Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Brief Communication
  • Published:

An investigation across 45 languages and 12 language families reveals a universal language network

Abstract

To understand the architecture of human language, it is critical to examine diverse languages; however, most cognitive neuroscience research has focused on only a handful of primarily Indo-European languages. Here we report an investigation of the fronto-temporo-parietal language network across 45 languages and establish the robustness to cross-linguistic variation of its topography and key functional properties, including left-lateralization, strong functional integration among its brain regions and functional selectivity for language processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: The language network in native speakers of diverse languages.
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available at https://osf.io/cw89s.

Code availability

The code used to analyze the data in this study is available at https://osf.io/cw89s.

References

  1. Lewis, M. P. Ethnologue: Languages of the World (SIL International, 2009).

  2. Chomsky, N. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use (Greenwood Publishing Group, 1986).

  3. Gibson, E. et al. How efficiency shapes human language. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 389–407 (2019).

  4. Evans, N. & Levinson, S. C. The myth of language universals: language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 429–448 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bates, E., McNew, S., MacWhinney, B., Devescovi, A. & Smith, S. Functional constraints on sentence processing: a cross-linguistic study. Cognition 11, 245–299 (1982).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. & Schlesewsky, M. The importance of linguistic typology for the neurobiology of language. Linguistic Typology 20, 615–621 (2016).

  7. Hudley, A. H. C., Mallinson, C. & Bucholtz, M. Toward racial justice in linguistics: interdisciplinary insights into theorizing race in the discipline and diversifying the profession. Language 96, e200–e235 (2020).

  8. Rueckl, J. G. et al. Universal brain signature of proficient reading: evidence from four contrasting languages. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 15510–15515 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Fedorenko, E., Hsieh, P.-J., Nieto-Castañón, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. & Kanwisher, N. New method for fMRI investigations of language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 1177–1194 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Mahowald, K. & Fedorenko, E. Reliable individual-level neural markers of high-level language processing: a necessary precursor for relating neural variability to behavioral and genetic variability. Neuroimage 139, 74–93 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Fedorenko, E., Behr, M. K. & Kanwisher, N. Functional specificity for high-level linguistic processing in the human brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 16428–16433 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Scott, T. L., Gallée, J. & Fedorenko, E. A new fun and robust version of an fMRI localizer for the frontotemporal language system. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 167–176 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Blank, I. A., Kanwisher, N. & Fedorenko, E. A functional dissociation between language and multiple-demand systems revealed in patterns of BOLD signal fluctuations. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 1105–1118 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Fedorenko, E. & Blank, I. A. Broca’s area is not a natural kind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 270–284 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Duncan, J. The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 172–179 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gurunandan, K., Arnaez-Telleria, J., Carreiras, M. & Paz-Alonso, P. M. Converging evidence for differential specialization and plasticity of language systems. J. Neurosci. 40, 9715–9724 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Nieto-Castañón, A. & Fedorenko, E. Subject-specific functional localizers increase sensitivity and functional resolution of multi-subject analyses. Neuroimage 63, 1646–1669 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. et al. Think globally: cross-linguistic variation in electrophysiological activity during sentence comprehension. Brain Lang. 117, 133–152 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bickel, B., Witzlack-Makarevich, A., Choudhary, K. K., Schlesewsky, M. & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. The neurophysiology of language processing shapes the evolution of grammar: evidence from case marking. PLoS ONE 10, e0132819 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Kemmerer, D. Do language-specific word meanings shape sensory and motor brain systems? the relevance of semantic typology to cognitive neuroscience. Linguistic Typology 20, 623–634 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Albert, M. L. Auditory sequencing and left cerebral dominance for language. Neuropsychologia 10, 245–248 (1972).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E. & Ivry, R. B. Motor sequence learning with the nondominant left hand. A PET functional imaging study. Exp. Brain Res. 146, 369–378 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Schlesewsky, M., Small, S. L. & Rauschecker, J. P. Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition: common computational properties. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 142–150 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Norman-Haignere, S., Kanwisher, N. & McDermott, J. H. Cortical pitch regions in humans respond primarily to resolved harmonics and are located in specific tonotopic regions of anterior auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 19451–19469 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Li, Y., Tang, C., Lu, J., Wu, J. & Chang, E. F. Human cortical encoding of pitch in tonal and non-tonal languages. Nat. Commun. 12, 1161 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Gil, D. Riau Indonesian: a language without nouns and verbs. In: Flexible Word Classes: Typological Studies of Underspecified Parts of Speech 89–130 (Oxford Scholarship Online, 2013).

  27. Beeman, M. Semantic processing in the right hemisphere may contribute to drawing inferences from discourse. Brain Lang. 44, 80–120 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Saxe, R. & Kanwisher, N. People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in ‘theory of mind’. Neuroimage 19, 1835–1842 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Radford, A. et al. Language models are unsupervised multitask learners. https://d4mucfpksywv.cloudfront.net/better-language-models/language_models_are_unsupervised_multitask_learners.pdf (2019).

  30. Schrimpf, M. et al. The neural architecture of language: Integrative modeling converges on predictive processing. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2105646118 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Bender, E. M. Linguistically naïve ! = language independent: why NLP needs linguistic typology. In: Proceedings of the EACL 2009 Workshop on the Interaction between Linguistics and Computational Linguistics: Virtuous, Vicious or Vacuous? 26–32 (Association for Computational Linguistics, 2009).

  32. Chi, E. A., Hewitt, J. & Manning, C. D. Finding universal grammatical relations in multilingual BERT. In: Proceedings of the 58th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics 5564–5577 (Association for Computational Linguistics, 2020).

  33. Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A. & Petitto, L. A. Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible ‘neural signature’ of bilingualism. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 153–169 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Costa, A. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 336–345 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Oldfield, R. C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113 (1971).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Braga, R. M., DiNicola, L. M., Becker, H. C. & Buckner, R. L. Situating the left-lateralized language network in the broader organization of multiple specialized large-scale distributed networks. J. Neurophysiol. 124, 1415–1448 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Fedorenko, E., Nieto-Castanon, A. & Kanwisher, N. Lexical and syntactic representations in the brain: an fMRI investigation with multi-voxel pattern analyses. Neuropsychologia 50, 499–513 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Blank, I., Balewski, Z., Mahowald, K. & Fedorenko, E. Syntactic processing is distributed across the language system. Neuroimage 127, 307–323 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Fedorenko, E., Blank, I. A., Siegelman, M. & Mineroff, Z. Lack of selectivity for syntax relative to word meanings throughout the language network. Cognition 203, 104348 (2020).

  40. Chen, X. et al. The human language system does not support music processing. Preprint at https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.06.01.446439v1.full.pdf (2021).

  41. Carroll, L. Alice’s Adventures in Wonderland (Broadview Press, 2011).

  42. Lindseth, J. & Tannenbaum, A. Alice in a World of Wonderlands: The Translations of Lewis Carroll’s Masterpiece (Oak Knoll Press, 2015).

  43. Wolff, P. Observations on the early development of smiling. Determ. Infant Behav. 2, 113–138 (1963).

    Google Scholar 

  44. Thesen, S., Heid, O., Mueller, E. & Schad, L. R. Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image-based tracking for real-time fMRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 457–465 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. & Nieto-Castanon, A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2, 125–141 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Behzadi, Y., Restom, K., Liau, J. & Liu, T. T. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage 37, 90–101 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Cordes, D. et al. Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in ‘resting-state’ data. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 22, 1326–1333 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Dale, A., Fischl, B. & Sereno, M. Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194 (1999).

  49. Kriegeskorte, N., Simmons, W. K., Bellgowan, P. S. F. & Baker, C. I. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 535–540 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Silver, N. C. & Dunlap, W. P. Averaging correlation coefficients: should Fisher’s z transformation be used? J. Appl. Psychol. 72, 146–148 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Seghier, M. L. Laterality index in functional MRI: methodological issues. Magn. Reson. Imaging 26, 594–601 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Duncan, J. The structure of cognition: attentional episodes in mind and brain. Neuron 80, 35–50 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Paunov, A. M., Blank, I. A. & Fedorenko, E. Functionally distinct language and Theory of Mind networks are synchronized at rest and during language comprehension. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 1244–1265 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J. & Kanwisher, N. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 16616–16621 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Lipkin, B. et al. LanA (Language Atlas): a probabilistic atlas for the language network based on fMRI data from >800 individuals. Preprint at https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2022.03.06.483177v1 (2022).

  57. Rombouts, S. A. R. B. et al. Test–retest analysis with functional MR of the activated area in the human visual cortex. Am. Soc. Neuroradiol. 18, 195–6108 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  58. Davis, M. & Johnsrude, I. Hierarchical processing in spoken language comprehension. J. Neurosci. 23, 3423–3431 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Hervais-Adelman, A. G., Carlyon, R. P., Johnsrude, I. S. & Davis, M. H. Brain regions recruited for the effortful comprehension of noise-vocoded words. Language and Cognitive Processes 27, 1145–1166 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Erb, J., Henry, M. J., Eisner, F., & Obelser, J. The brain dynamics of rapid perceptual adaptation to adverse listening conditions. J. Neurosci. 33, 10688–10697 (2013).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Z. Fan, F. Frank and J. Vera-Rebollar for help with finding and recording the speakers; Z. Fan, J. Vera-Rebollar, F. Frank, A. Verkerk, the Max Planck Institute in Nijmegen, C. Kidd and M. Xiang for help with locating the texts of Alice in Wonderland in different languages; I. Blank, A. Paunov, B. Lipkin, D. Greve and B. Fischl for help with some of the analyses; J. McDermott for letting us use the sound booths in his laboratory for the recordings; J. Wu, N. Jhingan and B. Lipkin for creating a website for disseminating the localizer materials and script; M. Lewis for allowing us to use the linguistic family maps from the GeoCurrents website; B. A. Cabrera for help with figures; EvLab and TedLab members and collaborators; the audiences at the Neuroscience of Language Conference at NYU-AD (2019) and at the virtual Cognitive Neuroscience Society conference (2020) for helpful feedback; T. Gibson, D. Blasi, M. Seghier and two anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript; Y. Diachek for collecting the data for the Russian speakers (used in Supplementary Fig. 4); J. Pryor and S. Lall for promoting this work when it was still at the early stages; and our participants. The authors would also like to acknowledge the Athinoula A. Martinos Imaging Center at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research at MIT and the support team (S. Shannon and A. Takahashi). S.M.-M. was supported by la Caixa Fellowship LCF/BQ/AA17/11610043, a Friends of McGovern Fellowship and the Dingwall Foundation Fellowship. E.F. was supported by NIH awards R00-HD057522, R01-DC016607 and R01-DC-NIDCD and research funds from the Brain and Cognitive Sciences Department, the McGovern Institute for Brain Research and the Simons Center for the Social Brain.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, project administration and supervision: E.F. Methodology: S.M.-M., D.A., J.G. and E.F. Investigation (data collection): S.M.-M., D.A., J.G., J.A., Z.M. and O.J. Data curation: S.M.-M., D.A. and J.A. Formal analysis: S.M.-M. Validation: S.M.-M. and J.A. Visualization: S.M.-M. and M.H. Software: S.M.-M., D.A., J.A. and Z.M. Writing—original draft: S.M.-M., D.A. and E.F. Writing—review and editing: J.G., J.A., M.H., Z.M. and O.J.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Saima Malik-Moraleda or Evelina Fedorenko.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Neuroscience thanks M. Florencia Assaneo, Narly Golestani and Mohamed Seghier for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Comparison of the individual activation maps for the Sentences > Nonwords contrast and the Native-language > Degraded-language contrast in the two native-English-speaking participants.

The two maps are voxel-wise (within the union of the language parcels) spatially correlated at r = 0.77 and r = 0.99 for participants 492 and 502, respectively (the correlations are Fisher-transformed). Across the full set of participants, the average Fisher-transformed spatial correlation between the maps for the Sentences > Nonwords contrast in English and the Native-language > Degraded-language contrast in the participant’s native language (again, constrained to the language parcels) is r = 0.88 (SD = 0.43) for the left hemisphere and 0.73 (SD = 0.38) for the right hemisphere. (Note that using the union of the language parcels rather than the whole brain is conservative for computing these correlations; including all the voxels would inflate the correlations due to the large difference in activation levels between voxels that fall within the language parcels vs. outside their boundaries. Instead, we are zooming in on the activation landscape within the frontal, temporal, and parietal areas that house the language network and showing that these landscapes are spatially similar between the two contrasts in their fine-grained activation patterns).

Extended Data Fig. 2 Activation maps for the Alice language localizer contrast (Native-language > Degraded-language) in the right hemisphere of a sample participant for each language (see Fig. 1 for the maps from the left hemisphere).

A significance map was generated for each participant by FreeSurfer44; each map was smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 4 mm full-width half-max and thresholded at the 70th percentile of the positive contrast for each participant (this was done separately for each hemisphere; note that the same participants are used here as those used in Fig. 1). The surface overlays were rendered on the 80% inflated white-gray matter boundary of the fsaverage template using FreeView/FreeSurfer. Opaque red and yellow correspond to the 80th and 99th percentile of positive-contrast activation for each subject, respectively. Further, here and in Fig. 1, small and/or idiosyncratic bits of activation (relatively common in individual-level language mapsfor example, 9, 10) were removed. In particular, clusters were excluded if a) their surface area was below 100 mm^2, or b) they did not overlap (by > 10%) with a mask created for a large number (n = 80456) participants by overlaying the individual maps and excluding vertices that did not show language responses in at least 5% of the cohort. (We ensured that the idiosyncrasies were individual- and not language-specific: for each cluster removed, we checked that a similar cluster was not present for the second native speaker of that language.) These maps were used solely for visualization; all the statistical analyses were performed on the data analyzed in the volume.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Volume-based activation maps for the Native-language > Degraded-language contrast in the left hemisphere of a sample participant for each language (the same participants are used as those used in Fig. 1 and Extended Fig. 2).

a) Binarized maps that were generated for each participant by selecting the top 10% most responsive (to this contrast) voxels within each language parcel. These sets of voxels correspond to the fROIs used in the analyses reported in Extended Data Fig. 4 (except for the estimation of the responses to the conditions of the Alice localizer, where a subset of the runs was used to ensure independence; the fROIs in those cases will be similar but not identical to those displayed). b) Whole-brain maps that are thresholded at the p < 0.001 uncorrected level.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Percent BOLD signal change across (panel a) and within each of (panel b) the LH language functional ROIs (defined by the Native-language > Degraded-language contrast from the Alice localizer, cf. the Sentences > Nonwords contrast from the English localizer as in the main text and analyses; Fig. 3a and Supplementary Fig. 3) for the three language conditions of the Alice localizer task (Native language, Acoustically degraded native language, and Unfamiliar language), the spatial working memory (WM) task and the math task.

The dots correspond to languages (n = 45), and the labels (panel a only) mark the averages for each language family. In all panels, box plots include the first quartile (lower hinge), third quartile (upper hinge), and median (central line); upper and lower whiskers extend from the hinges to the largest value no further than 1.5 times the inter-quartile range; darker-colored dots correspond to outlier data points. Across the six fROIs, the Native-language condition elicits a reliably greater response than both the Degraded-language condition (2.32 vs. 0.91 % BOLD signal change relative to the fixation baseline; t(44)=18.57, p < 0.001) and the Unfamiliar-language condition (2.32 vs. 0.99; t(44)=18.02, p < 0.001). Responses to the Native-language condition are also significantly higher than those to the spatial working memory task (2.32 vs. 0.06; t(44)=11.16, p < 0.001) and the math task (2.32 vs. −0.02; t(40)=20.8, p < 0.001). These results also hold for each fROI separately, correcting for the number of fROIs (Native-language > Degraded-language: ps<0.05; Native-language > Unfamiliar-language: ps<0.05; Native-language > Spatial WM: ps<0.05; and Native-language > Math: ps<0.05). All t-tests were two-tailed and corrected for the number of fROIs in the per-fROI analyses.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Percent BOLD signal change across the LH language functional ROIs (defined by the Sentences > Nonwords contrast) for the three language conditions of the Alice localizer task (Native language, Acoustically degraded native language, and Unfamiliar language), the spatial working memory (WM) task, and the math task shown for each language separately.

The dots correspond to participants for each language (n = 2 in all languages except Slovene, Swahili, Tagalog, Telugu, where n = 1). Box plots include the first quartile (lower hinge), third quartile (upper hinge), and median (central line); upper and lower whiskers extend from the hinges to the largest value no further than 1.5 times the inter-quartile range; darker-colored dots correspond to outlier data points. (Note that the scale of the y-axis differs across languages in order to allow for easier between-condition comparisons in each language).

Extended Data Fig. 6 A comparison of individual LH topographies between speakers of the same language vs. between speakers of different languages.

The goal of this analysis was to test whether inter-language / inter-language-family similarities might be reflected in the similarity structure of the activation patterns. To perform this analysis, we computed a Dice coefficient57 for each pair of individual activation maps for the Intact-language > Degraded-language contrast (a total of n = 3,655 pairs across the 86 participants). To do so, we used the binarized maps like those shown in Extended Data Fig. 3a, where in each LH language parcel the top 10% of most responsive voxels were selected. Then, for each pair of images, we divided the number of overlapping voxels multiplied by 2 by the sum of the voxels across the two images (this value was always the same and equaling 1,358 given that each map had the same number of selected voxels). The resulting values can vary from 0 (no overlapping voxels) to 1 (all voxels overlap). a) A comparison of Dice coefficients for pairs of maps between languages (left, n = 3,655 pairs) vs. within languages (right; this could be done for 41/45 languages for which two speakers were tested). If the activation landscapes are more similar within than between languages, then the Dice coefficients for the within-language comparisons should be higher. Instead, no reliable difference was observed by an independent-samples t-test (average within-language: 0.17 (SD = 0.07), average between-language: 0.16 (SD = 0.06); t(40.7)=−0.52, p = 0.61; see also Extended Data Fig. 8 for evidence that the range of overlap values in probabilistic atlases created from speakers of diverse languages vs. speakers of the same language are comparable). Box plots include the first quartile (lower hinge), third quartile (upper hinge), and median (central line); upper and lower whiskers extend from the hinges to the largest value no further than 1.5 times the inter-quartile range; darker-colored dots correspond to outlier data points. b) Dice coefficient values for all pairs of within- and between-language comparisons (the squares in black on the diagonal correspond to languages with only one speaker tested). As can be seen in the figure and in line with the results in panel a, no structure is discernible that would suggest greater within-language / within-language-family topographic similarity. Similar to the results from the within- vs. between-language comparison in a, the within-language-family vs. between-language-family comparison did not reveal a difference (t(19.8)=0.71, p = 0.49). In summary, in the current dataset (collected with the shallow sampling approach, that is, a small number of speakers from a larger number of languages), no clear similarity structure is apparent that would suggest more similar topographies among speakers of the same language, or among speakers of languages that belong to the same language family.

Extended Data Fig. 7 Inter-region functional correlations in the language and the Multiple Demand networks during story comprehension for each of the 45 languages.

Inter-region functional correlations for the LH and RH of the language and the Multiple Demand (MD) networks during a naturalistic cognition paradigm (story comprehension in the participant’s native language) shown for each language separately.

Extended Data Fig. 8 Comparison of three probabilistic overlap maps (atlases).

Comparison of three probabilistic overlap maps (atlases): a) the Alice atlas (n = 86 native speakers of 45 languages) created from the Native-language > Degraded-language maps; b) the English atlas (n = 629 native English speakers; this is a subset of the Fedorenko lab’s Language Atlas (LanA56) created from the Sentences > Nonwords maps; and) the Russian Atlas (n = 19 native Russian speakers) created from the Native-language > Degraded-language maps for the Russian version of the Alice localizer. All three atlases were created by selecting for each participant the top 10% of voxels (across the brain) based on the t-values for the relevant contrast in each participant, binarizing these maps, and then overlaying them in the common space. In each atlas, the value in each voxel corresponds to the proportion of participants (between 0 and 1) for whom that voxel belongs to the 10% of most language-responsive voxels. The probabilistic landscapes are similar across the atlases: within the union of the language parcels (see Extended Data Fig. 1 caption for an explanation of why this approach is more conservative than performing the comparison across the brain), the Alice atlas is voxel-wise spatially correlated with both the English atlas (r = 0.83) and the Russian atlas (r = 0.85). Furthermore, the range of non-zero overlap values is comparable between the Alice atlas (0.1–0.87; average within the language parcels=0.08, median=0.05) and each of the other atlases (the English atlas: 0.002–0.79; average within the language parcels=0.07, median=0.03; the Russian atlas: 0.05–0.84; average within the language parcels=0.13, median=0.11). The latter result suggests that the inter-individual variability in the topographies of activation landscapes elicited in 86 participants of 45 diverse languages is comparable to the inter-individual variability observed among native speakers of the same language.

Extended Data Fig. 9 Responses in the domain-general Multiple Demand network to the conditions of the Alice localizer task, the spatial working memory task, and the math task.

Percent BOLD signal change across the domain-general Multiple Demand (MD) network15,52 functional ROIs for the three language conditions of the Alice localizer task (Native language, Acoustically degraded native language, and Unfamiliar language), the hard and easy conditions of the spatial working memory (WM) task, and the hard and easy conditions of the math task. The dots correspond to languages (n = 45 except for the Math Task, where n = 41). Box plots include the first quartile (lower hinge), third quartile (upper hinge), and median (central line); upper and lower whiskers extend from the hinges to the largest value no further than 1.5 times the inter-quartile range; darker-colored dots correspond to outlier data points. As in the main analyses (Fig. 3c), the individual MD fROIs were defined by the Hard > Easy contrast in the spatial WM task (see54 for evidence that other Hard > Easy contrasts activate similar areas). As expected given past worke.g., 54, the MD fROIs show strong responses to both the spatial WM task and the math task, with stronger responses to the harder condition in each (3.05 vs. 1.93 for the spatial WM task, t(44)=23.1, p < 0.001; and 1.68 vs. 0.62 for the math task, t(40)=8.87, p < 0.001). These robust responses in the MD network suggest that the lack of responses to the spatial WM and math tasks in the language areas can be meaningfully interpreted. Furthermore, in line with past work e.g.58,59,60, MD fROIs show a stronger response to the acoustically degraded condition than the native language condition (0.26 vs. -0.10, t(44)=4.92, p < 0.01), and to the unfamiliar language condition than the native language condition (0.15 vs. -0.10, t(44)=4.96, p < 0.01). All t-tests were two-tailed with no adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Extended Data Fig. 10 Comparison of the individual activation maps for the Native-language > Degraded-language contrast and the Native-language > Unfamiliar-language contrast in four sample participants.

The activation landscapes are broadly similar: across the full set of 86 participants, the average Fisher-transformed voxel-wise spatial correlation within the union of the language parcels between the maps for the two contrasts is r = 0.66 (SD = 0.40). (Note that this correlation is lower than the correlation between the Native-language > Degraded-language contrast and the Sentences > Nonwords contrast in English (see Extended Data Fig. 1). This difference may be due to the greater variability in the participants’ responses to an unfamiliar language.) Furthermore, across the language fROIs, the magnitudes of the Native-language > Degraded-language and the Native-language > Unfamiliar-language effects are similar (mean = 1.02, SD(across languages)=0.41 vs. mean=1.07, SD = 0.37, respectively; t(44)=1.15, p = 0.26).

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figs. 1–8 and Supplementary Tables 1–4

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Malik-Moraleda, S., Ayyash, D., Gallée, J. et al. An investigation across 45 languages and 12 language families reveals a universal language network. Nat Neurosci 25, 1014–1019 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01114-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01114-5

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing