Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing

Key Points

  • Temporal information is crucial for goal reaching, neuroeconomics, and the survival of humans and other animals, and requires multiple biological mechanisms to track time over multiple scales. In mammals, the circadian clock is located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Another timer, which is responsible for automatic motor control in the millisecond range, relies on the cerebellum. Finally, a general-purpose, flexible, cognitively-controlled timer that operates in the seconds-to-hours range involves the activation thalamo-cortico-striatal circuits.

  • The hallmark of interval timing is that the error in estimating a duration is proportional to the duration to be timed, a property known as scalar timing. Scalar timing resembles Weber's law, which applies to most sensory modalities.

  • The way that time is perceived, represented and estimated has traditionally been explained using a pacemaker–accumulator model, which is not only straightforward but also surprisingly powerful in explaining behavioural and biological data. Pharmacological studies support a dissociation of the clock stage, which is affected by dopaminergic manipulations, and the memory stage, which is affected by cholinergic manipulations.

  • Despite explaining many findings, the relevance of the pacemaker–accumulator model to the brain mechanisms that are involved in interval timing is unclear. New models will require investigation of recent neurobiological evidence.

  • An impaired ability to process time is found in patients with disorders of the dopamine system, such as Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease and schizophrenia. By contrast, the failure of a neurological disorder — such as cerebellar injury — to affect interval timing is taken to indicate that the affected structures are not essential for temporal processing in the seconds-to-hours range.

  • Because interval timing depends on the intact striatum, but not on the intact cerebellum, the cerebellum is usually charged with millisecond timing and the basal ganglia with interval timing. Recent findings suggest that separate timing circuits can be dissociated when continuity, motor demands and attentional set are manipulated.

  • The basal ganglia, prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex are activated in both interval-timing tasks, and tasks that require integration of somatosensory signals or quantity/number processing. Electrophysiological data are consistent with the involvement of these structures in number, sequence or magnitude representation as well as in interval timing, thereby supporting a mode-control model of counting and timing in which number and time are processed by the same neural circuits.

  • Functional MRI shows that two clusters of foci are activated during millisecond and interval timing tasks. The 'automatic timing' cluster is activated by tasks that require repetitive movements and involve short timing intervals, and includes the supplementary motor area and primary somatosensory cortex. The 'cognitively controlled timing' cluster is activated when the durations are longer and the amount of movement required is limited, and includes the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, intraparietal sulcus and premotor cortex. The basal ganglia and the cerebellum are not specific to either cluster.

  • The striatal beat-frequency model describes interval timing as an emergent activity in the thalamo-cortico-striatal loops. In this model, timing is based on the coincidental activation of medium spiny neurons in the basal ganglia by cortical neural oscillators. The activity of the striatal neurons increases before the expected time of reward, and peaks at the criterion interval. The model demonstrates the scalar property, and incorporates features that would allow the integration of a number of lines of evidence into one vision of interval timing in the brain.

Abstract

Time is a fundamental dimension of life. It is crucial for decisions about quantity, speed of movement and rate of return, as well as for motor control in walking, speech, playing or appreciating music, and participating in sports. Traditionally, the way in which time is perceived, represented and estimated has been explained using a pacemaker–accumulator model that is not only straightforward, but also surprisingly powerful in explaining behavioural and biological data. However, recent advances have challenged this traditional view. It is now proposed that the brain represents time in a distributed manner and tells the time by detecting the coincidental activation of different neural populations.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Timing across different timescales.
Figure 2: The scalar property is a hallmark of interval timing at both the behavioural and neural levels.
Figure 3: The pacemaker–accumulator model and dopaminergic and cholinergic synapses.
Figure 4: Interval timing in patients with Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease and cerebellar lesions.
Figure 5: Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of thalamo-cortico-striatal circuits in the representation of time and numerosity.
Figure 6: Differential activation of the circuits involved in the processing of time and colour.
Figure 7: The striatal beat-frequency model.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Grothe, B. New roles for synaptic inhibition in sound localization. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 540–550 (2003).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Richelle, M. & Lejeune, H. Time in Animal Behavior (Pergamon, New York, 1980).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Gallistel, C. R. The Organization of Behavior (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1990).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Meck, W. H. (ed.) Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing (CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Pastor, M. & Artieda, J. (eds) Time, Internal Clocks and Movement (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Bradshaw, C. & Szabadi, E. (eds) Time and Behaviour: Psychological and Neurobehavioral Analyses (Elsevier, London, 1997).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Fraisse, P. Psychologie du Temps (P. U. F., Paris, France, 1957).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Gibbon, J., Church, R. M. & Meck, W. H. in Timing and Time Perception Vol. 423 (eds Gibbon, J. & Allan, L. G.) 52–77 (The New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Treisman, M. Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the 'internal clock'. Psychol. Monogr. 77, 1–31 (1963).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Gibbon, J. & Allan, L. G. Timing and Time Perception (The New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984). A classic collection of papers relating to the scalar expectancy theory and other aspects of timing and time perception in humans and other animals.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Gibbon, J., Malapani, C., Dale, C. L. & Gallistel, C. R. Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 170–184 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Meck, W. H. Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 227–242 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Czeisler, C. A. et al. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science 284, 2177–2181 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Kacelnik, A. Timing and foraging: Gibbon's scalar expectancy theory and optimal patch exploitation. Learn. Motiv. 33, 177–195 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  15. Sohn, M. & Carlson, R. Implicit temporal tuning of working memory strategy during cognitive skill acquisition. Am. J. Psychol. 116, 239–256 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Bateson, M. & Kacelnik, A. Starling's preferences for predictable and unpredictable delays to food. Anim. Behav. 53, 1129–1142 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Ohyama, T., Gibbon, J., Deich, J. D. & Balsam, P. D. Temporal control during maintenance and extinction of conditioned keypecking in ring doves. Anim. Learn. Behav. 27, 89–98 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  18. Buhusi, C. V., Sasaki, A. & Meck, W. H. Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). J. Comp. Psychol. 116, 381–390 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Drew, M. R., Zupan, B., Cooke, A., Couvillon, P. A. & Balsam, P. D. Temporal control of conditioned responding in goldfish. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 31, 31–39 (2005)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Roberts, S. & Church, R. M. Control of an internal clock. J. Exp. Psych. Anim. Behav. Process. 4, 318–337 (1978).

    Google Scholar 

  21. Buhusi, C. V., Perera, D. & Meck, W. H. Memory for timing visual and auditory signals in albino and pigmented rats. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 31, 18–30 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gallistel, C. R., King, A. & McDonald, R. Sources of variability and systematic error in mouse timing behavior. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 30, 3–16 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Gribova, A., Donchin, O., Bergman, H., Vaadia, E. & Cardoso de Oliveira, S. Timing of bimanual movements in human and non-human primates in relation to neuronal activity in primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area. Exp. Brain Res. 146, 322–335 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Brannon, E. M., Roussel, L. W., Meck, W. H. & Woldorff, M. Timing in the baby brain. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 227–233 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  25. Rakitin, B. C. et al. Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 24, 15–33 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Penney, T. B., Gibbon, J. & Meck, W. H. Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 1770–1787 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Edwards, C. J., Alder, T. B. & Rose, G. J. Auditory midbrain neurons that count. Nature Neurosci. 5, 934–936 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Schirmer, A. Timing speech: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 269–287 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  29. Mauk, M. D. & Buonomano, D. V. The neural basis of temporal processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 307–340 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Shaffer, H. in Timing and Time Perception Vol. 423 (eds Gibbon, J. & Allan, L.) 420–428 (The New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984).

    Google Scholar 

  31. Gibbon, J., Morrell, M. & Silver, R. Two kinds of timing in circadian incubation rhythm of ring doves. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 247, R1083–R1087 (1984).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Hinton, S. C. & Meck, W. H. The 'internal clocks' of circadian and interval timing. Endeavour 21, 82 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Reppert, S. M. & Weaver, D. R. Coordination of circadian timing in mammals. Nature 418, 935–941 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Levine, J. D., Funes, P., Dowse, H. B. & Hall, J. C. Resetting the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 298, 2010–2012 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Darlington, T. K. et al. Closing the circadian loop: clock-induced transcription of its own inhibitors per and tim. Science 280, 1599–1603 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Lewis, P. A., Miall, R. C., Daan, S. & Kacelnik, A. Interval timing in mice does not rely upon the circadian pacemaker. Neurosci. Lett. 348, 131–134 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Malapani, C., Dubois, B., Rancurel, G. & Gibbon, J. Cerebellar dysfunctions of temporal processing in the seconds range in humans. Neuroreport 9, 3907–3912 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Harrington, D. L., Lee, R. R., Boyd, L. A., Rapcsak, S. Z. & Knight, R. T. Does the representation of time depend on the cerebellum? Effect of cerebellar stroke. Brain 127, 561–574 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Spencer, R. M. C., Zelaznik, H. N., Diedrichsen, J. & Ivry, R. B. Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science 300, 1437–1439 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Jueptner, M. & Weiller, C. A review of differences between basal ganglia and cerebellar control of movements as revealed by functional imaging studies. Brain 121, 1437–1449 (1998).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Shadmehr, R. & Holcomb, H. H. Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science 277, 821–825 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Roberts, S. Isolation of an internal clock. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 7, 242–268 (1981).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Gibbon, J. Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychol. Rev. 84, 279–325 (1977). This seminal paper introduced the influential scalar expectancy theory.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Weber, E. H. Annotationes Anatomicae et Physiologicae (Anatomical and Physiological Observations) (C. F. Kohler, Lipsiae (Leipzig), Germany, 1851).

    Google Scholar 

  45. Meck, W. H. & Malapani, C. Neuroimaging of interval timing. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 133–137 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Hinton, S. C. in Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing (ed. Meck, W. H.) 419–438 (CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  47. Hinton, S. C. & Meck, W. H. Frontal-striatal circuitry activated by human peak-interval timing in the supra-seconds range. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 171–182 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  48. François, M. Contributions à l'étude du sens du temps: la température interne comme facteur de variation de l'appréciation subjective des durées. Année Psychol. 27, 186–204 (1927).

    Google Scholar 

  49. Woodrow, H. The reproduction of temporal intervals. J. Exp. Psychol. 13, 473–499 (1930).

    Google Scholar 

  50. Hoagland, H. The psychological control of judgements of duration: evidence for a chemical clock. J. Gen. Psychol. 9, 267–287 (1933).

    Google Scholar 

  51. Meck, W. H. Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 9, 171–201 (1983).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Maricq, A. V. & Church, R. M. The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 79, 10–15 (1983).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Meck, W. H. Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 25, 1185–1189 (1986).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Matell, M. S., King, G. R. & Meck, W. H. Differential modulation of clock speed by the administration of intermittent versus continuous cocaine. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 150–156 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Rammsayer, T. H. On dopaminergic modulation of temporal information processing. Biol. Psychol. 36, 209–222 (1993).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Meck, W. H. Choline uptake in the frontal cortex is proportional to the absolute error of a temporal memory translation constant in mature and aged rats. Learn. Motiv. 33, 88–104 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  57. Malapani, C. et al. Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson's disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 316–331 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Holson, R. R., Bowyer, J. F., Clausing, P. & Gough, B. Methamphetamine-stimulated striatal dopamine release declines rapidly over time following microdialysis probe insertion. Brain. Res. 739, 301–307 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Buhusi, C. V. & Meck, W. H. Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 291–297 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Plenz, D. & Kital, S. T. A basal ganglia pacemaker formed by the subthalamic nucleus and external globus pallidus. Nature 400, 677–682 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Church, R. M. & Broadbent, H. A. Alternative representations of time, number, and rate. Cognition 37, 55–81 (1990).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Miall, R. C. The storage of time intervals using oscillating neurons. Neur. Comp. 1, 359–371 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  63. Grossberg, S. & Schmajuk, N. A. Neural dynamics of adaptive timing and temporal discrimination during associative learning. Neural Netw. 2, 79–102 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  64. Buhusi, C. V. & Schmajuk, N. A. Timing in simple conditioning and occasion setting: a neural network approach. Behav. Process. 45, 33–57 (1999).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Machens, C. K., Romo, R. & Brody, C. D. Flexible control of mutual inhibition: a neural model of two-interval discrimination. Science 307, 1121–1124 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Dragoi, V., Staddon, J. E., Palmer, R. G. & Buhusi, C. V. Interval timing as an emergent learning property. Psychol. Rev. 110, 126–144 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Killeen, P. R. & Fetterman, J. G. A behavioral theory of timing. Psychol. Rev. 95, 274–295 (1988).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Fraisse, P. Perception and estimation of time. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 35, 1–37 (1984).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Lewis, P. A. & Miall, R. C. in Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing (ed. Meck, W. H.) 515–532 (CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, USA, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  70. Malapani, C., Deweer, B. & Gibbon, J. Separating storage from retrieval dysfunction of temporal memory in Parkinson's disease. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 311–322 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Paulsen, J. S. et al. fMRI biomarker of early neuronal dysfunction in presymptomatic Huntington's disease. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 25, 1715–1721 (2004). Proposes that the dysfunction of interval timing and the hyperactivation of SMA are early markers of Huntington's disease.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Rammsayer, T. Temporal discrimination in schizophrenic and affective disorders: evidence for a dopamine-dependent internal clock. Int. J. Neurosci. 53, 111–120 (1990).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Tracy, J. I. et al. Information-processing characteristics of explicit time estimation by patients with schizophrenia and normal controls. Percept. Mot. Skills 86, 515–526 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Volz, H. P. et al. Time estimation in schizophrenia: an fMRI study at adjusted levels of difficulty. Neuroreport 12, 313–316 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Elvevag, B., Brown, G. D. A., McCormack, T., Vousden, J. I. & Goldberg, T. E. Identification of tone duration, line length, and letter position: an experimental approach to timing and working memory deficits in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 113, 509–521 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Penney, T. B., Meck, W. H., Roberts, S. A., Gibbon, J. & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. Interval-timing deficits in individuals at high risk for schizophrenia. Brain Cogn. 58, 109–118 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C. & Kawato, M. Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 338–347 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Ivry, R. B. & Spencer, R. M. The neural representation of time. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 225–232 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Koekkoek, S. K. E. et al. Cerebellar LTD and learning-dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses. Science 301, 1736–1739 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Rammsayer, T. H. & Brandler, S. Aspects of temporal information processing: a dimensional analysis. Psychol. Res. 69, 115–123 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Rammsayer, T. H. Neuropharmacological evidence for different timing mechanisms in humans. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 52, 273–286 (1999).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  82. Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R. & Pouthas, V. Processes involved in tempo perception: a CNV analysis. Psychophysiology 40, 69–76 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Meck, W. H. Neuropsychology of timing and time perception. Brain Cogn. 58, 1–8 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Matell, M. S. & Meck, W. H. Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. Bioessays 22, 94–103 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Matell, M. S. & Meck, W. H. Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 139–170 (2004). Develops the biological assumptions of the SBF model of interval timing, and shows simulations using this model.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Meck, W. H. & Benson, A. M. Dissecting the brain's internal clock: how frontal-striatal circuitry keeps time and shifts attention. Brain Cogn. 48, 195–211 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Ravizza, S. M. & Ivry, R. B. Comparison of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in shifting attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 285–297 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Ivry, R. B., Spencer, R. M., Zelaznik, H. N. & Diedrichsen, J. The cerebellum and event timing. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 978, 302–317 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Lewis, P. A. & Miall, R. C. Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: evidence from neuroimaging. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 250–255 (2003). An original review of the brain areas that are activated in neuroimaging studies of interval timing as a function of the characteristics of the timing task.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Macar, F. et al. Activation of the supplementary motor area and of attentional networks during temporal processing. Exp. Brain Res. 142, 475–485 (2002). An excellent review of neuroimaging studies of interval timing that points out the importance of adequate control tasks.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Nieder, A. & Miller, E. K. Coding of cognitive magnitude: compressed scaling of numerical information in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron 37, 149–157 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Meck, W. H. & Church, R. M. A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 9, 320–334 (1983). Describes similarities between timing and counting in preverbal animals.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S. & Spelke, E. Core systems of number. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 307–314 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Walsh, V. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 483–488 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Brannon, E. M. & Roitman, J. D. in Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing (ed. Meck, W. H.) 143–182 (CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, USA, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  96. Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Buchel, C. & Nobre, A. C. Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia 38, 808–819 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Sawamura, H., Shima, K. & Tanji, J. Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. Nature 415, 918–922 (2002). Shows changes in the firing of PPC neurons during a sequence of actions.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Nieder, A. & Miller, E. K. A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical information in the monkey. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 7457–7462 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J. & Miller, E. K. Representation of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 297, 1708–1711 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Shuman, M. & Kanwisher, N. Numerical magnitude in the human parietal lobe: tests of representational generality and domain specificity. Neuron 44, 557–569 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Matell, M. S., Meck, W. H. & Nicolelis, M. A. Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. Behav. Neurosci. 117, 760–773 (2003). Dissociates motor coding from time coding in striatal neurons.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Fiorillo, C. D., Tobler, P. N. & Schultz, W. Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science 299, 1898–1902 (2003). Shows that in tasks that involve uncertainty, dopaminergic neurons burst at trial onset and at the expected time of reward, and show sustained activation throughout the trial.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Apicella, P., Scarnati, E., Ljungberg, T. & Schultz, W. Neuronal activity in monkey striatum related to the expectation of predictable environmental events. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 945–960 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Schultz, W., Apicella, P., Scarnati, E. & Ljungberg, T. Neuronal activity in monkey ventral striatum related to the expectation of reward. J. Neurosci. 12, 4595–4610 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Olton, D. S., Wenk, G. L., Church, R. M. & Meck, W. H. Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by simultaneous temporal processing. Neuropsychologia 26, 307–318 (1988).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Meck, W. H., Church, R. M., Wenk, G. L. & Olton, D. S. Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory. J. Neurosci. 7, 3505–3511 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Pang, K. C., Yoder, R. M. & Olton, D. S. Neurons in the lateral agranular frontal cortex have divided attention correlates in a simultaneous temporal processing task. Neuroscience 103, 615–628 (2001). A key study showing that cortical neurons are activated by simultaneous temporal processing.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Coull, J. T. & Nobre, A. C. Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. J. Neurosci. 18, 7426–7435 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Rubia, K. et al. Prefrontal involvement in 'temporal bridging' and timing movement. Neuropsychologia 36, 1283–1293 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Ferrandez, A. M. et al. Basal ganglia and supplementary motor area subtend duration perception: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 19, 1532–1544 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Macar, F., Vidal, F. & Casini, L. The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes. Exp. Brain Res. 125, 271–280 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Vidal, F., Bonnet, M. & Macar, F. Programming the duration of a motor sequence: role of the primary and supplementary motor areas in man. Exp. Brain Res. 106, 339–350 (1995).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Meck, W. H. & N'Diaye, K. Un modèle neurobiologique de la perception et de l'estimation du temps. Psychologie Francaise 50, 47–63 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  114. Lustig, C., Matell, M. S. & Meck, W. H. Not 'just' a coincidence: frontal-striatal interactions in working memory and interval timing. Memory 13, 441–448 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Salinas, E. & Sejnowski, T. J. Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2, 539–550 (2001).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  116. Silva, L. R., Amitai, Y. & Connors, B. W. Intrinsic oscillations of neocortex generated by layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Science 251, 432–435 (1991).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Galarreta, M. & Hestrin, S. Spike transmission and synchrony detection in networks of GABAergic interneurons. Science 292, 2295–2299 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Riehle, A., Grun, S., Diesmann, M. & Aertsen, A. Spike synchronization and rate modulation differentially involved in motor cortical function. Science 278, 1950–1953 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Steinmetz, P. N. et al. Attention modulates synchronized neuronal firing in primate somatosensory cortex. Nature 404, 187–190 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E. & Desimone, R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560–1563 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Beiser, D. G. & Houk, J. C. Model of cortical-basal ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events. Clin. Neurophysiol. 79, 3168–3188 (1998).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  122. Houk, J. C. in Models of Information Processing in the Basal Ganglia (eds Houk, J. C., Davis, J. L. & Beiser, D. G.) 3–10 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  123. Charpier, S. & Deniau, J. M. In vivo activity-dependent plasticity at cortico-striatal connections: evidence for physiological long-term potentiation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94, 7036–7040 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Gubellini, P., Pisani, A., Centonze, D., Bernardi, G. & Calabresi, P. Metabotropic glutamate receptors and striatal synaptic plasticity: implications for neurological diseases. Prog. Neurobiol. 74, 271–300 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Schultz, W. Neural coding of basic reward terms of animal learning theory, game theory, microeconomics and behavioural ecology. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 139–147 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Schultz, W. Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1, 199–207 (2000).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  127. van Rossum, M. C. W. A novel spike distance. Neural Comput. 13, 751–763 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Glimcher, P. W. Decisions, Uncertainty, and the Brain: The Science of Neuroeconomics (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA, 2004).

    Google Scholar 

  129. Meck, W. H. Interval timing and genomics: what makes mutant mice tick? Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 14, 211–231 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  130. Aschoff, J. in Timing and Time Perception Vol. 423 (eds Gibbon, J. & Allan, L. G.) 442–468 (The New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984).

    Google Scholar 

  131. Rousseau, R., Poirier, J. & Lemyre, L. Duration discrimination of empty time intervals marked by intermodal pulses. Percept. Psychophys. 34, 541–548 (1983).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Rammsayer, T. H. & Vogel, W. H. Pharmacologic properties of the internal clock underlying time perception in humans. Neuropsychobiology 26, 71–80 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y. & Hermanowicz, N. Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology 12, 3–12 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  134. Nagarajan, S. S., Blake, D. T., Wright, B. A., Byl, N. & Merzenich, M. M. Practice-related improvements in somatosensory interval discrimination are temporally specific but generalize across skin location, hemisphere, and modality. J. Neurosci. 18, 1559–1570 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  135. Karmarkar, U. R. & Buonomano, D. V. Temporal specificity of perceptual learning in an auditory discrimination task. Learn. Mem. 10, 141–147 (2003).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  136. Lejeune, H. & Wearden, J. H. The comparative psychology of fixed-interval responding: some quantitative analyses. Learn. Motiv. 22, 84–111 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  137. Church, R. M. & Gibbon, J. Temporal generalization. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 8, 165–186 (1982).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  138. Gibbon, J. in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (ed. Bower, G.) 105–135 (Academic, New York, 1986).

    Google Scholar 

  139. Gibbon, J., Fairhurst, S. & Goldberg, B. in Time and Behaviour: Psychological and Neurobehavioral Analyses Vol. 120 (eds Bradshaw, C. & Szabadi, E.) 329–374 (Elsevier, London, 1997).

    Google Scholar 

  140. Wearden, J. H., Denovan, L., Fakhri, M. & Haworth, R. Scalar timing in temporal generalization in humans with longer stimulus durations. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 23, 502–511 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  141. Crystal, J. Circadian time perception. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 27, 68–78 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  142. Malapani, C. & Rakitin, B. in Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing (ed. Meck, W. H.) 485–513 (CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  143. Coull, J. T., Vidal, F., Nazarian, B. & Macar, F. Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science 303, 1506–1508 (2004). A state-of-the-art neuroimaging study using a dynamic control task and parametric variation of the interval-timing procedure.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank M. Matell for providing data for figures 5 and 7. This work was supported, in part, by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health to C.V.B. and a James McKeen Cattell award to W.H.M.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Warren H. Meck.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Related links

Related links

DATABASES

OMIM

Huntington's disease

Parkinson's disease

FURTHER INFORMATION

The Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy Time

Glossary

GLOBAL POSITIONING SYSTEM

(GPS). A network of artificial satellite transmitters that provide highly accurate position fixes for Earth-based, portable receivers.

COINCIDENCE DETECTION

The activation of neurons not by single inputs, but by the simultaneous activity of several inputs. For example, coincidental activation or inactivation of specific dendritic inputs might trigger a neuron to fire, thereby transforming a time code into a rate code. Similarly, in the binaural auditory system, coincidental activation that results from hearing a sound with a specific interaural time difference is used to transform a time code into a spatial code.

INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCE

The difference in the time of arrival of a sound wave at an animal's two ears. It ranges from 100 μs in gerbils to about 650 μs in humans and is one of the sources of information used by various species to make a topographic representation of space.

INTERVAL TIMING

Perception, estimation and discrimination of durations in the range of seconds-to-minutes-to-hours.

CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS

Repetition of certain phenomena in living organisms at about the same time each day. The most thought of circadian rhythm is sleep, but other examples include body temperature, blood pressure, and the production of hormones and digestive secretions.

MILLISECOND TIMING

Perception, estimation and discrimination of durations in the sub-second range.

WEBER'S LAW

Formulated by Ernst Weber in 1831 to explain the relationship between the physical intensity of a stimulus and the sensory experience that it causes. Weber's Law states that the increase in a stimulus needed to produce a just-noticeable difference is constant. Later, Gustav Fechner (1801–1887) generalized Weber's law by proposing that sensation increases as the logarithm of stimulus intensity: S = k logI, where S = subjective experience, I = physical intensity, and k = constant.

FEEDBACK

To signal the end of the to-be-timed duration to the participant, a feedback signal is presented. In experiments involving animals, the feedback is usually an appetitive stimulus (for example, food) or aversive stimulus (for example, footshock). In experiments that involve human participants, the feedback may take various forms, including verbal reward, gaining 'points', and so on.

TEMPORAL MEMORY TRANSLATION CONSTANT

A parameter in the scalar expectancy theory that is responsible for producing scalar transforms of sensory input taken from an internal clock and stored in temporal memory. It is used to explain systematic discrepancies in the accuracy of temporal memory.

NEURAL OSCILLATOR

Repetitive, periodical activation of a neuron. The intrinsic mechanisms that control the period of the oscillator (the interval between two neuronal spikes) range from fast ion currents (for example, 40 Hz oscillations in sparsely spiny neurons in the frontal cortex) to slow transcriptional feedback loops (for example, 24-h oscillation in the SCN).

ATTENTIONAL SET

Set of to-be-attended features that are primed for use in a specific task, such that participants would be more likely to attend to the features in the attentional set than to other features of the task.

MOTOR SET

Sets of to-be-activated motor programs that are primed for use in a specific task, such that participants would be more likely to respond using one of the motor programs in the motor set than using other responses.

DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASKS

Presentation of a stimulus is followed by a delay, after which a choice is offered and the originally presented stimulus must be chosen. With small stimulus sets, the stimuli are frequently repeated, and therefore become highly familiar. So, typically, such tasks are most readily solved by short-term or working memory rather than by long-term memory mechanisms.

LONG-TERM POTENTIATION

(LTP). An enduring increase in the amplitude of excitatory postsynaptic potentials as a result of high-frequency (tetanic) stimulation of afferent pathways. It is measured both as the amplitude of excitatory postsynaptic potentials and as the magnitude of the postsynaptic-cell population spike. LTP is most frequently studied in the hippocampus and is often considered to be the cellular basis of learning and memory in vertebrates.

LONG-TERM DEPRESSION

(LTD). An enduring weakening of synaptic strength that is thought to interact with LTP in the cellular mechanisms of learning and memory in structures such as the hippocampus and cerebellum. Unlike LTP, which is produced by brief high-frequency stimulation, LTD can be produced by long-term, low-frequency stimulation.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Buhusi, C., Meck, W. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6, 755–765 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1764

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1764

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing