend across each of the three ethanol conditions. Ratings of fatigue also showed a significant primary linear trend (p<0.05). Although visual analog ratings of dizziness were increased by ethanol (p<0.05), they were not diminished by either caffeine dose. None of the omnibus-F values were significant for the afternoon ratings (p>0.05 for all).

DISCUSSION

This is the first study to show that caffeine can reverse the sedative effects of moderate ethanol consumption and does so in a dose-related manner. Caffeine also reversed the effects of ethanol on recall memory. Similar to previous studies, caffeine reversed the performance-impairing effects of ethanol (Kerr et al, 1991; Hasenfratz et al, 1993; Azcona et al, 1995). The present study adds to the literature by demonstrating that the antagonistic effects of caffeine on ethanol-induced performance impairment extend to a standardized measure of daytime alertness and that the attenuations of performance deficits are present even with small doses of caffeine. The performance and memory measures assessed in the present study were selected based on their sensitivity to variations in physiological alertness (Drake et al, 2001). Caffeine's reversal of ethanol's effects on these measures provide evidence for the hypothesis that caffeine reverses ethanol-induced impairment through a reduction in sleepiness. The consistent linear dose-related effects of caffeine on alertness and performance measures following ethanol administration provide additional support for this possibility. These findings in conjunction with previous research that has demonstrated a reversal of ethanol-induced performance impairment with a short nap (Roehrs et al, 1993) suggest that the performance impairment associated with ethanol may be related to increased physiological sleepiness.

In contrast to the reversal of physiological sleepiness, reduced psychomotor performance, and memory impairment, some of the subjective effects of ethanol remained despite caffeine administration up to 300 mg. Although the operational definition of ‘drunkenness’ is not uniform throughout the literature, we considered the visual analog scales ‘dizziness’ and ‘high’ to be measures of drunkenness/intoxication. The low dose of caffeine reversed the ‘high’ associated with ethanol, whereas neither dose of caffeine impacted the subjective feeling of ‘dizziness’. Thus, caffeine similar to other stimulants (Stein et al, 1996; Bishop et al, 1997) appears to reverse some aspects of ‘drunkenness’, but other negative effects remain. These results are consistent with previous research on the interaction of similar caffeine and ethanol doses, where it has been demonstrated that caffeine can reduce some specific performance impairments induced by moderate ethanol consumption without reducing particular subjective effects (Liguori and Robinson, 2001).

While the specific mechanisms by which caffeine attenuates the performance and alertness-impairing effects of ethanol are unknown, pharmacodynamic rather than pharmacokenetic interactions have been proposed (Dar, 1988). Although caffeine is a benzodiazepine receptor antagonist, this property is only present at toxic levels of plasma caffeine (File et al, 1982) and cannot explain the effects observed in the present study. Recent research suggests that antagonism of adenosine A(2a) receptors (Pedata et al, 1984; Varani et al, 2000; Scammell et al, 2001) or an increase in sympathetic activation/catecholamines (Kamimori et al, 2000) may be involved.

It must be emphasized that, while the performance and sedative effects of moderate alcohol consumption were reversed with low-dose caffeine, other measures such as driving ability were not studied and thus generalization to such situations is unwarranted. Also, as in previous studies, feelings of ‘drunkenness’ remained after caffeine ingestion, indicating that specific negative effects of ethanol continued to be present and would clearly have the potential to adversely affect judgment and thus driving ability. Also, while ethanol adversely affected most measures, the dose used in the present study was in the low-to-moderate range and produced BrECs below the legal limit in most states. Additional studies using a range of ethanol doses are needed to determine the limits of caffeine's ability to reverse the sedative effects of ethanol. Finally, the modest effect of ethanol on MSLT latency (decrease of 2 min) observed in the present study suggests that our results pertain to the lower end of this continuum (ie, low-to-moderate ethanol impairment).

Similar to previous studies (Azcona et al, 1995; Kerr et al, 1991; Hasenfratz et al, 1993), the present results demonstrate caffeine antagonism of ethanol-induced impairment. Other studies have found only partial antagonism of ethanol's effects on performance (Burns and Moskowitz, 1989/90; Rush et al, 1993; Forney and Hughes, 1965; Franks et al, 1975; Nuotto et al, 1982; Oborne and Rogers, 1983; Liguori and Robinson, 2001). This discrepancy between studies may be related to the doses of each substance used, differences in prior habitual caffeine and/or ethanol intake of participants and the sensitivity of tests of impairment. One limitation of the present study is that only one dose of ethanol was used, and in previous studies where ethanol-induced impairment remained following caffeine intake, relatively higher doses of ethanol were administered. One significant strength of the present study is that habitual caffeine use history was carefully assessed and individuals who reported >200 mg/day were excluded from participation, thus reducing the variability in caffeine response between participants.

In the present study a moderate dose (0.5 g/kg) of ethanol produced impairments in alertness, memory, and performance similar to those seen in previous studies. However, following low (150 mg), and moderate (300 mg) doses of caffeine, many of these impairments were reversed, and in some cases measures of alertness improved (ie, MSLT). Despite this antagonistic effect of caffeine, a measure of the subjective effect of ethanol, ‘dizziness’, was unaffected by either caffeine dose. Thus, while caffeine is effective at reversing several of the specific effects of ethanol, other negative effects that may be unrelated to sleepiness remain. These results suggest that many of the performance impairments induced by moderate doses of alcohol may be due to increased physiological sleepiness.