INTRODUCTION

Research with neurological patients and brain-imaging technologies has demonstrated that the capacity to make effective real-life decisions, involving choices between actions leading to uncertain outcomes, depends upon the integrity of the orbitofrontal cortex and its interconnected neural circuitry (Bechara et al, 1996; Damasio, 1994; Rogers et al, 1999b,1999c). Recent extension of this work has suggested that deficits in the decision-making of psychiatric patients may involve dysfunction in the same neural machinery (Bechara et al, 2001; Rahman et al, 2001; Rogers et al, 1999b). Despite these advances, relatively little is known about how altered activity of the ascending reticular arousal systems influences decision-making, even though such information would help build a more complete picture of the principal neurochemical determinants of human choice, and improve our understanding of the cognitive effects of monoaminergic therapies. In a previous study, we found that rapid dietary tryptophan depletion—leading to reduced central serotonin function (Nishizawa et al, 1997)—impaired the decision-making of healthy volunteers (Rogers et al, 1999b). However, the complexity of the decision-making task used in that study precluded examination of the mechanisms that might have produced this result. In the present study, we used a novel decision-making procedure to reveal the impact of tryptophan depletion on separable factors known to determine human choice: specifically, the magnitude of expected gains (or reward), the magnitude of expected losses (or punishment), and the probabilities with which these outcomes are delivered (Goldstein and Hogarth, 1997).

There are several mechanisms by which altered serotonin function might modulate decision-making. According to theoretical perspectives that emphasise the role of serotonin in mediating anxiety (Deakin and Graeff 1991; Iversen, 1984; Gray, 1987) and the effective processing of aversive signals (Tye et al, 1977; Wilkinson et al, 1995), we might expect lowered serotonin to impair decision-making by inducing a failure to process expected losses adequately; that is, punishment cues might fail to generate anticipatory anxiety states associated with risky or maladaptive choices. Complementing this approach, the involvement of serotonin in behavioural suppression and control of ‘punished’ or ‘nonrewarded’ responding (Harrison et al, 1997, 1999; Soubrie, 1986) suggests that lowering serotonin may alter volunteers’ decision-making by undermining their capacity to inhibit activated responses sufficiently in order to integrate aversive signals relevant to the imminent choice. For these reasons, we might predict that rapid tryptophan depletion will alter decision-making in healthy volunteers through altering their processing of expected losses.

By contrast, another perspective highlights the role of serotonin in modulating reinforcement and incentive-motivational processes, possibly through complex interactions with the mesolimbic dopamine system (Baumgarten and Grozdanovic, 1995) and the regulatory functions of several serotonin receptor subtypes (eg Parsons et al, 1996; Walsh and Cunningham, 1997). Evidence of a role for serotonin in reward processing includes recent demonstrations that the behavioural and reinforcing effects of cocaine in rats are potentiated by treatment with selective serotonin-reuptake blockers (SSRIs) (Cunningham and Callahan, 1991; Kleven and Koek, 1998; Sasaki-Adams and Kelley, 2001), and that brain self-stimulation thresholds can be dose-dependently altered with SSRI treatment (Harrison and Markou, 2001a, 2001b). Also relevant are serotonin's acknowledged involvement in clinical depression (eg Schildkraut, 1965) and the more general observation that SSRI treatment is relatively effective in the treatment of clinical depressive illness, while the temporary reduction of central serotonin function in vulnerable individuals through rapid dietary tryptophan depletion has been shown to reinstate depressive symptoms and flat affect (Moore et al, 2000; Smith et al, 1997). Finally, depleted serotonin function within limbic structures associated with reward processing, such as the nucleus accumbens, has also been implicated as an important mediating factor in the affective/motivational aspects of withdrawal phenomena in drug-dependent individuals (Markou et al, 2001; Weiss et al, 1996).

In studies with healthy volunteers, serotonin also appears to play a role in affective learning and, in particular, in the effective acquisition of stimulus–reward linkages as evidenced by the finding that tryptophan depletion produces deficits in simultaneous visual discrimination and reversal learning studies (Park et al, 1994; Rogers et al, 1999a). Since reversal learning is believed to involve the orbitofrontal cortex (Dias et al, 1996; Jones and Mishkin, 1972; Thorpe et al, 1983)—as does decision-making (Bechara et al, 1996; Rogers et al, 1999b, 1999c)—serotonin may undermine decision-making through altering the representation of reward within the orbitofrontal cortex. Accordingly, we might predict that tryptophan depletion alters decision-making through a change in the processing of reward cues.

One way to study decision-making is to present volunteers with two ‘gambles’, each consisting of a given probability of winning a certain outcome or losing a certain outcome; for example, a choice between a 25% chance to win $100 and a 75% chance of losing $20 vs a 25% chance to win $240 and a 75% chance to lose $60. Variation in volunteers’ preferences, as determined by differences between magnitudes of gains and losses in each gamble, as well as their respective probabilities, has been researched extensively to examine the degree to which human choice conforms to, or departs from, what would be normatively rational in terms of choices maximising some positive outcome, or ‘expected value’, over the longer term (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tversky and Kahneman, 1992). The ‘expected value’ of a gamble is the sum of its component values (gains and losses), each weighted by its probability of occurrence. The expected value of the above two gambles is (0.25×100)+(0.75×−20)=10 and (0.25×240)+(0.75×−60)=15. Thus, the normatively rational choice, in terms of maximising value over the longer term according to standard expected value theory (see Goldstein and Hogarth, 1997) would be the latter of these two gamble.

Two well-known examples of violations of normative decision-making include risk-averse choices when choosing between gain, and risk-seeking choices when choosing between losses. For example, most volunteers will choose a 100% chance of a gain of $40 over a 50% chance of an $80 and a 50% chance of nothing (indicating risk-aversion for gains), but choose a 50% chance of an $80 loss and a 50% chance of no loss over a 100% chance of a loss of $40 (indicating risk-seeking for losses) (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). Notice that the ‘expected value’ of each gamble in both of these pairs is equal, predicting that volunteers should be indifferent to the options (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). In the present study, we have adapted these techniques in a procedure that allows us to assess the impact of reduced tryptophan on separable factors known to be involved in human decision-making: the magnitude of expected gains (or reward), the magnitude of expected losses (or punishment), and the probabilities with which each of these outcomes will be delivered. We also incorporated separate conditions to test the effects of tryptophan depletion on choices between gambles driven by risk-aversive (gains) and -seeking (losses) biases.

METHODS

The study was approved by the Oxfordshire Psychiatric Research Ethics Committee (OPREC). All volunteers provided written informed consent.

Subjects

In all, 36 healthy volunteers participated. Each volunteer was examined by a psychiatrist (ZB) to ensure that neither of the following exclusion criteria was met: (i) major physical illness; (ii) a history of DSM-IV depressive illness as assessed by an SCID-I interview. (Note: a family history of depression was not an exclusion criterion.) Verbal intelligence was assessed with the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, 1982).

Design

The study was a double-blind, placebo-controlled design. A total of 18 volunteers were administered an amino-acid drink without tryptophan (the T− group) and 18 volunteers were administered an amino-acid drink with tryptophan (the T+ group).

Procedure

All volunteers followed a low-protein diet (less than 20 g total) the day before the study and then fasted overnight. Volunteers attended the laboratory at 8.30 am on the day of the study. Blood samples (15 ml) were taken at this time to obtain baseline levels of total plasma tryptophan. Volunteers then drank an amino-acid drink over a 60-min period. The composition of the drinks was (for males and females, respectively): L-alanine (5.5 g, 4.58 g); L-arginine (4.9 g, 4.08 g); L-cysteine (2.7 g, 2.25 g); glycine (3.2 g, 2.25 g); L-histidine (3.2 g, 2.67 g); L-isoleucine (8.0 g, 6.67 g); L-leucine (3.5 g, 11.25 g); L-lysine monohydrochloride (11.0 g, 9.17 g); L-methionine (3.0 g, 2.5 g); L-phenylalanine (5.7 g, 4.75 g); L-proline (12.2 g, 10.17 g); L-serine (6.9 g, 5.75 g); L-threonine (6.5 g, 5.42 g); L-tyrosine (6.9 g, 5.75 g); L-valine (8.9 g, 7.42 g). All amino acids were supplied by SHS International. None of the volunteers reported any side effects beyond transitory nausea. Volunteers were also given a low-protein (less than 2 g total) lunch at midday. A second blood sample was taken 5 h after consumption of the amino-acid drink (+5 h) in order to assess reductions in total plasma tryptophan. All volunteers then completed the computerised decision-making task.

Additionally, 16 T+ and 14 T− volunteers completed psychometric assessments of state positive and negative affects (PANAS; Watson et al, 1988a) at baseline and at +5 h after consumption of the amino-acid drinks. This information was used to assess the relationship between the effects of tryptophan depletion on decision-making and any effects on the mood of the volunteers (see Moore et al, 2000).

Decision-Making Task

Each trial required volunteers to choose to play one of the two simultaneously presented gambles. Each gamble was represented visually by a histogram, the height of which indicated the relative probability of gaining a given number of points. The expected gains were indicated in green ink above the histogram, while the expected losses were indicated in red ink underneath the histogram. On each trial, one gamble (coloured yellow) was always the control gamble, consisting of a 50% probability of winning 10 points and a 50% probability of losing 10 points (see Figure 1a). The alternative ‘experimental’ gamble (coloured blue) varied in the probability of winning which was either high or low (75 vs 25%), the expected gains which were either large or small (80 vs 20 points), and the expected losses which were either large or small (80 vs 20 points). The combination of these variables, in a completely crossed design, resulted in eight trial types (see Table 1). Figure 1a shows an ‘experimental’ gamble with a 25% chance of winning 80 points (and a 75% chance of losing 20 points).

Figure 1
figure 1

(a) One trial from the decision-making task consisting of an ‘experimental’ gamble with a 25% chance of winning 80 points and a 75% chance of losing 20 points vs the control gamble with a 50% chance of winning 10 points and a 50% of losing 10 points. (b) A ‘gains only’ trial from the decision-making task consisting of a certain win of 40 points vs 50% chance of winning 80 or 0 points. (c) A ‘losses only’ trial consisting of a certain loss of 40 points vs a 50% chance of a loss of 80 or 0 points.

Table 1 Eight Types of ‘Experimental’ Gamble Resulting from the Combination, in a Completely Crossed Design, of Two Levels of Probability, Expected Gains, and Expected Losses

The control gamble and the ‘experimental’ gamble appeared randomly on the left or right of the display. The volunteer was required to press the ‘1’ or the ‘2’ key on a standard computer keyboard to indicate choice of the gamble presented on the left or the right. The dependent measure was the proportion of choices of the ‘experimental’ over the control gamble as a function of the combination of probability, the size of expected gains, and the size of the expected losses in the ‘experimental’ gamble. We also measured volunteers’ discrimination between differences in probability, gains, and losses by calculating the absolute difference between the proportion of choices of the ‘experimental’ gamble over the control gamble when each of these factors was high (eg when the expected losses were large) and the proportion of choices of the ‘experimental’ gamble when that factor was low (eg when the expected losses were small).

In addition, we included two extra trial types that represented choices between gambles known to be subject to the non-normative biases of risk-aversion and risk-seeking behaviour described earlier (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). The first type was a ‘gains only’ trial in which volunteers were presented simultaneously with a guaranteed win of 40 points or a 50% chance of winning 80 points. Neither option involved any associated losses (see Figure 1b). By contrast, in the ‘losses only’ trial type, volunteers were presented simultaneously with a guaranteed loss of 40 points or a 50% chance of losing 80 points. Neither option offered any associated gains (Figure 1c). For the ‘gains only’ and ‘losses only’ trials, the dependent measure was the proportion of choices on which volunteers chose the guaranteed outcome.

All 10 trial types were presented pseudorandomly within four blocks. At the beginning of each block, volunteers were given 100 experimenter-defined points, and asked to make choices that would increase this amount by as much as possible. These points had no monetary value. Visual feedback was given after each choice and the revised points total was presented for 2 s before the next trial. Across the four blocks, there were eight repetitions of each ‘experimental’ gamble and eight repetitions of the ‘gains only’ and ‘losses only’ trial types.

Analysis

All the data were analysed with SPSS (Version 9.0; SPSS Inc., Cary, NC). The dependent measures of the decision-making task were the proportions of trials on which volunteers chose the ‘experimental’ over the control gamble (‘proportionate choice’), and the mean deliberation time (ms) associated with their choices. The proportionate choices were arcsine-transformed, as is appropriate whenever the variance of a measure is proportional to its mean (Howell, 1987); however, all of the data reported in the text, figures, and tables describe untransformed values. The results were analysed using a single repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with the between-subject factors of group (T+ vs T−) and gender (male vs female), and the within-subject factors of probability (high versus low), expected gains (large versus small), and expected losses (large vs small). Discrimination measures for the three factors were analysed by separate ANOVAs, with group as a single between-subjects factor. The ‘gains only’ and ‘losses only’ trials were analysed with group and gender as between-subject factors and trial type (‘gains only’ vs ‘losses only’) as a single within-subject factor.

RESULTS

The T+ and T− volunteers were perfectly matched in terms of gender (nine males and nine females in each group) and were well matched in terms of age (F[1, 32]=0.36, p=0.55) and estimated verbal IQ (F[1, 34]=0.05, p=0.83) (see Table 2). Total plasma tryptophan was significantly reduced in the T− volunteers over the 5 h following consumption of the amino-acid drink (F[1, 16]=425.71, p<0.0001), but increased in the T+ volunteers (F[1, 16]=81.39, p<0.0001). (A verbal IQ score was unavailable for one T− volunteer; one pair of baseline and +5 h plasma samples was un-available for one T− volunteer; and one +5 h plasma sample was unavailable for one T+ volunteer). Critically, at +5 h, the total plasma tryptophan of the T− volunteers was significantly lower than that of the T+ volunteers (F[1, 32]=352.79, p<0.0001).

Table 2 Demographic Details of Volunteers who Consumed an Amino Acid with (T+) and without (T−) L-Tryptophan, and total plasma tryptophan (TRP) (μg/ml) at baseline and at +5 h

Probability, Wins, and Losses

Proportionate choice

In general, volunteers chose the ‘experimental’ gamble significantly more often when its probability of winning was high compared to when it was low (F[1, 32]=128.27, p<0.0001) (see Figure 2a). There was no evidence that this pattern of choice was changed in the T− compared to the T+ volunteers (F[1, 32]=0.38, p=0.54). Similarly, volunteers chose the ‘experimental’ gamble significantly less often when its expected losses were large compared to when its expected losses were small (F[1, 32]=44.97, p<0.0001), and to a similar extent in both groups (F[1, 32]=0.29, p=0.59) (see Figure 2b). However, while all the volunteers chose the ‘experimental’ gamble more often when its expected gains were large compared to when they were small (F[1, 32]=97.67, p<0.0001), this pattern of decision-making was significantly attenuated in the T− compared to the T+ volunteers (F[1, 32]=6.1, p<0.02) (see Figure 2c). Additional analysis showed that both groups of volunteers showed comparable discrimination between different probabilities of winning (F[1, 32]=0.54, p=0.47), and between different magnitudes of expected losses (F[1, 32]=0.37, p=0.55), but that the T− volunteers showed significantly less discrimination than T+ volunteers between different magnitudes of expected gains (F[1, 32]=4.98, p<0.04), (see Figure 2c, inset).

Figure 2
figure 2

Proportion of choices of the ‘experimental’ over the control gamble by the T+ and T− volunteers for the three manipulated factors: (a) high vs low probability of winning (inset the mean difference between the proportions of choices of the ‘experimental’ over the control gamble when its probability of winning was high and the proportions of choices of the ‘experimental’ gamble when its probability of winning was low); (b) large vs small losses (inset the mean difference between the proportions of choices of the ‘experimental’ gamble over the control gamble when its expected losses were large and the proportions of choices of the ‘experimental’ gamble when its expected losses were small); (c) large vs small gains. Group (T+ vs T− volunteers)×expected gains (large vs small): F[1, 32]=6.07, p<0.02 (inset the mean difference between the proportions of choices of the ‘experimental’ over the control gamble when its expected gains were large and the proportions of choices when its gains were small; *F[1, 34]=4.98, p<0.05).

Subsidiary analyses of the data from the 16 T+ volunteers and the 14 T− volunteers who completed psychometric measures of state positive and negative affect confirmed that the reduced discrimination between magnitudes of expected gains in the decision-making of the T− volunteers compared to the T+ volunteers was not dependent upon gross changes in mood induced by tryptophan depletion. In both sets of volunteers, positive affect declined significantly in the 5 h following consumption of the drink (F[1, 26]=6.19, p<0.03) (Table 3), but no more so for T− than for T+ volunteers (F[1, 26]=1.57, p=0.22). Negative affect did not change significantly over this time (F[1, 26]=0.56, p=0.46). Crucially, there were no significant differences between the T− and T+ volunteers in positive or negative affects at cognitive testing (F[1, 28]=0.67, p=0.42 and F[1, 28]=0.38, p=0.54, respectively).

Table 3 State Positive and Negative Affects (PANAS; Watson et al, 1988a), and the Proportion of Choices of the ‘Experimental’ over the Control Gamble in Healthy Volunteers who Consumed an Amino-acid Drink with (T+; n=16) and without (T−; n=14) L-Tryptophan

Despite closely matched state affect in these volunteers, the increased choice of the ‘experimental’ gamble when its expected gains were large relative to when they were small remained significantly attenuated in the T− compared to the T+ volunteers (F[1, 26]=10.92, p<0.005). Analysis of the simple effects indicated that the T− volunteers chose the ‘experimental’ gamble significantly less often than the T+ volunteers when its gains were large (F[1, 26]=6.36, p<0.02) (Table 3). Moreover, the T− volunteers exhibited significantly reduced discrimination between different magnitudes of expected gains associated with the ‘experimental’ gamble compared to the T+ volunteers (F[1, 28]=7.19, p<0.02), but a similar discrimination between different probabilities F[1, 28]=0.69, p=0.41) and magnitudes of losses F[1, 28]=0.53, p=0.47). Finally, entering state positive and negative affect of measures at cognitive testing as covariates into the analysis left the central result unchanged; that is, the T− volunteers showed attenuated choice of the ‘experimental’ gamble over the control gamble as a function of the size of its expected gains (F[1, 24]=10.20, p<0.005).

Overall, male volunteers tended to choose the ‘experimental’ gamble slightly less often than female volunteers (0.50±0.02 vs 0.55±0.02) (F[1, 32]=4.21, p<0.05). There were no significant interactions involving gender and any task variable or/and treatment group. Repeat analyses, including age and estimated verbal IQ as covariates, indicated that age tended to covary with choice of the ‘experimental’ gamble (F[1, 28]=4.03, p=0.06) and, in particular, with the reduction in choice of the ‘experimental’ gamble associated with larger compared to smaller losses (F[1, 28]=4.58, p<0.05). Finally, estimated verbal IQ was not a significant covariate of choice of the ‘experimental’ gamble (F[1, 28]=2.68, p=0.11).

Deliberation times

Volunteers were significantly faster to make their choices when the probability of winning on the ‘experimental’ gamble was high compared to when it was low (F[1, 32]=35.23, p<0.0001). This effect was not greater in the T− volunteers compared to the T+ volunteers (F[1, 32]<0.001, p=0.98) (see Table 4). All volunteers made significantly slower choices when the ‘experimental’ gamble was associated with large expected losses compared to small expected losses (F[1, 32]=7.29, p<0.02), but significantly faster choices when it was associated with large expected gains compared to small expected gains (F[1, 32]=6.32, p<0.02). Neither of these effects were significantly altered in the T− volunteers compared to the T+ volunteers (F[1, 32]=0.28, p=0.60) and F[1, 32]=0.27, p=0.61, respectively). Overall, the T− volunteers tended to make their choices faster than the T+ volunteers (1566±102 vs 1913±161 ms) (F[1, 32]=3.30, p=0.08), as did male compared to female volunteers (1557±117 vs 1922±151 ms) (F[1, 32=3.66, p=0.07).

Table 4 Mean Deliberation Times (ms) as a Function of High and Low Levels of Probability of Winning, High and Low Levels of Expected Losses and Expected Gains in Healthy Volunteers Who Consumed an Amino-Acid Drink with (T+; n=16) and without (T−; n=14) L-Tryptophan

Repeat analyses of the mean deliberation times including state positive and negative affect measured at +5 h following consumption of the amino-acid drink, or age and estimated verbal IQ, as covariates did not reveal any significant interactions involving treatment group or probability of winning on the ‘experimental’ gamble or the size of expected gains or losses.

‘Gains Only’ vs ‘Losses Only’ Trials

Proportionate choice

Volunteers overwhelmingly chose the guaranteed outcome significantly more often on the ‘gains only’ trials (offering a choice between a certain gain of 40 points and a 50% chance of 80 or 0 points) than on the ‘losses only’ trials (offering a certain loss of 40 points or a 50% chance of a loss of 80 or 0 points) (F[1, 32]=90.54, p<0.0001) (Figure 3). However, this pattern of choices (risk-aversion when choosing between gains and risk-seeking when choosing between losses) was not significantly different in the T− volunteers compared to the T+ volunteers (F[1, 32]=0.88, p=0.36).

Figure 3
figure 3

Proportion of choices of the guaranteed outcome by the T+ and T− volunteers for the ‘gains only’ trials and the ‘losses only’ trials.

Deliberation times

Volunteers were significantly quicker to make their choices on the ‘gains only’ trials compared to the ‘losses only’ trials (F[1, 32]=41.78, p<0.0001), with no difference in the size of this effect in the T− volunteers (1305±149 vs 2689±407 ms) compared to T+ volunteers (1622±214 vs 2871±333 ms) (F[1, 32]=0.11, p=0.74).

DISCUSSION

These data suggest that rapid dietary depletion of tryptophan—reducing central serotonin function (Nishizawa et al, 1997)—alters decision-making in healthy volunteers by attenuating their discrimination between different magnitudes of expected gains. There was little evidence that tryptophan depletion had any similar effects on volunteers’ discrimination between magnitudes of expected losses, or differences in the probabilities with which each of these outcomes were delivered. Moreover, analyses of the data from those volunteers who completed ratings of positive and negative affect suggest that this selective effect on reward processing was not driven by gross changes in mood associated with tryptophan depletion. While volunteers who underwent tryptophan depletion (T−) and those who did not (T+) reported some reduction in positive affect between baseline and cognitive testing, this effect was not significantly greater in the T− compared to the T+ volunteers, and most probably reflects the nonspecific changes in mood experienced across the experimental session. In essence, our results suggest that lowered central serotonergic function alters the decision-making of healthy volunteers principally by altering the processing of reward cues—in this case, reducing the discrimination between small and large rewards—rather than by any obvious effects on the processing of punishment cues or probabilistic cues.

The finding that tryptophan depletion alters the choices of healthy volunteers by reducing their discrimination between magnitudes of reward is consistent with the evidence that serotonin plays a significant role in modulating reward and incentive motivational processes, possibly through the modulation of the mesolimbic dopamine system (Baumgarten and Grozdanovic, 1995). SSRI treatment has been found to dose-dependently modulate brain-stimulation reward thresholds via 5-HT1A receptors (Harrison and Markou, 2001a, 2001b), and to potentiate the stimulant effects of cocaine (Cunningham and Callahan, 1991; Kleven and Koek, 1998). The role of serotonin in reinforcement is also evidenced by the finding that low doses of cocaine block the serotonin transporter more potently than the dopamine transporter (Ritz and Kuhar, 1987), and by the demonstration that both these transporters appear necessary for cocaine's rewarding effects in a conditioned place preference procedure (Sora et al, 2001).

More directly, tryptophan depletion has been shown to diminish the euphoric effects of intravenous cocaine in cocaine addicts (Aronson et al, 1995), and to reduce the craving experienced when presented with cocaine-related paraphernalia (Satel et al, 1995). The latter finding suggests that serotonin affects the way that reward-associated signals influence behaviour; consistent with reports that fluoxetine potentiates the control of ongoing responding by available conditioned reinforcers (Sasaki-Adams and Kelley, 2001; Fletcher et al, 1999). In this context, it is important to note that the results of the present study do not, in themselves, show that tryptophan depletion reduces sensitivity to reward per se rather the data indicate that the T− volunteers did not discriminate as effectively as the T+ volunteers between differences in the magnitude of rewards associated with different choices. Further research will be needed to discover whether this effect is best characterised as involving the attenuated impact of conditioned signals that predict reward or, alternatively, reduced sensitivity to reward itself. Nevertheless, although the effects of serotonin on dopaminergic tone and associated reward-based processing are likely to be highly complex (Sasaki-Adams and Kelley, 2001), the present data do at least suggest that a generalised reduction in central serotonin function undermines decision-making in healthy volunteers by altering the processing of reward-related information when choosing between behavioural options.

As discussed above, several studies employing both lesion and electrophysiological techniques have shown that the capacity to learn stimulus–reward associations—as exemplified by visual discrimination and reversal-learning tasks—depends upon circuitry encompassing the orbitofrontal cortex (Dias et al, 1996; Jones and Mishkin, 1972; Thorpe et al, 1983). Additionally, decision-making involving choices between uncertain rewards and punishments is widely thought to involve the functioning of this same portion of the prefrontal cortex (Bechara et al, 1996; Damasio, 1994; Rogers et al, 1999b, 1999c) while, most recently, Breiter et al (2001) have reported that the ‘expected value’ of monetary outcomes delivered noncontingently to healthy volunteers was associated with increased neural activity within the orbitofrontal cortex and its interconnected neural circuitry. The demonstrations that tryptophan depletion undermines both reversal learning in healthy volunteers (Park et al, 1994; Rogers et al, 1999a) and decision-making through the processing of reward information in the current study suggest that serotonin has a significant modulatory function in the affective processing associated with the orbitofrontal cortex.

All volunteers showed a significant reduction in the ‘experimental’ gamble when the expected losses were large compared to when they were small, suggesting that their choices were influenced by differences in the magnitude of expected punishments. However, there was no sign that the size of this effect was greater in the T− compared to the T+ volunteers. This result may seem surprising in view of serotonin's proposed role in the modulation of anxiety (Deakin and Graeff, 1991; Gray, 1987; Iversen, 1984), and the specific proposal that serotonergic activity mediates anticipatory anxiety states activated by aversive stimuli (Deakin and Graeff, 1991; Melik et al, 2000; Tye et al, 1977; Wilkinson et al, 1995). Of course, the involvement of serotonin in mediating anxiety remains controversial with, for example, continuing difficulties in demonstrating impaired conditioning to discrete stimuli as opposed to contextual stimuli following serotonin depletions (Mansbach and Geyer, 1988; Wilkinson et al, 1995). However, we suggest tentatively two factors that may account for the absence of altered decision-making through effects on the processing of punishment cues in this study.

First, it might have been that the sizes of the expected losses were too small to serve as effective punishment signals and engage sufficiently the serotonin system in the aversive aspect of the decision-making task. However, as against this, Figure 2b and Table 4 clearly show that volunteers did find the prospect of large losses sufficiently aversive to induce them to reduce their choice of the ‘experimental’ gamble alongside a marked increase in the associated deliberation time. Second, the size of the smaller and larger rewards was matched perfectly with the size of the smaller and larger punishments (20 vs 80 points in each case) so that the relative balance between approach and avoidant response tendencies activated by the gains and losses would have been roughly balanced across the trials. In this case, there may have been an insufficient requirement for inhibition of activated approach responses on the presentation of aversive cues consisting of large expected losses. Such an inhibitory operation might have been undermined by reduced central serotonin (Soubrie, 1986). We are currently exploring these possibilities.

The lack of an effect of tryptophan depletion on the processing of probabilistic cues is unsurprising in view of the evidence suggesting, for example, that 5,7-hydroxytryptamine- induced lesions in rats do not disrupt or change substantially the discounting of differences in the probability with which particular rewards are delivered (Mobini et al, 2000). However, in the present study, it was clear that both the T− and T+ volunteers found the discrepancy between the low probability (25%) and high probability (75%) of reward and punishment in the ‘experimental’ gambles particularly large, perhaps reducing the sensitivity of this particular measure in detecting differences as the result of tryptophan depletion. Nevertheless, with this acknowledged caveat, we can conclude that the failure to find an effect of tryptophan depletion is at least consistent with the existing literature that serotonin function is not clearly implicated in the processing of probabilistic information.

Finally, the finding that the effects of reduced tryptophan on discrimination between the magnitudes of reward may have important clinical implications. Several theories have suggested that lowered mood and anhedonia in depression are associated with altered processing of reward (Lewinsohn and Graf, 1973) and a failure to activate appetitive responses upon presentation of reward cues (Henriques and Davidson, 1991; Henriques et al, 1994). Other researchers have linked depression explicitly with reduced positive affect measured psychometrically (Watson et al, 1988a); and depressive symptomatology, including difficulties in the experience of pleasure, to altered serotonergic modulation of dopamine function (Van Praag et al, 1987). The motivational/affective aspects of withdrawal phenomena have been linked to serotonergic reductions in limbic circuitry of drug-dependent individuals (Markou et al, 2001). In the present study, subsidiary analyses suggested that tryptophan depletion did not affect the way in which reward cues were processed in the context of decision-making through gross changes in positive or negative affect (Watson et al, 1988b). Nevertheless, tryptophan depletion in the T− volunteers did produce a cognitive and emotional state in which differences in the probability of outcomes and the magnitude of punishment associated with these outcomes loomed larger than differences in the magnitude of reward in the context of choices between competing courses of action. It is possible that this state, if prolonged, represents a precursor to lowered mood in vulnerable groups. Further research is needed to examine whether such alterations in the use of reward cues in decision-making are found in mood disorders, whether they represent traits or states associated with illness, and whether they can be remediated by upregulation of serotonin function.