Introduction

Amajor hypothesis about information processing is that information is processed in a contingent sequence involving sensory processing, stimulus identification and response selection, preparation and execution stages or processes1. Therefore, any theory of cognitive plasticity must provide an explanation of how information is transmitted between these processing stages1,2,3,4. This study examines how information transmission occurs during learning-induced plasticity in brain cognitive systems, a much debated but largely unresolved issue in cognitive science5.

By examining the transmission speed of various attributes contained by a stimulus, research has explored the information transmission pattern between processing stages6. Even a stimulus as simple as a line-drawn triangle contains multiple attributes (e.g., shape, size, color). Under certain circumstances, some attributes could be transmitted to the next stage before the processing of others. Partial information transmission has been studied in a series of event-related potential (ERP) experiments using both visual and auditory channel information as partial information to be transmitted7,8,9. Research using reaction-time task paradigms with simple visual or sound attributes has led to two competing theories of information transmission. The first theory maintains that information transmission occurs in a synchronous pattern whereby processing is completed at each stage before progressing to the next1, while the second proposes an asynchronous transmission pattern, where information is processed simultaneously in different stages6,10 (see11 for a review). Arguably, an asynchronous pattern of information transmission should allow faster initial response times, but lead to more errors due to lack of temporal congruence between stimulus components, whereas a synchronous pattern might initially produce slower response times but lead to greater accuracy by maintaining temporal congruence12.

Research, however, also revealed plasticity in the information transmission. De Sanctis and Sommer found that the information transmission pattern could be influenced by task requirements under experimental conditions13. For example, an asynchronous information transmission pattern might temporarily switch to a synchronous one (or vice versa) when the accuracy of reactions (or the speed of reaction) was required. In addition, the information transmission pattern is also related to the perceptual discriminability of the attributes of a stimulus9. The highly discriminable stimulus attribute tended to be transmitted earlier as partial information in both visual6 and auditory channels7,8. However, these studies only revealed a strategic effect, which merely brought a temporary trade-off performance under certain experimental conditions. It is also important to investigate how learning induces plasticity in information processing pattern and how learning-induced plasticity speaks to information processing theories.

This study used lateralized readiness potential (LRP), an established electrophysiological measure of information processing in the brain from the motor cortex, in the context of a “go”, “no-go” reaction-time task14. The LRP is derived by subtracting the potential in the correct response hand hemisphere from that in the other hemisphere15. Where the LRP component associated with stimulus processing and response preparation (S-LRP16) is absent in “no-go” trials and that during response execution (LRP-R16) is maintained, this indicates full processing has occurred prior to response execution and a synchronous pattern of information transmission. LRP-R can also be used to exclude movement complexity reduction induced by irrelevant factors17. If the S-LRP-no-go remains following learning, this indicates that stimulus processing is still ongoing during the response preparation phase and an asynchronous pattern of information transmission. Previously we have shown that when two sound attributes with different perceptual discriminability are used, asynchronous information transmission occurs and the performance is not error-free. Here we have investigated if this transmission pattern is maintained where both error-free performance and fast response times are required. We hypothesized that extensive training to achieve this outcome would switch an asynchronous into a synchronous transmission pattern, thereby helping reconcile the two competing theories.

Results

Behavioral data

Behavioral results showed that for subjects receiving training on the auditory task, accuracy rate was sigificantly improved and by the third session was virtually error free (Fig. 1b, F = 22.51, p = .0002). Reaction times were also improved (Fig. 1d, F = 7.51, p = .003). Furthermore, the expected advantage of pitch over intensity discrimination observed initially (test 1: t = 8.66, p = .000003) also disappeared by the final test (test 3: t = 1.89, p = .085), showing that training had significantly reduced the difference in processing efficiency between them. By contrast, in the control group there were no significant improvements in performance across the three tests (Fig. 1b,c,d). More details see Table S1,S2,S3.

Figure 1
figure 1

Experimental tasks and behavior results.

(a) Experimental protocol, (b) Mean ± Std.error accuracy, (c) pitch vs intensity sensitivity and (d) response times on the go/no-go task for experimental and control groups. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01 test 3 vs. test 1.

Event related potentials data

Analyses of evoked potentials revealed no significant differences in latencies or amplitudes of N1 and P2 components (indices of sensory processing18) or the LRP-R (response execution7) across the three tests in either group. Thus, learning had no effect on sensory or response execution processing (Figs. 2a,b and Figs. 3a,b). Hackley and Miller (1995) suggested that complex movements induced greater amplitude of S-LRP than simple movements17. In this study, LRP-R served as an index to rule out the possibility that the disappearance of S-LRP-no-go in test 3 was simply due to the reduction in movement complexity caused by the training. Actually, in the experimental group the S-LRP latency was significantly reduced across the three tests (Fig. 2c, Fgo = 258.94, Fno-go = 180.8, ps < .000001), suggesting improved stimulus identification and response preparation processing leading to reduced response times. The mean amplitude of S-LRP in test 1 was significantly larger for “no-go” trials than the baseline (t = 5.62, p = .00015) and smaller than “go” trials (t = 4.32, p = .001), confirming asynchronous information transmission processing6,8,9. However, the S-LRP amplitude on “no-go” trials decreased at test 2 (t = 6.16, p = .00007) and disappeared at test 3 (t = 1.64, p = .13), revealing a switch into a synchronous processing pattern. No S-LRP changes occurred in the control group (Fig. 3c). Furthermore, by comparing participants' performance in test 1 and test 3, we found that the reduced amplitude of S-LRP for no-go trials was positively correlated with improved accuracy (r = 0.62, p = 0.03) and reduced response time (r = 0.79, p = 0.002) supporting its relationship to task performance in the experimental group (Fig. 4). More details see Table S1,S2,S3.

Figure 2
figure 2

Averaged evoked potentials from the experimental group showing (a) N1, P2; (b) LRP-R and (c) S-LRP latencies and amplitudes across tests.

Figure 3
figure 3

Averaged evoked potentials from control group showing (a) N1, P2; (b) LRP-R and (c) S-LRP latencies and amplitudes across tests.

Figure 4
figure 4

Reduced amplitude of S-LRP for no-go trials was positively correlated with improved accuracy and reduced response time, supporting its relation to task performance.

Discussion

From our results we propose a model summarizing how learning in this task changes an asynchronous into a synchronous pattern of information processing by improving the learners' ability to discriminate certain attributes (Fig. 5). The model proposes that pitch and intensity initially take different amounts of time to process due to variation in discrimination difficulty9. Pitch information is processed more quickly and transmitted to the response selection and preparation stage before intensity is fully processed, resulting in a large mean amplitude of S-LRP-no-go and an asynchronous transmission pattern (Test 1 in Fig. 5). As learning achieves better performance, processing time for intensity shortens disproportionately compared to pitch, resulting in a reduced mean amplitude of S-LRP-no-go and asynchronization (Test 2 in Fig. 5). Then, as learning achieves error-free performance, processing time for intensity and pitch become equivalent and the S-LRP-no-go disappears indicating the end of continuously reduced asynchronization and the transmission pattern has turned into a synchronous one (Test 3 in Fig. 5). For simple tasks where stimulus components have equivalent discriminability then minimal learning is required and a synchronous processing pattern will be employed from the outset.

Figure 5
figure 5

Model showing how training reduces processing times for intensity and pitch until they are equivalent and the S-LRP dissappears, indicating a switch from an asynchronous to a synchronous pattern of transmission.

S: Stimulus onset; RT: Reaction time. (This figure is adapted from Miller's figure 1 in 19926.)

In summary, our results help to reconcile the two conflicting theories of information processing in brain cognitive systems by demonstrating that both can occur in the same task as a function of learning. We suggest that an asynchronous processing pattern is initially employed where stimulus components in tasks have varying discriminability and processing times. The objective of cognitive plasticity during learning is to produce equivalent processing of stimulus components allowing a synchronous processing pattern to be used so rapid, error free performance is achieved.

Methods

Participants

There were 24 healthy adult subjects (12 female, mean = 24 yrs, range = 21 to 26 yrs), none of whom had received formal auditory training and who were randomly assigned to either experimental (n = 12) or control (n = 12) groups. Two subjects from the control group did not complete the training course and were therefore excluded. All subjects had normal hearing and normal or corrected-to-normal vision. They were reimbursed ¥200. The study was done with approval from the University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Ethics Board. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Stimuli and apparatus

Four pure-tone stimuli (1000 Hz vs. 2000 Hz; 55 dB vs. 59 dB) were generated by using Adobe Audition 3.0 and rechecked by means of the PRAAT speech editing software. Tones of 375 msec in duration were binaurally presented via headphones. The sound pressure level was double-checked by applying a digital sound level meter.

Design and procedure

The stimuli were presented in a random order in eight blocks, each consisting of 90 trials in the experimental task. On each trial, subjects were presented with a stimulus which was either a louder (59 dB) or softer (55 dB) 1000 Hz or 2000 Hz pure tone. The pitch of the tone indicated which hand to use for the response, while its intensity indicated whether a response should be performed “go” or not “no-go” (see Fig. 1a). The pitch (hand) and intensity (go/no-go) assignments were counter-balanced across subjects. The likelihood for “go” and “no-go trials” was 0.67 and 0.33 respectively. While an even likelihood of 0.5 is also effective, a higher probability for “go” trials tends to produce more consistent data19.

Subjects were seated in a dimly lit, sound attenuated and electrically shielded room and were instructed to look at a fixation cross throughout the trials. Five hundred milliseconds before the task stimulus was presented, the fixation cross would flash to signal the upcoming critical tone, which then lasted for 375 msec. Subjects were instructed to respond quickly and accurately in the “go” trials by pressing a sequence of three keys (C, Z and X keys in a standard key-board for left-hand responses; the comma, slash and period keys for right-hand responses) with the index, ring and middle fingers. This design is based on the finding that the robustness of LRPs is related to the complexity of the action required by a task17. For “no-go” trials, subjects were told to avoid making any response.

Daily training took place in a sound-attenuated laboratory. Subjects from the experimental group were trained individually for 2 h daily over a period of one month using the same intensity and pitch-based reaction time task. Subjects from the control group performed a completely unrelated task for 2 h daily (typing words on a computer keyboard as fast and as accurately as possible).

Electrophysiological (EEG) recording and data analysis

The EEG data were measured with an electrode cap of 64 Ag-AgCl electrodes connected according to an extended 10–20 system and digitized with a sampling rate of 500 Hz (Brain Products GmbH). The impedance for all electrodes was kept below 5 kΩ and all the data were band-pass filtered (0.01–100 Hz) online. All channels were recorded with frontal vertex (i.e., FCz) as the reference and re-referenced to “infinity” zero provided by the reference electrode standardization technique (REST) off-line20. AFz served as the ground electrode during recording. To control for eye movement artifacts, horizontal and vertical electrooculograms (EOGs) were recorded21.

Off-line EEG analysis was performed according to a standard procedure using a Brain Vision Analyzer Version 2.0.1.We computed the ERP data without offline filtering in accordance with previous recommendations22.

The N1 and P2 components of auditory evoked potentials were evaluated in terms of the average peak latency and amplitude from three middle-line electrodes (Fz, Pz, Cz). The lateralized readiness potential (LRP) was measured with the difference between the electrodes over the primary motor cortices, contra- and ipsilateral to the responding hand (i.e., C3–C4 and C4–C3 for the right- and left-hand responses, respectively). The onset latency of the stimulus-locked LRP (S-LRP) and response-locked LRP (LRP-R) were made using a Jack-Knife procedures and the mean amplitudes were computed with a 100 ms time window after the onset latency23.

The d′ (perceptual sensitivity) is calculated according to the hit and false alarm rates8. To calculate d′, one level of each stimulus dimension was arbitrarily defined as the signal and the other as noise. Specifically, for pitch analyses the pitch associated with left-hand reactions was defined as the signal and for right-hand reactions as noise. For intensity analyses, the intensity associated with the “go” response was defined as the signal and that with the “no-go” reaction as noise. Hit and false alarm rates for the pitch dimension and intensity were computed respectively and converted into d′6.

Based on the standard experimental procedure and data analysis methods6, we calculated hit and false alarm rates in go trials for the pitch attribute and in both go and no-go trials for the intensity attribute. For pitch analyses, a hit was defined as a left-hand reaction to a stimulus cueing a left-hand response, regardless of whether the intensity of the tone indicated a go or a no-go response. If the subject responded with his or her left hand to the other pitch, it would constitute a false alarm. For intensity analyses, a hit was defined as a go response to an assumed go stimulus regardless of pitch, whereas a go response to any no-go stimulus constituted a false alarm.” Hit and false alarm rates for the pitch and the intensity attributes were computed respectively and converted into d′.