Introduction

The European Commission recently voted to place a moratorium on the use of three neonicotinoid insecticides (i.e. imidacloprid, thiamethoxam and clothianidin) after the European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) determined that there were “high acute risks” to bees from these products through several exposure routes1, e.g. by direct dust exposure during seed planting operations2, or by ingestion of residues in guttation droplets3, nectar and pollen from treated plants4. In their assessment of the risks from imidacloprid on bees, the EFSA considered toxic endpoints for acute (3.7 ng/bee) and chronic exposure (20 μg/kg or L, henceforth ppb) that represent a rough consensus of the toxicity studies reported in the literature (see review in5); however, they cautioned that there are no guidelines for chronic and sublethal exposure testing in bees and expressed concern regarding the uncertainty about the biological significance of such exposures.

One way of resolving that uncertainty is to consider time as one of the exposure factors, such as concentration. Indeed, toxicity scaling with time can lend insight into the effects of residual pesticide contamination and can provide guidance to regulators for determining acceptable contamination levels.

Neonicotinoid insecticides are based on the natural toxin nicotine6 and are of particular concern because they bind virtually irreversibly to the nicotinic-acetylcholine receptors in the insect's nervous system7, so the damage can accumulate and therefore the toxic effects can be reinforced with chronic exposure8. Time-to-effect studies lend themselves to a simple time-dependent power-law empirical model which can guide expectations for field toxicity effects9. Here we attempt to quantify risks to honeybees by considering the time-dependent toxicity of imidacloprid, the most ubiquitous of the neonicotinoid pesticides and the number one insecticide sales in the world10. The lethal time-to-effect relationship can be expressed as a power law

where the toxic effect is the time when half of the insects succumb to the toxin (LT50), D is the dose of toxin per unit time and P is the power law exponent. A scaling relationship, such as Equation 1, is useful to extend predicted effects to low doses where experimental data do not exist.

There are only a few studies with honeybees that follow the insects beyond ten days, e.g.11, so we also looked at studies with ants and termites to corroborate the temporal toxicity dependence that we find in the honeybee studies. Finally, we consider a simple toxicological model to explain the experimental data.

Time-to-effect studies with terrestrial insects

The studies analyzed here include cases where the exposure was by direct ingestion of the pesticidal toxin by adult honeybees and ants, a study where the toxin was fed to larvae of stingless bees and finally a study of toxic exposure through treated soil for termites. Rust et al.12 reported time-dependent toxicity measurements for imidacloprid on Argentine ants, Linepithema humile (Mayr). Data for termites (Reticulitermes flavipes (Kollar)) were taken from Ramakrishnan et al.13, who reported mortality and feeding inhibition for termites observed up to 21 days in different soils treated with imidacloprid. The stingless bee Melipona quadrifasciata anthidioides Lepeletier, provides a good subject for studying the effect of the toxin on developing insects because the larva feeds only from food sealed inside the cell, so the total dose ingested can be accurately determined. The study by Tomé et al.14 followed these bees for 45 days from the egg until just after emerging as an adult over a wide range of toxic doses of imidacloprid.

For honeybees (Apis melifera L.), a paper by Suchail et al.15 and another by Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16 report time-to-effect mortality data after chronic exposure to imidacloprid. However, there has been controversy about Suchail et al. results because two other studies did not show such a high sensitivity to the toxin17,18. Hence, we also include time-to-effect measurements from a study by the UK Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs (DEFRA19) and a recent compilation of many researchers' acute median lethal doses (LD50) values for 24, 48, 72 and 96 h periods (FERA20).

Case studies

Poisoning of adult insects by ingestion of imidacloprid

In order to plot the acute toxicity data along with chronic data on the same graph, average daily dose rates were calculated by dividing the acute LD50 values by the time intervals that were used for the measurement. For the chronic data, we use the reported concentration and consumption rates to obtain the daily dose rate. In both cases, the reported consumption of toxin (ng/day) is plotted versus the time of exposure until half the bees have died (LT50).

Most studies follow the insects for 10 days or less, so at low toxin concentrations the LT50 time cannot be reached. This is true for several of the data sets we analysed20, so whenever mortality is reached between 15% and 50% with respect to the control, we extrapolate from the last two time points to estimate the LT50. The paper by Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16 contains a couple of data points with time-to-effect numbers for relatively low sublethal concentrations. Data from their paper (Figure 1) show that 4–8 μg/L (ppb) concentrations of imidacloprid in syrup eventually kill 50% of bees in about 30 days. Figure 1 also shows how we determine the LT50 point from the mortality time series using Abbott's formula21. LT50 times falling between reported sample points are linearly interpolated from neighboring points.

Figure 1
figure 1

Time-to-effect curves for imidacloprid fed to honeybees at 4 and 8 μg/L in syrup (adapted from Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16).

The data for poisoning of adult insects after ingesting imidacloprid are plotted in Figure 2. A power law best-fit line for each data set is also shown. The slope of the fitting line is −1/P, the exponent of power law Equation 1. For example, the LT50 times for the Argentine ants fit remarkably well a simple power law model with roughly t1.7 dependence (r2 = 0.97, p = 0.013). This adds confidence to the utility of the empirical power law model for imidacloprid with these social insects. Recall that simple accumulation to a toxic threshold (Haber's rule) would appear as directly proportional to time (t1.0)22. An exponent larger than one can be interpreted as coming from damaging secondary physiological effects that develop over time. We will come back to this interpretation later.

Figure 2
figure 2

Time-dependent toxicity from ingestion of imidacloprid for honeybees and Argentine ants.

Published insect mortality results from several researchers plotted as time-to-effect versus daily dose per bee. Field exposure range assumes <5 ppb a.i. at 20 μl consumption/day.

The DEFRA honeybee data stand out as not falling within the averages of many other researchers in the FERA report20; about a tenfold difference, despite a good fit to the power model (r2 = 0.99, p < 0.001). There appears to be a wide variation between experiments, which could be due to the individual colony or strain of bees' sensitivity to this insecticide23,or by other factors not considered in the experimental design (temperature, season, condition of bees, nutritive status, etc.) The DEFRA study reports time-to-effect numbers and consumption data, so it provides a good data set for determining time scaling even if the DEFRA bees are not particularly representative of most other bees tested. The mortality for the DEFRA bees can be very well explained by the power law formulation with about t1.6 dependence, very similar time exponent to the ants. Finally, the average LD50 values for 24, 48, 72 and 96 hours, compiled from many researchers, also fits the power law model (r2 = 0.98, p < 0.001), here about t2. The Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16 data are included in this power fit line.

Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16 results, using concentrations of 4 and 8 ppb, (0.08 and 0.16 ng/day, respectively) are relevant to field-realistic exposure levels. Residue concentrations of imidacloprid in pollen and nectar from treated agricultural crops are frequently reported in the 1 to 30 ppb range in France24 and the US25. Bees foraging on those crops could be subject to the test levels used by Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16, which caused 50% mortality in about 30 days.

The Suchail et al.15 results show a much higher sensitivity to imidacloprid than the trends we see with the other honeybee data sets in Figure 2. Rather than a t2 dependence, lethal effects seem to scale as toxic concentration times t5 (r2 = 0.87, p = 0.021). It is tempting to disregard the Suchail et al.15 results; however one test site in trials reported by Schmuck18 also showed very high sensitivity to a metabolite of imidacloprid (6-chloro-nicotinic acid) as well. That study reports 10-day mortality at the Germany II location: control 10%; 0.004 ng/bee/d 67%; 0.038 ng/bee/day 77%; 0.38 ng/bee/day 97%. Other locations did not show such high sensitivity to the insecticide. It is worth considering secondary stressors that could result in a higher sensitivity to the toxin. Suspects would be bacterial or viral pathogens or the Nosema microsporidian26, or even synergistic interaction with other insecticides27.

Compelling new research by Di Prisco, et al. found that very low doses of both the neonicotinoids clothianidin and imidacloprid caused immune suppression in honeybees as evidenced by replication of the deformed wing virus (DWV)28. They provided evidence that the neonicotinoids were downgrading the innate immunity pathway governed by NF-κB. Replication of the virus was found to increase with both dose and time.

Pathogen interactions with a host could lead to several time-dependent processes. First, damage to the host immune system has to occur for a pathogen to get a foothold. Then, before the host succumbs to the pathogen, the infectious organism must grow and multiply to lethal levels. Damage from the pathogen itself may take time to manifest in the host organism. Several of these time-dependent processes, initiated by pesticide exposure, could produce a high order time dependency like that observed by Suchail et al15.

Addressing this question, the interaction between Nosema and imidacloprid was studied by Alaux et al.29. The study fed 200,000 Nosema spores to bees to initiate infection and then subjected the bees to various doses of imidacloprid. There was some interaction between the pesticide and the pathogen, but the study only followed the bees for ten days, so it is hard to draw conclusions at field-realistic imidacloprid doses, as the Nosema infection dominated the experiment at this short time scale. Nevertheless, two data points from that study are plotted in Figure 2 (crosses) where estimates could be made of the LT50 time with the addition of imidacloprid, using the Nosema infected bees as the control. However, imidacloprid-alone concentrations from 0.7 to 70 ppb in that study resulted at day 10 in only about 7% to 12% mortality above the control, not sufficient to render an LT50 estimate.

Poisoning by ingestion of toxin in larvae

When imidacloprid is fed to developing larvae, the dose-time dependency is markedly different. Stingless Melipona bee workers will succumb to the toxin's effects between day 11 and day 19 of their development for a toxic dose range spanning more than two orders of magnitude: from 14 to 2800 ng/bee, as shown in Figure 314.

Figure 3
figure 3

Time-dependent toxicity for larval stingless bees fed imidacloprid (adapted from Tomé et al.14).

After the larval stage, the survival probability for the individuals making it to the pupae stage improves dramatically. At the lowest dose tested, 5.6 ng imidacloprid a.i. in 130 μl of larva food before sealing the cell, more than half of the bees survive well into adulthood, yet a third of that cohort died between day 11 and pupation at day 23. In this case the authors observed an almost dose-independent effect that caused mortality during a specific period of larval development. The time dependence for this section of the toxicity curve is t13! (r2 = 0.80, p < 0.001). This might also be (at least partially) due to the changing physiology of the developing insect, rather than to the cumulative effect. Moreover, the food uptake increases in the growing larva, thus increasing the exposure.

It is worth noting that had the authors followed the mortality of the larva for just ten days, as is common with adult honeybee toxicity studies, they would have missed the main feature of delayed toxicity which started at day 11. Similarly, mortality of honeybees in the Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16 study started to occur about day 19 of exposure in the 4 ppb concentration (Figure 1).

The greater survivability once the bees reach the pupae stage can be explained by two factors. First, the pupae are no longer consuming the toxin-laced larval food supply, so the internal concentration of toxicant would plateau or even decrease. More importantly, the insects continue to develop neurologically. A growth spurt of synaptic dendrites is known to occur at metamorphic transitions30 which would allow repair and recovery from previous damage in the surviving cohort. This mechanism may mean that larval sublethal exposure is only relevant for that stage of development, but largely irrelevant for insects that are further exposed as adults.

Poisoning of termites in imidacloprid-treated soils

The previous studies refer to oral ingestion of imidacloprid. Moreover, time-to-death data for Reticulitermes termites exposed by contact to several imidacloprid-treated soils reveal a similar toxicity pattern (Figure 4). In this case, the power law model renders exponent values that range from 1.46 to 1.62 for loam and sandy-loam soils, respectively, to 2.6 in sand soil and 2.9 in clay-loam soils (r2 range 0.73 to 0.94, p range 0.005 to 0.045). The authors of that study13 explain that the different adsorption properties of each soil tested can account for the different exposure of the termites, hence the variable patterns of chronic toxicity. Besides the lethal endpoint, they also report significant feeding inhibition for termites in the imidacloprid-laced soils, possibly a contributor to the lethal effect, as observed also with bumble bees31. Although wide-ranging in absolute toxicity depending on the soil type, the time dependence of the toxic effects is similar to the other adult insects we have looked at above, with time exponents near two.

Figure 4
figure 4

Toxicity of imidacloprid-treated soil to termites (data from Ramakrishnan et al.13).

A Toxicity Model for neonicotinoids

Imidacloprid and all other neonicotinoids are designed to bind to the nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs), which are located on the dendritic terminal membrane at the synapses of the insect nervous system6. When the neonicotinoid molecule binds to the nAChR the ion channel opens, as it would if it was stimulated normally by the acetylcholine (ACh) neurotransmitter; in other words, neonicotinoids are agonists that compete with the natural neurotransmitters. The neonicotinoid molecule remains bound to the nAChR in insects, holding the channel open, whereas normally ACh remains bound for only about a millisecond32 before dissociating from the receptor and being removed from the synaptic junction by the enzyme acetylcholinesterase (AChE). In mammals and other vertebrates, the lesser affinity of neonicotinoids for their nAChRs appears to be related to the different configuration of the subunits that make up this receptor, so the toxin binding is weak and/or does not last as long as in insects6.

A toxicokinetic-toxicodynamic model (TKTD) to explain the time-cumulative toxicity of imidacloprid shown above is proposed here. The model is also applicable for all neonicotinoids. The toxicokinetic portion of our model consists of a rate equation that describes accumulation of bound toxin on the synapses. We take the toxic exposure dose, D(t), to be either a single initial dose or a continuous, much lower, chronic dose. The change in total-body toxic load, C, can be written

where τM is the metabolic decay time. Neurotoxins produce an effect only when bound to specific receptors, so the toxicodynamic part of the model relies on the relative velocities of binding and dissociation to and from the receptor. Therefore, the amount of bound toxin, CB, will depend upon the total toxic load and the time constants for receptor binding, τA and dissociation, τD.

We assume here that there are plenty of binding sites and that the binding rate does not decrease because a large fraction of the sites become filled. However, one would expect that a very small percentage of the ion channels bound with toxin and held open continuously would be damaging to the organism; by way of analogy, it is like having a power switch turned on for a long time, squandering energy unnecessarily. Desensitisation, endocytosis or neuronal adaptation to the bound neonicotinoid could also occur, which we do not account for explicitly in our model other than through the empirically determined time constants.

It is less clear how the amount of bound neurotoxin produces toxic effect over time, even if there is ample evidence of it occurring33. An explanation of the mechanisms leading from sublethal effects to delayed lethal effects over time needs to consider the specific mode of action of the toxin22. A likely candidate is the phenomenon of excitotoxicity, where prolonged and excessive stimulation of nerve cells leads to cell death from a variety of mechanisms34. For example, continuous muscle stimulation by the affected nerve can lead to death of the muscle cell. Excessive stimulation of the synapse can lead to Ca++ imbalance and demise of the nerve cell itself. Both of these mechanisms are the result of the accumulation of insults to the affected cells from synaptic stimulation over time. We can express the damaging biological effect, E(t), as

We interpret a measured toxic endpoint to be where E(t) reaches an arbitrary threshold, e.g. E(t) = 1.0 corresponds to LT50.

To apply the model, we need estimates of kinetic time constants τD, τM and τA. Estimates for τM and τA are obtained from the radiocarbon metabolic analysis done by Suchail et al.35. Although the parent compound is quickly metabolized and excreted with a lifetime of about 5 hours, several toxic metabolites remain with a much slower apparent decay time35. Approximately 10% of the initial dose is still found in the insects after 72 hours as metabolites. This implies that the ratio τMA ≈ 0.1. An accurate estimate of τD was not possible, because the bees were only followed for 72 hours in Suchail et al.33 experiments, not long enough to establish the decay rate of the bound component. For irreversible binding, τD would approach infinity.

An example of this simple model is shown in Figure 5 for chronic exposure of honeybees to imidacloprid at environmentally relevant oral doses of 0.16 ng/day. The model is scaled so that at the observed LT50 of honeybees, the model's biological damaging effect, E(t), is approximately equal to one. In the chronic case, the total body toxic load remains at about 20% of the daily dose because the toxin is being continuously ingested and rapidly metabolized. However, the bound toxin that is trapped on synapses grows linearly, while the biological damage increases with t2 and this outcome of the model matches very well the mortality observed in terrestrial insects (see Figures 2 and 4).

Figure 5
figure 5

Model results for chronic exposure of honeybees to imidacloprid at 0.16 ng/day.

Unbound total-body toxic load (solid line) is a small fraction of the daily intake because most of the toxin is metabolized while the amount of bound toxin (short dashes) grows linearly with time. Relative biological damage grows as t2. The model is scaled such that biological damage is unity at the point the LT50 is reached for honeybees.

Further considerations of the model utility are discussed in the Supplementary Information (SI).

Discussion

At present, regulatory guidelines primarily assess the survival of adult honeybees after a short exposure to pesticides, acutely up to 4 days and with chronic caged bee studies up to 10 days36. However, some authors have stressed the importance of also testing for longer chronic toxicity, larval toxicity and sublethal effects of pesticides37. Without explicit long term tests, we are left to estimate long term effects by extending the toxicity scaling that we find in shorter duration studies. We can extrapolate the curve in Figure 2, beyond the Dechaume-Moncharmont et al.16 data points with LT50 at about 30 days, to the lifespan of wintering bees at 150 days38. The extrapolation suggests that daily ingestion of about 0.005 ng/day of imidacloprid would produce LT50 in 150 days. For bees consuming 0.02 g honey per day4, this implies a concentration of 0.25 ppb in honey as the lowest concentration capable of causing long-term mortality. Such a low concentration is about the detectable limit of this insecticide by LC/MS and below its limit of quantitation24. Hence, even with healthy bees, exposure to modest field-realistic residues of imidacloprid in pollen (range 0.5–30 ppb) and honey (range 0.7–13 ppb) could easily cause problems for summer bees and especially for longer-lived bees going through the winter. Certainly, not all pollen or honey are contaminated with imidacloprid, so the oral exposure of bees would be scaled down by a certain factor, since the average frequency of detection in pollen is between 9% and 40% for the US and France, respectively24,25,39.

Extrapolation of the toxicity curve should not be considered definitive, but rather cautionary. The toxicity scaling observed up to 30 days may not continue, as there is uncertainty in the actual decay rate of bound toxin molecules. Our model results suggest that decay times as short as 10 days are consistent with the observed data (See Figure S2). It is also likely that there are differences in the rate of food consumption and physiological differences between summer and winter bees that could affect the mortality rate40,41.

Colony health may not suffer significantly due to mortality of older bees as long as the colony remains otherwise healthy with a productive queen that is able to maintain the colony population. It is well known that colonies can compensate for the losses of worker bees. Indeed, full colony field studies on neonicotinoid treated crops show not much difference in performance between the insecticide treated and non-treated colonies41,42. Also, a field study investigating imidacloprid-treated maize found the level of contamination in stored honey was between 0.05 ppb and 0.5 ppb and yet mortality rates in apiaries were inversely correlated with the surface of maize fields treated but not with imidacloprid43. Additionally, risk analysis studies and reviews of neonicotinoid use have downplayed the importance of the neonicotinoids on honeybee losses, especially as the sole agent5,17,44. However, the explicit connection between innate immunity loss and the neonicotinoids28, was not known or considered in these studies. The potential for accumulation of neonicotinoids at receptor sites to the level where loss of innate immunity has been demonstrated may be much more likely for sublethal exposure to these chemicals than direct poisoning. Furthermore, it must be recognized that worker losses do occur and declining homing rates can be accurately measured using radio frequency identification (RFID) tags45,46,47.

From the time-to-effect studies we have examined, in all cases it appears that imidacloprid lethal effects accumulate in the insects. Observations by many authors show that the longer the exposure time, the less amount of total chemical needed to kill the insects; in other words, the LD50s decrease with exposure time, as shown in Figures 2 to 4. Whether it is enhanced, cumulative, or delayed toxicity, all these terms describe this situation when the power law scaling exponent is greater than one. One explanation is that the damage at cellular/tissue level might occur immediately after the first administration of the toxin, even if no permanent binding is observed. However, this damage might produce no immediate visible effect – mortality of the insect will occur later on. Thus there might be acute poisoning, with no mortality, during the initial phase of the experiment and its delayed lethal effect would increase the exponent value even if no toxin accumulation occurs.

Insecticides with delayed toxicity pose a particularly vexing problem when attempting to protect beneficial insects. Separation of residual toxin in time and space between target insect pests and pollinators becomes increasingly important as the order of time dependence increases. For example, consider a case where application of the pesticide is designed to kill target insects within 2 days. Assume that the pollinator lifespan is 50 days and we wish to not harm the pollinator. Further, assume that the 48 hour LD50 is the same for the target insect and pollinator (since they are both insects). The usual approach to protecting pollinators is to apply insecticide when bees are not present so the full force of the insecticide can act upon the target pest while only a residual amount will remain in the crop when bees encounter the pesticide. Table 1 shows the level of pesticide contamination compared to the application rate required to protect a long-lived pollinator and the safety factors that need to be applied.

Table 1 Maximum residual insecticide contamination allowed to protect a long-lived pollinator (e.g. life-span 50 days) compared to the application rate when the insect pest is targeted in 2 days (Ratio pollinator/pest = 50/2). Assumes identical sensitivity to the toxin for both target and non-target insects. Relative safety factors (x 3) required to protect the beneficial pollinator are indicated for each case

Compounds that exhibit enhanced toxicity require much more care for the toxin to be well-eliminated from the pollinator's environment. Systemic insecticides pose a particular risk in this regard, because once the toxin has been taken up in the plant, it can remain there for an extended period, including plant bloom, so their residues translocate to pollen and nectar48,49. Achieving the protection ratio required for pollinator safety may be impossible for long-lived compounds. Current regulatory standards emphasize a toxic threshold that does not include any time dependence. Utilizing a simple toxic threshold to define safe residual levels based on acute LD50s, which works for many pesticides, has led to complacency when faced with compounds that exhibit enhanced toxicity, such as imidacloprid and related chemicals. Compounds with a t2 toxicity scaling require protection levels hundreds of time more stringent than those with merely a static lethal threshold (Table 1), because the eco-toxicological consequences of their delayed toxicity are potentially far-reaching. Many invertebrate species have long-lived stages of development in which delayed toxicity of residual pesticide could directly affect the organisms.

We examined studies that addressed bee mortality after exposure to imidacloprid. Bees thus affected may not immediately die, but they may be practically useless to the colony, as they may be unable to forage or navigate11,50,51,52 and hence become lost and perish outside the hive. Naturally, while some bees are lost, others are produced to replace them, so the population in the colony is compensated, making it difficult to estimate the actual mortality rates. Quantifying behavioral effects is more difficult than counting dead bees in a hive and may require the use of sophisticated equipment to track down each individual bee53. We would expect that time-to-effect scaling for behavioral effects would be similar to the mortality data, but would merely show up sooner. Extrapolating from individual bee mortality or neurological damage to whole-colony health remains an important unresolved question. Neurological damage can effect foraging success and hence colony nutrition54, or grooming behavior, learning and olfactory sensory abilities50,55, which are important colony-wide responses to parasites and pathogens.

It is generally accepted that multiple pathogens ultimately bring down stressed colonies56,57. Cornman et al.57 found that Colony Collapse Disorder (CCD) colonies were more likely to have higher levels of a wide variety of pathogens than weak, but non-CCD, colonies. Not only were the levels of pathogens higher, but multiple agents were frequently found in combinations not typical of non-CCD colonies. It appears that the immune system in the CCD colonies has gone awry and some authors hypothesize this is the case58. Could it be a few parts per billion of insecticide that makes the difference? The Di Prisco study showed both imidacloprid and clothianidin adversely affect insect immunity and promote replication of a viral pathogen, DWV, in honey bees at exposure levels 1 ppb or less with exposure of one to three days28. More research is still needed here, perhaps looking at pesticide interaction with KBV or AMPV virus, since these viruses show up in CCD colonies.

Aufauvre et al.59 looked at the interaction of the systemic insecticide fipronil and Nosema infection and found a strong synergy between the pesticide and the pathogen, similar to what happened with imidacloprid29. Bees with either Nosema alone or fipronil alone suffered mortality similar to the control, since low doses of toxin and Nosema spores were used. However, given enough time, the combination was especially lethal. The study followed the bees for 22 days, the added time crucial for the delayed toxic and synergistic effects to show up.

The choice of Nosema as the pathogen for pesticide-pathogen interaction studies is convenient because the infection is easily accomplished with a spore solution and the progress of the infection can be followed with microscopic examination of the bees26. Viruses are much more difficult to use as the infectious agent because of the difficulty of diagnosing their presence and quantifying the infectious dose and infection progress. However, viruses are more ubiquitous in honeybee colonies than Nosema and could easily have a similar synergistic interaction with pesticides on the bee's health.

A final point to consider is the effect of imidacloprid on queen bees. Queen bees can live for several years. Queen bees also need to consume large amounts of food (e.g. honey and royal jelly) in order to churn out the quantity of eggs they lay every day. This makes queen bees especially susceptible to a cumulative toxin in the food supply. Queen failure was one of the precursors to colony mortality found by vanEngelsdorp et al.60. Equally, colonies of bumble bees (Bombus spp.) exposed to imidacloprid suffered an 85% reduction in production of new queens compared to control colonies61. The situation is aggravated when the bees are exposed to not just imidacloprid alone but two or more insecticides27, as it occurs in the real world. Queen failure, even if the bees succeed in raising a new one, will leave the colony without a fresh supply of young bees. A colony with pesticide stress may be relying on young bees to make up for those that disappear before their time.

Conclusions

Accurate time-to-effect scaling is an important tool for estimating the effect of chronic pesticide exposure to honeybees. In order to achieve this, chronic tests for pesticide toxicity to pollinators should be extended to 30 days or more and use time-to-effect measurements to clearly establish scaling. This is because in some experiments there were no deaths in either the control or exposed bees during the first 10 days16, yet the toxic effect was clearly evident and different from the controls by day 30. Similarly, with stingless bee larva, the growing larvae required at least 11 days before the toxic effects of imidacloprid became apparent. The model shown here reproduces well the observations made by many authors and explains the delayed and time-cumulative toxicity of imidacloprid based on its mode of action. Toxins that have enhanced time-dependent chronic toxicity, such as imidacloprid and other neonicotinoids, need to be well-characterized to avoid accidental poisoning of beneficial organisms. It follows that compounds with delayed toxicity should be avoided as pesticides because of the intrinsic difficulty they pose as environmental contaminants.