Introduction

Serotonin (5-HT) neurotransmission has a key role in stress sensitivity and in vulnerability to negative affect.1, 2 The serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) encodes the serotonin transporter protein (5-HTT), which acts as a key regulator by removing serotonin from the synaptic cleft. A variation in the promoter region of SLC6A4, serotonin transporter-linked promoter region (5-HTTLPR), influences its transcriptional activity and regulates 5-HTT expression and density in human cell lines.1, 3, 4 The 5-HTTLPR is a repeat polymorphism with long (L) and short (S) alleles. The S allele is associated with lower 5-HTT expression and function, as well as anxiety and negative mood in healthy individuals.3 The recently discovered A/G single-nucleotide polymorphism (rs25531) within the length polymorphism led to the distinction between the variants LA and LG, with the latter one found to be functionally similar to the S allele.5, 6 As the rs25531 G allele is almost always in phase with the L allele,7 5-HTTLPR is often considered a triallelic polymorphism (LA, LG and S).

Besides the intense study of the interplay between the 5-HTTLPR and stress-reactive phenotypes like depression, anxiety and substance use, the study of endophenotypes like neural activation has gained great scientific interest in this context. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies consistently demonstrated that carriers of the S allele show augmented amygdala response to salient environmental stimuli, particularly unpleasant stimuli indicating increased stress sensitivity.8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13 So far, however, it is not clear how this genotype effect on amygdala function is mediated. One plausible mechanism could be through in vivo 5-HTT levels (as depicted in the model in Figure 1), which were shown to be negatively associated with amygdala activity.14 However, imaging studies investigating the association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT expression in vivo, have revealed inconsistent results. In healthy LL, particularly LALA carriers, several positron emission tomography (PET) studies using the selective and non-competitively binding 5-HTT ligand [11C]DASB reported increased 5-HTT availability in the midbrain,15 putamen16 and caudate.17 Using the tracer [123I]β-CIT, one single photon emission computed tomography study found increased 5-HTT availability in the midbrain of healthy LL carriers,18 which were later all shown to be carriers of two LA alleles.15 Another [123I]β-CIT single photon emission computed tomography study reported equal 5-HTT levels in homozygous S- and L-allele carriers, which were increased in comparison with heterozygotes.19 Other single photon emission computed tomography and PET studies using the ligands [123I]β-CIT, [11C]McN5652 and [11C]DASB have found no significant association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT.20, 21, 22, 23, 24 These inconsistencies partly reflect that, beyond the genetic impact, 5-HTT levels are affected by numerous additional factors, including environmental influences like daylight,17, 25 stress,26 alcohol and tobacco,18, 27, 28 and parameters such as sex and age.28, 29, 30 Furthermore, methodological differences between the studies might explain discrepant results. For example, some radioligands such as [123I]β-CIT compete with serotonin for binding to 5-HTT31, whereas others such as [11C]DASB do not.

Figure 1
figure 1

Model of association between 5-HTTLPR, 5-HTT availability and amygdala reactivity. Previous studies indicate that the genotype effect on amygdala reactivity (that is, increased amygdala reactivity in S-allele carriers) is mediated by 5-HTT and/or neurodevelopmentally induced effects on brain structure and function. However, although reduced 5-HTT availability has been associated with increased amygdala activation, findings on the association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT availability have been controversial, as indicated by the dotted line arrow. Inconsistent results might be due to the influence of environmental factors like daylight, stress and smoking on 5-HTT availability.

In addition or alternatively, the association between the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and amygdala function could be mediated by genetic effects on neurodevelopment, which shapes the structure and function of brain regions critical for negative emotion processing10, 12 (Figure 1). In line with this, the genetically driven loss of 5-HTT function in 5-HTT knockout mice led to morphological alterations in the basolateral amygdala.32 Healthy S-allele carriers may not only show increased amygdala response to emotional stimuli but also reduced amygdala volume.12, 33 Smaller amygdala volume and increased amygdala response to emotional stimuli are also observable in unmedicated patients with depression,34, 35, 36 although those structural alterations do not necessarily drive functional amygdala reactivity.12

Our multimodal study aimed to elucidate whether the polymorphic effect of the serotonin transporter gene on amygdala function is mediated by its influence on in vivo 5-HTT and/or amygdala structure. Thus, for the first time, we determined 5-HTTLPR and measured amygdala reactivity and amygdala volumes using fMRI and structural MRI and in vivo 5-HTT availability using [11C]DASB PET in the same individuals. We were furthermore interested in the effect of nicotine dependence and therefore investigated both non-smokers and smokers. We hypothesized that (1) 5-HTTLPR would be associated with 5-HTT levels, with S-allele carriers showing lower 5-HTT levels in vivo, and that this effect would explain the polymorphic effect on amygdala reactivity (increased amygdala activation in S-allele carriers). Consequently, and in line with a previous study, we furthermore expected to find a negative association between 5-HTT levels and amygdala reactivity.14 We additionally hypothesized that (2) S-allele carriers would show decreased amygdala volumes and that these structural alterations might furthermore contribute to increased amygdala reactivity. With regard to nicotine dependence, we hypothesized that chronic smoking might elevate 5-HTT availability,28 thereby interfering with the 5-HTT genotype.

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 54 healthy individuals were included in the final sample (29 males and 25 females); see below for exclusion criteria. The sample consisted of 29 non-smokers and 26 smokers. Participants were 41.6±7.9 (mean±s.d.) years of age (range: 30–59). They provided informed, written consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The local ethics committee and the Federal Office for Radiation Protection approved the study. Participants were recruited by public announcement.

The Munich-Composite International Diagnostic Interview37 was used to assess axis 1 mental disorders. Exclusion criteria were pregnancy, a lifetime history of alcohol and other substance use disorders (except nicotine dependence for smokers), schizophrenia, bipolar disorder or 12 months prevalence of any other axis I disorder. Non-smokers had smoked a maximum of 20 cigarettes in their lives, but none during the past 12 months. All smokers fulfilled the DSM IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV) criteria for nicotine dependence. They had a Fagerstrøm Test for Nicotine Dependence score of 6.2±1.7 (mean±s.d.) and had smoked 24.5±6.6 (mean±s.d.) cigarettes per day during the past 3 months. A urine test for illicit drug use and for pregnancy was applied. Participants were not taking any medication regularly and declared not to have taken any medication and not to have consumed any alcohol 3 days before the experiment. All participants were right handed (laterality quotient in the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory38 >+50) and had a sufficient visual acuity (binocular (corrected) 0.8).

In all, 12 of the initial 66 participants were excluded from the study because (1) they did not pass thorough screening (and consequently did not undergo brain imaging and blood sampling) (n=4), because of (2) excessive head movement during the functional and/or structural MRI scan (n=5) or because of withdrawn consent in the course of the study (n=3). Non-smokers had participated in another study investigating the effects of nicotine on emotional and motivational processing.39 Those participants underwent two fMRI scans within 2 weeks receiving a nicotine gum in one session and a placebo gum in the other session in a randomized order. For analysis of the current study, the fMRI data from the placebo session were used. Although we think that it is unlikely that a single dose of nicotine alters 5-HTT availability in a persistent manner, a potential confound of our data could be the administration of a 2-mg nicotine gum in an additional fMRI session, which preceded the PET scan in 26 non-smokers. In those cases, the time lag between nicotine application and PET scan was 8.4±5.4 (mean±s.d.) days (range: 3–28 days). Notably, it was not associated with 5-HTT availability in the midbrain (r=−0.03, P=0.89).

The time difference between fMRI and PET scans was 9.8±10.5 (mean±s.d.) days (range: 1–52 days). Participants completed the German version of the state-trait anxiety inventory.40 They scored 28.9±4.7 (mean±s.d.; range: 21–41) on the state anxiety subscale and 30.8±6.6 (mean±s.d.; range: 20–51) on the trait anxiety subscale.

Genotyping

DNA was isolated from whole blood by standard techniques. 5-HTTLPR variants were determined with PCR techniques. PCR amplification of 5-HTTLPR was as follows: in a total volume of 20 μl, 25 ng of genomic DNA was amplified with the primers as reported previously:7 forward 5′-TCCTCCGCTTTGGCGCCTCTTCC-3′ and reverse 5′-TGGGGGTTGCAGGGGAGATCCTG-3′, in the presence of 1 × Promega PCR Master Mix (Promega Corporation, Madison, WI, USA), without multiplexing. PCR conditions were: 5 min at 95 °C, followed by 35 cycles of 30 s at 95 °C, 90 s at 70 °C, 60 s at 72 °C, and a final extension of 10 min at 72 °C. Detection of rs25531 by restriction digest was performed as follows: 20 μl PCR product was digested with 10 Units HpaII (New England BioLabs, Ipswich, MA, USA) for 4 h at 37 °C. Samples were run on 3% agarose gel and precast stained with GelRed (Biotium, Hayward, CA, USA). To score the genotypes, undigested and digested PCR products of the same sample were run on adjacent lanes as described previously.7

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Task

Affectively unpleasant, pleasant and neutral pictures from the International Affective Picture System41 were used to elicit amygdala activation during fMRI. For each category, 40 slides were presented for 2000 ms in an individually randomized order using an event-related design. The inter-trial interval was randomized between 2.0 and 9.9 s (mean 6.0 s). During the inter-trial interval, a fixation cross was presented. Participants were instructed to passively view the stimuli, as even simple rating tasks can alter brain activation.42 To control for attention, a recognition memory test was conducted inside the scanner directly after the scan. The scanning time was 16 min and 40 s. For further details, see our previous publications.39, 43

fMRI data acquisition and analysis

Scanning was performed using a 3-T whole-body MRI scanner (Magnetom TRIO; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a standard head coil. For functional imaging, a standard echo planar image (EPI) sequence was used (repetition time=2410 ms; echo time)=25 ms; flip angle: 80°). fMRI scans were obtained from 42 transversal slices, orientated 30° clockwise to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line, with a thickness of 2 mm (1-mm gap), a field of view of 192 × 192 mm and an in-plane resolution of 64 × 64 pixels, resulting in a voxel size of 3 × 3 × 2 mm. Images were presented through goggles using MRI Audio/Video Systems (Resonance Technology, Northridge, CA, USA). Task presentation was performed using Presentation (Version 9.90, Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA, USA).

Data were analyzed with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM5; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). Before data analysis, functional data underwent preprocessing. The first five images were discarded to reduce T1 saturation effects. Data were temporally realigned with descending slice order to minimize temporal differences in slice acquisition. Spatial realignment was performed to correct for head motion over the course of the session. Functional data were normalized to a standard EPI template, resampled with a voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm and smoothed using an isotropic Gaussian kernel (8 mm full-width at half-maximum).

On an individual level, different conditions (such as pleasant, unpleasant and neutral pictures) were modeled as explanatory variables within the context of the general linear model on a voxel-by-voxel basis. Time derivatives were included in the model to account for temporal differences in the peak of the hemodynamic response.

To interrelate amygdala activation with 5-HTTLPR, 5-HTT and amygdala volume, we extracted the mean signal change from the individual contrast images unpleasant-neutral stimuli applying a bilateral amygdala mask (left amygdala: 161 voxels, right amygdala: 158 voxels) provided by the WFU Pickatlas (Department of Radiology, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC, USA). To demonstrate that unpleasant versus neutral stimuli elicit significant amygdala activation, we calculated one-sample t-tests using SPSS (PASW Statistics 17, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). To additionally illustrate the main effects of the task at the whole brain level, we performed a second-level voxel-wise random-effects full factorial analysis with the factor condition (unpleasant, pleasant, neutral). We applied a threshold of P<0.01, corrected for family-wise error, with a minimum cluster size of n=20 adjacent voxels. For explorative purposes, we also extracted the mean amygdala signal change from the individual contrast images pleasant-neutral stimuli and unpleasant-pleasant stimuli.

Structural MRI data acquisition and analysis

For analysis of brain volume, a three-dimensional T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo image data set was acquired (repetition time=1900 ms, echo time=2.26 ms, field of view=256 × 256 mm, 176 slices, 1 × 1 × 1 mm voxel size, flip angle=9°). The scanning time for the magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo was 5 min 59 s. To measure the volumes of the left and right amygdala, masks of the amygdala structure were created using the automated, model-based, segmentation tool FIRST of FMRIB's Software Library.44, 45 To account for differences in head sizes, total intracranial volume was measured using FreeSurfer (Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA, USA)46, 47 and used as a covariate in the analysis.

Positron emission tomography

PET data acquisition and analysis

[11C]DASB was synthesized as described previously.48 After an intravenous bolus injection of 20mCi [11C]DASB, cerebral radioactivity distribution was measured over 90 min using a GE Advance PET scanner (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI, USA) in two-dimensional acquisition mode. For attenuation correction, a transmission scan with 500.000 kilocounts was used. Three external markers had been attached to the skull to support realignment. Filtered backprojection (128 × 128 pixel=30 cm) with a Hanning filter (cutoff 4.6 mm) was used to reconstruct images. Images were realigned and stereotactically normalized with Matlab (Version 7, MathWorks, Sherborn, MA, USA) and SPM2 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience). For stereotactic normalization, early summation images (0–5 min p.i.) were compared with the standard PET perfusion template provided by SPM. Three-dimensional regions of interest for the midbrain, thalamus, amygdala and cerebellum were taken from previous studies with [11C]DASB.15, 26 Although the thalamus, amygdala and cerebellum have been defined manually in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space upon anatomical criteria, the midbrain region of interest has been created for [11C]DASB PET studies by applying an isocontour threshold of BPND>2.5 to a group of healthy subjects. 5-HTT-binding potential BPND=k3/k4=fND × Bmax/KD (fND=free fraction in the first tissue compartment; Bmax=concentration of binding sites, that is, serotonin transporters; KD=dissociation constant) was calculated for each voxel with the multilinear reference tissue model 2.49 The washout from the reference tissue k2R, which is required for this analysis, was estimated as described previously.15

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS. The threshold of statistical significance was set at P<0.05. Those hypotheses which were directed because of consistent findings from previous studies were tested one-tailed; all others two-tailed. We grouped heterozygous and homozygous S-allele carriers together (LL=LALA; S′=LALG/LAS/SS/LGS/LGLG). LL-allele carriers were coded with 0, and S-allele carriers with 1.

Bivariate correlations were conducted to replicate findings of an association between 5-HTTLPR and amygdala activation to unpleasant stimuli and to display relationships among 5-HTTLPR, midbrain 5-HTT availability, left and right amygdala structures and left and right amygdala activations. Data on amygdala activation elicited by pleasant-neutral stimuli and unpleasant-pleasant stimuli and their associations with 5-HTTLPR, 5-HTT availability and amygdala volume are presented in Supplementary Figure S1 and in Supplementary Table S1. Moreover, state and trait anxiety scores were associated with 5-HTTLPR, 5-HTT availability, amygdala volumes and activations (Supplementary Table S1).

Path analyses using SPSS AMOS 17 (Analysis of Moment Structures; IBM Corporation, Somers, NY, USA) were conducted to test our a priori model of the role of 5-HTT availability and amygdala structure in mediating the relationship between 5-HTTLPR and amygdala activation. The path model (Figure 3) comprised connections between 5-HTTLPR, midbrain 5-HTT availability, left and right amygdala structures and left amygdala activation. In the model, 5-HTTLPR has direct effects on left amygdala activation, bilateral amygdala volumes and midbrain 5-HTT availability and has indirect effects on left amygdala activation through midbrain 5-HTT availability and bilateral amygdala volumes. Moreover, 5-HTT availability and bilateral amygdala volumes have direct effects on left amygdala activation. To further explore the influence of smoking status and to control for the influences of covariates, further analyses were conducted: a multi-group analysis testing for differences between smokers and non-smokers and a path model including the covariates sex, age, intracranial volume and state anxiety. In addition, we calculated a path model including correlated error terms of the left and right amygdala volumes to account for a possible dependency of residual variances (that is, not accounted for by the paths in the model). Significance for all relationships was determined at P<0.05; trends were noted at P<0.10. Model fits were assessed by χ2 goodness-of-fit tests and by root mean square error of approximations, which are two common indices for deviance of the model from the data. They are presented in Supplementary Information.

To further investigate the relationship between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT availability, to test for effects of smoking status on 5-HTT availability and to validate preliminary findings of sex differences in 5-HTT availability and a smoking-specific interaction of those sex differences,28 an ANOVA (analysis of variance) was conducted using midbrain 5-HTT availability as a dependent variable, 5-HTTLPR, smoking status and sex as fixed factors and age as covariate.

Results

Genotype data, 5-HTT availability, amygdala volume and amygdala activation in response to unpleasant stimuli

Triallelic genotyping of 5-HTTLPR revealed that of the 54 participants, 16 (29.6%) were LL, 29 (53.7%) were S′L and 9 (16.7%) were S′S′ (LL=LALA; S′L=SLA/LALG; S′S′=SS/SLG/LGLG). Frequencies did not differ significantly from the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (χ2=0.46; P=0.50). 5-HTT availability was highest in the midbrain, thalamus, amygdala and striatum (Supplementary Figure S2). Midbrain 5-HTT availability was highly associated with thalamic 5-HTT availability (r=0.84, P<0.001) and amygdala 5-HTT availability (r=0.67, P<0.001). For subsequent analysis, we used midbrain 5-HTT availability. Distributions of thalamic and amygdala 5-HTT availability are reported in Supplementary Table S2.

Magnetic resonance imaging volumetry showed that the size of the left amygdala was 1266.6±280.5 mm3 (mean±s.d.) and that the size of the right amygdala was 1314.8±330.8 mm3.

The analysis of mean signal change extracted from the bilateral amygdala showed that unpleasant compared with neutral stimuli elicited a significant activation in the right and left amygdala: 0.90±0.88 (mean±s.d.; t(53)=7.53, P=6 × 10–10) and 0.88±1.10 (mean±s.d.; t(53)=6.16, P=1 × 10–7), respectively. The results of the voxel-wise whole-brain analysis are displayed in Figure 2a and in Supplementary Table S3.

Figure 2
figure 2

(a) Amygdala activation during presentation of unpleasant compared with neutral stimuli. The statistical parametric map was overlaid on a template T1-weighted magnetic resonance image at P<0.01, corrected for family-wise error (FWE). MNI coordinates for the maximal t-score in the right and left amygdala are x=22, y=−6; z=−14 (cluster size: 139 voxels; t-score: 9.1) and x=−20; y=−8, z=−16 (cluster size: 100 voxels; t-score: 8.7), respectively. (b) Mean left and mean right amygdala response to unpleasant compared with neutral stimuli in the LL and SS/SL genotypes of triallelic 5-HTTLPR. The S genotype carriers had a higher left amygdala response (r=0.29, P=0.017) but not right amygdala response (r=0.16; P=0.129). Error bars indicate+1 standard error, N=54.

Association between 5-HTTLPR and amygdala activation and the mediating role of 5-HTT availability and amygdala volume

5-HTTLPR was associated with left amygdala activation in response to unpleasant stimuli (r=0.30, P=0.015). The association between 5-HTTLPR and right amygdala activation was not significant (r=0.16, P=0.129, Figure 2b). Table 1 presents bivariate correlations between variables. Path analysis showed that 5-HTTLPR had direct effects on bilateral amygdala volumes (left amygdala volume: path coefficient (β)=−0.30, P=0.012; right amygdala volume: β=−0.28, P=0.017, one-tailed), indicating smaller amygdalae volumes in S-allele carriers (Figure 3). The effect of 5-HTTLPR on midbrain 5-HTT availability was negligible. The path between right amygdala volume and left amygdala activation was significant (β=−0.30, P=0.010, one-tailed), indicating that individuals with small right amygdala volumes showed increased left amygdala activation. Notably, the direct effect of 5-HTTLPR on left amygdala activation was no longer significant (β=0.22, P=0.053, one-tailed). No further relationships were significant (see Supplementary Table S4 and Supplementary Information for a detailed account of the path coefficients). Accounting for the influences of age and sex on midbrain 5-HTT availability, the influence of intracranial volume on amygdala structure and the influence of state anxiety on amygdala activation in response to unpleasant stimuli did not significantly alter the results (see Supplementary Table S5 and Supplementary Figure S3). Finally, the correlation between the error terms of right and left amygdala volumes neither altered the pattern of results nor significantly improved model fit (χ2=2.202, df=1, P>0.1). Thus, the more parsimonious model presented in Figure 3 is to be preferred.

Table 1 Bivariate relationships among midbrain 5-HTT availability, 5-HTTLPR, left and right amygdala volume, left and right amygdala activation, smoking status, sex and age.
Figure 3
figure 3

Path analysis predicting left amygdala activation (L Amy BOLD); path coefficients: 5-HTTLPR (triallelic), 5-HTT availability midbrain (5-HTT Mid), left and right amygdala volumes (L Amy Vol, R Amy Vol). Significant paths are depicted in bold lines.

Association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT availability and influence of smoking status and sex

The ANOVA testing for main effects and interactions of 5-HTTLPR, smoking status and sex in predicting 5-HTT availability revealed no overall differences in midbrain 5-HTT availability between the S and the LL genotypes. Neither did we find a significant influence of smoking status or sex on midbrain 5-HTT availability. However, there was an interaction between 5-HTTLPR and smoking status (F(1,45)=4.61; P=0.037) indicating increased midbrain 5-HTT availability in non-smoking homozygote L-allele carriers compared with smoking homozygote L-allele carriers and decreased midbrain 5-HTT availability in non-smoking S-allele carriers compared with smoking S-allele carriers (Figure 4 and Supplementary Table S2). Notably, we did not find an interaction between smoking status and sex regarding the availability of 5-HTT as reported previously.28

Figure 4
figure 4

Interaction between 5-HTTLPR and smoking status: non-smoking LL genotype carriers showed increased midbrain 5-HTT levels compared to non-smoking S genotype carriers, whereas the reverse effect was found in smokers (interaction between 5-HTTLPR and smoking status: F(1,45)=4.61; P=0.037). In the whole group midbrain 5-HTT availability did not differ between LL and S genotypes. Error bars indicate±1 standard error.

Association between 5-HTT availability and amygdala activation in response to unpleasant stimuli

Midbrain 5-HTT availability was not associated with left amygdala activation (r=−0.001, P=0.496). However, it was negatively correlated with right amygdala activation (r=−0.25, P=0.037, one-tailed; partial correlation controlling for age and sex: r=−0.25, P=0.036, one-tailed, see Supplementary Figure S4). There were no associations between left or right amygdala activation and 5-HTT availability in other brain regions.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that investigated the association between 5-HTTLPR, amygdala activation in response to unpleasant stimuli, amygdala volumes and in vivo 5-HTT availability in the same individuals. We tested whether the frequently reported genotype effect on amygdala function8, 10, 11, 13 (that is, increased amygdala activation in S-allele carriers) is mediated by in vivo serotonin transporter (5-HTT) levels and/or by amygdala structure, which can be shaped through genetically induced influences during neurodevelopment.

As expected, S-allele carriers showed increased left amygdala activation in response to unpleasant compared with neutral stimuli. Contrary to our hypothesis, this genotype effect on amygdala reactivity, however, could not be explained by direct genotype effects on 5-HTT expression: in the whole group, we found no association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT availability. Neither was 5-HTT availability associated with left amygdala activation. Instead, the polymorphic effect of 5-HTTLPR on amygdala reactivity was mediated by amygdala structure. First of all, S-allele carriers had smaller bilateral amygdala volumes and right amygdala volumes predicted left amygdala responses, with smaller volumes being associated with higher activations. Furthermore, in our path model, the direct effect of 5-HTTLPR on amygdala activation was no longer significant. Although decreased amygdala volume and increased amygdala response have been reported in healthy S-allele carriers, as well as in unmedicated patients with depression,8, 10, 12, 36 a direct association between amygdala volume and amygdala response to emotional stimuli has so far only been demonstrated in adolescents with bipolar disorder.50 In line with our data, the association was inverse in this study. A possible mechanism discussed in the literature and potentially explaining our finding is the effect of 5-HTTLPR polymorphic variation on neurodevelopment, which shapes the structure and function of brain regions critical for negative emotion processing. Thus, it is possible that 5-HTTLPR exerts an early neurodevelopmental influence on amygdala structure, which translates the polymorphic variation into the endophenotype ‘amygdala reactivity’. This neurodevelopmental view is supported by studies with 5-HTT knockout mice and rats and with mice and rats prenatally exposed to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI), who both exhibit increased extracellular serotonin levels.51 Those studies show that 5-HTT blockade during early development caused alterations in the morphology of pyramidal neurons in the amygdala32 and increased anxiety and depression which persisted into adulthood (opposing the effects induced by adult SSRI intake).52, 53 It is argued that those behavioral and morphological effects are similar to the effects of the S-allele of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism.

Notably, amygdala volumes did not explain the entire genotype effect on amygdala response to unpleasant stimuli as indicated by the path between 5-HTTLPR and amygdala activation, which showed a weakened (and no longer significant) but not an entirely vanished association. Therefore, it is likely that additional mediators which we did not capture in our model are involved, such as volumes of other brain structures and/or connections between different brain structures.

In line with previous studies,10, 13 the association between 5-HTTLPR and amygdala function was condition specific: it was present in response to unpleasant compared with neutral stimuli, and not in response to pleasant compared with neutral stimuli (which also elicited pronounced amygdala activation). In contrast to those previous studies and despite a high correlation between left and right amygdala activations (r=0.70, P<0.001), 5-HTTLPR unexpectedly was not significantly associated with right amygdala activation. Even more surprisingly, we found that right amygdala volume was stronger associated with left amygdala activation than with right amygdala activation. Interestingly, in the non-human and human primate brains, no major interamygdaloid connections have been found.54, 55 However, functional connectivity is frequently observed in brain regions which are not or only sparsely connected,56 pointing to the mediating role of third regions. Therefore, the high correlation of left and right amygdala activations, which is in line with findings from a recent meta-analysis,57 could be mediated by a third region—for example, the prefrontal cortex—which has strong anatomical and functional connections to the amygdala.10, 58 The mediating influence of a third region might also explain our unexpected finding of an association between right amygdala volume and left amygdala activation. The lack of association between left amygdala volume and left amygdala activation might be due to insufficient power. Future studies will have to replicate our data and clarify the role of laterality.

Interestingly, the association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT availability was modulated by smoking status, with non-smokers replicating previous findings of higher 5-HTT levels in LL genotype carriers compared with S-allele carriers15, 16 and smokers showing opposite patterns. None of the previous studies which found no association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT availability have reported smoking status of participants, and nicotine abuse or dependence was not an explicit exclusion criterion. Possibly, a converse association between 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTT availability in non-smokers and smokers can lead to a leveling out of whole group effects. Strikingly, a [123I]β-CIT single photon emission computed tomography study showed increased midbrain 5-HTT availability in healthy individuals homozygous for the L-allele compared with S-allele carriers and the inverse relationship for alcoholic patients.18 This and our present finding indicate a potential role of chronic substance intake in modulating the association between 5-HTTLPR and in vivo 5-HTT availability, which has to be further explored. With regard to the direct effect of nicotine dependence on 5-HTT availability, however, we could not replicate previous findings of increased 5-HTT availability in smokers28 as we found no significant differences between smokers and non-smokers.

Our finding of an inverse association between right amygdala activity with unpleasant stimuli and midbrain 5-HTT availability complements findings from a previous study,14 although it was less pronounced and not replicated for 5-HTT availability in the amygdala—despite a close association between midbrain and amygdala 5-HTT availability.

A limitation of our study is the relatively small group of homozygous L-allele carriers, which represents the naturally occurring 5-HTTLPR genotype distribution. To increase power, future studies could perform genotyping before assignment to the imaging study to get even sample sizes of L- and S-allele carriers. This would further allow separate analysis of homozygous and heterozygous S-allele carriers.

The fundamental study of the interplay between serotonergic and affective mechanisms in the healthy human brain is a prerequisite for understanding the pathophysiology of mood and anxiety disorders, as well as the effects of pharmacological treatment targeting the serotonergic system. Our multimodal imaging genetic study indicates that the 5-HTTLPR genotype effect on amygdala activation is procured by amygdala structure rather than by current effects of 5-HTT availability. Our findings suggest that the path for this genetic effect on emotional reactivity may be paved during neurodevelopment, thereby concurring with animal studies which demonstrate that prenatal SSRI exposure is accompanied by long-term effects on brain structure and by behavioural effects which can dramatically differ from the effects of 5-HTT blockade during adulthood.51 Future studies need to investigate the effects of prenatal SSRI exposure on human brain structure and function. Although more than two decades have passed since the introduction of the first SSRI, those long-term consequences are completely unknown.

Furthermore, although most previous studies have investigated 5-HTTLPR in relation to amygdala activation or 5-HTT levels, future studies need to highlight the impact of environmental factors in this context and grasp gene–environment interactions, such as the influence of substances like tobacco and alcohol.