Skip to main content

Tapping Your Inner Rain Man

A blow to the head can sometimes unmask hidden artistic or intellectual gifts

A 10-year-old boy, Orlando Serrell, knocked unconscious one day by a baseball, discovered afterward that he could bring to mind the exact day of the week for any date after the accident and could remember the weather for each day since the trauma as well. He could also recall the most minute daily events.

Jason Padgett, the victim of a brutal mugging in 2002 that left him with a severe concussion, soon afterward began to see what he describes simply as “images.” He began to sketch them out on paper. When he showed his work to others, he learned that the repeating, self-similar patterns were fractals.

These two people have a remarkable condition known as acquired savant syndrome. In the more familiar savantism—made famous by the 1988 movie Rain Man—people are endowed from early in life with extraordinary but narrowly defined musical, artistic, mathematical, memory and mechanical skills that stand in contrast to their marked impairments in language, social interactions and other mental faculties overall. In Rain Man, for instance, actor Dustin Hoffman's character, Raymond Babbitt, had stunning mathematical, calendar-calculating and other skills and a massive memory but also showed severe cognitive and behavioral limitations from his underlying autism.


On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing. By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.


In acquired savant syndrome, in contrast, near-genius levels of artistic or intellectual skills show up after dementia, a severe blow to the head or another insult to the brain. Discovery of this unusual phenomenon raises the possibility that dormant potential in some artistic or intellectual realm—an “inner savant”—resides in each of us. If so, perhaps a way can be found to tap these buried abilities in the absence of disease or injury.

Your Inner Sculptor
I have studied savant syndrome for much of my career. Until the mid-1980s, I assumed it was present from birth—in other words, savant syndrome was a congenital condition. But then I attended the premier exhibition of exceptional sculptures made by Alonzo Clemons. As an infant, Clemons seemed to learn quickly. At about age three, however, a fall resulted in a brain injury, slowing his cognitive development precipitously and leaving him with a serious intellectual disability, including limited vocabulary and speech. But afterward, he developed a spectacular skill for sculpting using whatever materials were handy—even shortening from the kitchen. With his new talent came a growing fascination with animals. For example, he could look at a photograph of a horse in a magazine and then sculpt a three-dimensional replica in less than half an hour, each muscle and tendon reproduced in exacting detail.

Clemons sparked my interest in acquired savant syndrome, and I looked for reports of it in the medical literature but found only a few instances. In 1923 psychologist Blanche M. Minogue described the appearance of extraordinary musical abilities in a three-year-old following a bout of meningitis. In 1980 T. L. Brink, another psychologist, reported on a nine-year-old boy in whom superior mechanical skills appeared after a bullet wound to his left brain. In his case, he was able to dismantle, reassemble and modify multigear bicycles and invented a punching bag that could simulate the bobbing and weaving of a live opponent.

These sparse reports in the years before 1980 reflected the condition's rarity: a concussion or stroke does not usually enhance cognitive or creative capacity. I then decided to collect descriptions of such cases. By 2010 I had assembled a worldwide registry of 319 known savants, of whom only 32 had the acquired form.

Among the reports entered in my registry was work done by neurologist Bruce Miller, now at the University of California, San Francisco, and his colleagues. In 1996 Miller began compiling the first of 12 cases of people who had a disorder known as frontotemporal dementia (FTD). These elderly patients demonstrated musical or artistic skills for the first time, sometimes at prodigious levels, after their diagnosis. Frontotemporal dementia differs from Alzheimer's dementia in that the degenerative process affects only the frontal lobes and not wider areas of the brain.

FTD often targets the left anterior temporal area of the brain and the orbitofrontal cortex. Both regions normally inhibit activity in the visual system at the back of the brain, which is involved in processing incoming signals from the eyes. The disease seems to foster a newfound artistic sensibility by turning off inhibitory signals from the front of the brain. The releasing of the brakes allows the brain to process sight and sound in new ways. It unleashes artistic or other creative sensibilities even though damage to the frontal lobes may lead to the inappropriate behaviors that characterize the disorder. “FTD is an unexpected window into the artistic process,” Miller says.

Further work implies that accidental genius results from diminished activity in some brain areas that is combined with a counterbalancing intensification in others. More specifically, it involves a set of events I call the three R's that occur after the brain is damaged—most often after the left hemisphere is stricken, similar to what happened with Miller's FTD cases. The process begins with “recruitment,” a rise in electrical activity in still intact cortical tissue, often in the right hemisphere. Then the brain circuitry undergoes “rewiring” to establish newly formed connections between regions that were not previously linked. Next comes “release” of dormant capacity as a result of increased access to the newly connected brain areas.

An experiment done by Richard Chi and Allan Snyder, both then at the Center for the Mind at the University of Sydney, has used a relatively new technology to provide some evidence that these brain changes account for the acquired savant skills. Using transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS), these researchers induced savantlike abilities in human volunteers. The technique generates a polarized electric current to diminish activity in a part of the left hemisphere involved with sensory input, memory, language and other brain processes while increasing activity in the right hemisphere (the right anterior temporal lobe).

The investigators then asked study participants to solve the challenging “nine-dot” puzzle either with or without tDCS—a task that requires the creativity to search for a solution in an unconventional way. Participants had to connect three rows of three dots using four straight lines without lifting a pen or retracing lines. None of them could solve it before stimulation, and when 29 subjects were exposed to “sham” stimulation—electrodes emplaced without any current to test for placebo effects—they were still at a loss. With the current switched on, however, some 40 percent—14 of 33 participants—worked their way through the puzzle successfully.

How can a person suddenly perform so much better at the flip of a switch? Because these instant savants—and congenital and acquired savants as well—“know things” innately they were never taught. Clemons, the sculptor, had no formal training in art but knew instinctively how to produce an armature, the frame for the sculpture, to enable his pieces to show horses in motion.

One plausible explanation for the hidden talents that emerge in savant syndrome—whether early in life or induced by injury—is that these reservoirs of skill and knowledge must be inherited in some way. We do not start life with a blank slate that subsequently gets inscribed through education and other life experiences. The brain may come loaded with a set of innate predispositions for processing what it sees or for understanding the “rules” of music, art or mathematics. Savants can tap into that inherited knowledge far better than the average person can.

The Genius Switch
Knowing that these talents can emerge even later in life raises the question of whether everyone has the capacity to become a savant—and whether it might be possible to do so without facing the travails of brain injury or dementia.

Another way to unleash buried brilliance would be to apply tDCS—or a related technology called repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation—also a form of “thinking cap” that would toggle brain regions on and off to potentially augment a person's creative capacity. A technological solution may not be an absolute prerequisite, however. Meditation or simple adherence to assiduous practice of an artistic skill may suffice to allow us to switch on the more creative right side of the brain and thus explore undiscovered artistic capabilities.

As investigators understand the brain better, they may find other ways of determining what happens when brain circuits are turned up or down. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and diffusion tensor tracking (DTT), which pinpoint connections among neurons (“fiber tracking”), are better suited than earlier methods for revealing the intricacies of the wiring inside a person's head, enabling researchers to correlate brain activity to the sudden appearance of skills. These more precise technologies can provide three-dimensional images of the fibers that tie together brain cells.

One challenge to uncovering the neurobiology underlying savantism has been the difficulty of observing brains as they carry out creative tasks that require movement. Not only is it hard to sculpt or play a piano inside an MRI machine, but any movement compromises image acuity. A newer technique—near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS)—would sidestep such problems by replacing bulky machines with a comfortable skullcap that measures the amount of oxygen in blood flowing through the brain's blood vessels and relays the information to image-processing software. Even more promising is a recently developed helmet that uses another imaging method—positron-emission tomography (PET)—for monitoring when a person is sitting, standing or even exercising.

Such studies are worth pursuing. Acquired savantism provides strong evidence that a deep well of brain potential resides within us all. The challenge now is to find the best ways to tap into our inner savant—that little bit of Rain Man—while keeping the rest of our mental faculties intact.

MORE TO EXPLORE

Savant-Like Skills Exposed in Normal People by Suppressing the Left Fronto-Temporal Lobe. Allan W. Snyder et al. in Journal of Integrated Neuroscience, Vol. 2, No. 2, pages 149–158; December 2003.

Islands of Genius: The Bountiful Mind of the Autistic, Acquired, and Sudden Savant. Darold A. Treffert. Jessica Kingsley Publishers, 2010.

Struck by Genius: How a Brain Injury Made Me a Mathematical Marvel. Jason Padgett and Maureen Seaberg. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2014.

FROM OUR ARCHIVES

Islands of Genius. Darold A. Treffert and Gregory L. Wallace; June 2002.

SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN ONLINE Watch Treffert discuss savant syndrome at ScientificAmerican.com/aug2014/savantism

Darold A. Treffert, a psychiatrist, met his first savant in 1962 and continues research on savant syndrome at the Treffert Center in Fond du Lac, Wis. He was a consultant for the 1988 movie Rain Man and maintains a Web page at www.savantsyndrome.com, hosted by the Wisconsin Medical Society.

More by Darold A. Treffert
Scientific American Magazine Vol 311 Issue 2This article was originally published with the title “Accidental Genius” in Scientific American Magazine Vol. 311 No. 2 (), p. 52
doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0814-52