Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

Cognitive neuroscience

Flutter Discrimination: neural codes, perception, memory and decision making

Key Points

  • Frequency discrimination in the sense of flutter is a useful model to understand how neural codes are related to perception, working memory and decision making. The task can be thought of as a series of steps: encoding a stimulus frequency, maintaining it in memory, encoding a second frequency, comparing it to the frequency in memory, and communicating the result of the comparison to the motor system. Neurophysiological studies have explored the brain regions that participate in each of these steps and how these regions interact to solve the task.

  • Encoding. How is the flutter frequency represented in the nervous system? Does this neural code reflect behavioural responses? Changes in neuronal firing rate as a function of stimulus frequency are evident in several areas (particularly in the primary somatosensory cortex) during the flutter discrimination task. Moreover, several lines of evidence (particularly microstimulation experiments) indicate that these rate variations affect behaviour. By contrast, the high periodicity that flutter elicits does not seem to contribute to frequency discrimination.

  • Memory. The clearest neural correlate of working memory during frequency discrimination is found in the prefrontal cortex, which contains neurons that increase their activity in a frequency-dependent manner during the delay period between the two flutter stimuli. This activity does not seem to be related to the impending motor response. The prefrontal cortex might not be the only structure in which such a mnemonic correlate exists, as neurons with similar activity have been found in the secondary somatosensory cortex and in the medial premotor cortex.

  • Comparison process and decision making. The comparison between the two stimulus frequencies can be simply conceptualized as the difference between them. The firing of some neurons in the secondary somatosensory cortex shows dynamic changes as a function of both frequencies, and evolves to encode the difference between them. Moreover, the firing patterns of these neurons are a good indicator of the actual behavioural response, indicating that this neural activity might be involved in the decision-making process. Similar, but significantly different, dynamic changes have been found in the prefrontal and medial premotor cortices.

  • There is a large overlap between sensory-, mnemonic- and decision-related activity during frequency discrimination. As a result, the comparison between stored and ongoing sensory information seems to take place in a distributed manner, and no single area can be identified as the unique site of decision making. We therefore propose that the motor plan that is established to respond after a discrimination trial already contains two possible outcomes, and that sensory information helps in the selection of one of them. Future studies should rigorously explore this possibility.

Abstract

Recent studies combining psychophysical and neurophysiological experiments in behaving monkeys have provided new insights into how several cortical areas integrate efforts to solve a vibrotactile discrimination task. In particular, these studies have addressed how neural codes are related to perception, working memory and decision making in this model. The primary somatosensory cortex drives higher cortical areas where past and current sensory information are combined, such that a comparison of the two evolves into a behavioural decision. These and other observations in visual tasks indicate that decisions emerge from highly-distributed processes in which the details of a scheduled motor plan are gradually specified by sensory information.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Flutter discrimination task.
Figure 2: Comparison between S1 activity and psychophysical performance in the flutter discrimination task.
Figure 3: Neuronal responses evoked by the base stimulus in four brain areas during the flutter discrimination task.
Figure 4: Population dynamics in four cortical areas during the flutter discrimination task.
Figure 5: Psychophysical performance in frequency discrimination with mechanical stimuli delivered to the fingertips, and with electrical stimuli delivered directly to primary somatosensory cortex (S1) neurons.
Figure 6: Differential activity in the medial premotor cortex (MPC).

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Werner, G. & Mountcastle, V. B. Neural activity in mechanoreceptive cutaneous afferents: stimulus–response relations, Weber functions, and information transmission. J. Neurophysiol. 28, 359–397 (1965). The authors characterized how the amplitude of a mechanical indentation delivered to the skin is encoded by primary afferents in anaesthetized monkeys. Along with reference 3, this work on neural codes represents one of the earliest attempts to relate neural activity to human psychophysics in a quantitative way. The use of information theory is impressive, even by today's standards.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Mountcastle, V. B., Talbot, W. H., Darian-Smith, I. & Kornhuber, H. H. Neural basis of the sense of flutter-vibration. Science 155, 597–600 (1967).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Talbot, W. H., Darian-Smith, I., Kornhuber, H. H. & Mountcastle, V. B. The sense of flutter-vibration: comparison of the human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from the monkey hand. J. Neurophysiol. 31, 301–334 (1968). A landmark paper. The authors showed that the sensations produced by low- and high-frequency mechanical vibrations are transmitted by separate sets of afferent fibres. They described the receptive fields of three types of mechanoreceptors and thoroughly characterized their responses as functions of stimulus amplitude and frequency. They compared psychometric detection curves from humans to their neurometric counterparts from primary afferents of anaesthetized monkeys. The idea was to “explain the behavioural capacity of the first in terms of the dynamic encoding properties of the latter.”

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. LaMotte, R. H. & Mountcastle, V. B. The capacities of humans and monkeys to discriminate between vibratory stimuli of different frequency and amplitude: a correlation between neural events and psychological measurements. J. Neurophysiol. 38, 539–559 (1975).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Mountcastle, V. B., Steinmetz, M. A. & Romo, R. Frequency discrimination in the sense of flutter: psychophysical measurements correlated with postcentral events in behaving monkeys. J. Neurosci. 10, 3032–3044 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Romo, R. & Salinas, E. Touch and go: decision mechanisms in somatosensation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 107–137 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Vallbo, A. B. in The Cognitive Neurosciences (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 237–252 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Ochoa, J. & Torebjörk, E. Sensations evoked by intraneural microstimulation of single mechanoreceptor units innervating the human hand. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 42, 633–654 (1983). | PubMed |

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Macefield, G., Gandevia, S. C. & Burke, D. Perceptual responses to microstimulation of single afferents innervating joints, muscles and skin of the human hand. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 429, 113–129 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. DiCarlo, J. J., Johnson, K. O. & Hsiao, S. S. Structure of receptive fields in area 3b of primary somatosensory cortex in the alert monkey. J. Neurosci. 18, 2626–2645 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. DiCarlo, J. J. & Johnson, K. O. Velocity invariance of receptive field structure in somatosensory cortical area 3b of the alert monkey. J. Neurosci. 19, 401–419 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. DiCarlo, J. J. & Johnson, K. O. Spatial and temporal structure of receptive fields in primate somatosensory area 3b: effects of stimulus scanning direction and orientation. J. Neurosci. 20, 495–510 (2000). References 10, 11 and 12 applied modern stimulation and analysis techniques to generate the most thorough description of receptive field properties in the earliest processing stage of S1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Connor, C. E., Hsiao, S. S., Phillips, J. R. & Johnson, K. O. Tactile roughness: neural codes that account for psychophysical magnitude estimates. J. Neurosci. 10, 3823–3836 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Connor, C. E. & Johnson, K. O. Neural coding of tactile texture: comparisons of spatial and temporal mechanisms for roughness perception. J. Neurosci. 12, 3414–3426 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Johnson, K. O., Hsiao, S. S. & Blake, D. T. in Somesthesis and Neurobiology of the Somatosensory System (eds Franzen, O., Johansson, R. S. & Terenius, L.) 213–228 (Birkhaüser, Basel, 1996).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  16. Blake, D. T., Hsiao, S. S. & Johnson, K. O. Neural coding mechanisms in tactile pattern recognition: the relative contributions of slowly and rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors to perceived roughness. J. Neurosci. 17, 7480–7489 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Werner, G. in Medical Physiology. Vol. 1 (ed. Mountcastle, V. B.) 605–628 (Mosby, St Louis, USA, 1980). An excellent review of psychophysical techniques useful for anyone interested in experimental design.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Hernández, H., Salinas, E., García, R. & Romo, R. Discrimination in the sense of flutter: new psychophysical measurements in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 17, 6391–6400 (1997).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Johnson, K. O. Sensory discrimination: neural processes preceding discrimination decision. J. Neurophysiol. 43, 1793–1815 (1980).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Vogels, R. & Orban, G. How well do response changes of striate neurons signal differences in orientation: a study in the discriminating monkey. J. Neurosci. 10, 3543–3558 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Vázquez, P., Cano, M. & Acuña, C. Discrimination of line orientation in humans and monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 2639–2648 (2000).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Romo, R., Brody, C. D., Hernández, A. & Lemus, L. Neuronal correlates of parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Nature 339, 470–473 (1999). This paper describes the clearest neural correlate of working memory in the flutter discrimination task. Prefrontal neurons fire throughout a delay interval during which monkeys need to remember the frequency of a vibrating stimulus. The dependence on stimulus frequency of these responses is similar to that of S2. | PubMed

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Gray, C. M. The temporal correlation hypothesis of visual feature integration: still alive and well. Neuron 24, 31–47 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Shadlen, M. N. & Movshon, J. A. Synchrony unbound: a critical evaluation of the temporal binding hypothesis. Neuron 24, 67–77 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Salinas, E. & Sejnowski, T. J. Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2, 539–550 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Gamzu, E. & Ahissar, E. Importance of temporal cues for tactile spatial-frequency discrimination. J. Neurosci. 21, 7416–7427 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Cascio, C. J. & Sathian, K. Temporal cues contribute to tactile perception of roughness. J. Neurosci. 21, 5289–5296 (2001). References 26 and 27 are meticulous psychophysical studies showing that temporal features have an important role in the perception of textures.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Phillips, J. R., Johansson, R. S. & Johnson, K. O. Responses of human mechanoreceptive afferents to embossed dot arrays scanned across fingerpad skin. J. Neurosci. 12, 827–839 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Johnson, K. O. & Hsiao, S. S. Neural mechanisms of tactual form and texture perception. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 227–250 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Ahissar, E. & Vaadia, E. Oscillatory activity of single units in a somatosensory cortex of an awake monkey and their possible role in texture analysis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 87, 8935–8939 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Ahissar, E. Temporal-code to rate-code conversion by neuronal phase-locked loops. Neural Comput. 10, 597–650 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Mountcastle, V. B., Talbot, W. H., Sakata, H. & Hyvarinen, J. Cortical neuronal mechanisms in flutter vibration studied in unanesthetized monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 452–484 (1969).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Recanzone, G. H., Merzenich, M. M. & Schreiner, C. E. Changes in the distributed temporal response properties of SI cortical neurons reflect improvements in performance on a temporally based tactile discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol. 67, 1071–1091 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Salinas, E., Hernández, H., Zainos, A. & Romo, R. Periodicity and firing rate as candidate neural codes for the frequency of vibrotactile stimuli. J. Neurosci. 20, 5503–5515 (2000). The sensory responses of primary and secondary somatosensory neurons during flutter discrimination are described. Emphasis is on two questions: how is stimulus frequency encoded, and what are the differences between areas? Information theory is used to compare how well stimulus frequency is encoded by changes in mean firing rate and by periodic spiking patterns. Modulations in rate covary with behaviour across trials and seem sufficient to explain the encoding of stimulus frequency.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Romo, R., Hernández, A., Zainos, A., Lemus, L. & Brody, C. D. Neuronal correlates of decision-making in secondary somatosensory cortex. Nature Neurosci. 5, 1217–1225 (2002). Describes the responses of secondary somatosensory neurons during the comparison period in the flutter discrimination task. These neurons combine information about past and current sensory stimuli such that the end product is a signal that corresponds to the output of the discrimination process. So, this activity matches the requirements of the task, rather than just the physical attributes of a stimulus.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Pons, T. P., Garraghty, P. E., Friedman, D. P. & Mishkin, M. Physiological evidence for serial processing in somatosensory cortex. Science 237, 417–420 (1987).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Pons, T. P., Garraghty, P. E. & Mishkin, M. Serial and parallel processing of tactual information in somatosensory cortex of rhesus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 518–527 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Burton, H., Fabri, M. & Alloway, K. Cortical areas within the lateral sulcus connected to cutaneous representations in areas 3b and 1: a revisited interpretation of the second somatosensory area in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 355, 539–562 (1995).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Burton, H., Sathian, K. & Shao, D. H. Altered responses to cutaneous stimuli in the second somatosensory cortex following lesions of the postcentral gyrus in infant and juvenile macaques. J. Comp. Neurol. 291, 395–414 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Zhang, H. Q. et al. Parallel processing in cerebral cortex of the marmoset monkey: effect of reversible SI inactivation on tactile responses in SII. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 3633–3655 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Zhang, H. Q. et al. Functional characteristics of the parallel SI- and SII-projecting neurons of the thalamic ventral posterior nucleus in the marmoset. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1805–1822 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hernández, A., Zainos, A. & Romo, R. Temporal evolution of a decision-making process in medial premotor cortex. Neuron 33, 959–972 (2002). The responses of premotor neurons during flutter discrimination are described. Although some of these units tend to covary most strongly with the motor aspects of the task, this is not the rule. As in S2, there are sensory and mnemonic responses, as well as dynamic combinations of past and current sensory signals.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Sinclair, R. J. & Burton, H. Neuronal activity in the second somatosensory cortex of monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during active touch of gratings. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 331–350 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Jiang, W., Tremblay, F. & Chapman, C. E. Neuronal encoding of texture changes in the primary and the secondary somatosensory cortical areas of monkeys during passive texture discrimination. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 1656–1662 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Sinclair, R. J. & Burton, H. Neuronal activity in the primary somatosensory cortex in monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during active touch of textured surface gratings: responses to groove width, applied force, and velocity of motion. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 153–169 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Leopold, D. A. & Logothetis, N. K. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature 379, 549–553 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Pruett, J. R. Jr, Sinclair, R. J. & Burton, H. Neural correlates for roughness choice in monkey second somatosensory cortex (SII). J. Neurophysiol. 86, 2069–2080 (2001). A nice combination of neurophysiological and psychophysical experiments. The authors varied several stimulus attributes that influences the perception of roughness in monkeys. This generated a pattern of psychophysical responses. The firing rates of neurons in S2 mimicked this pattern and correlated with the monkey's hit and error responses.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Green, D. M. & Swets, J. A. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics (Wiley, New York, 1966).

    Google Scholar 

  49. Britten, K. H., Shadlen, M. N., Newsome, W. T. & Movshon, J. A. The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. J. Neurosci. 12, 4745–4765 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Shadlen, M. N., Britten, K. H., Newsome, W. T. & Movshon, J. A. A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion. J. Neurosci. 16, 1486–1510 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Hernández, A., Zainos, A. & Romo, R. Neuronal correlates of sensory discrimination in the somatosensory cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 6191–6196 (2000). Two kinds of neurometric curves were calculated from the responses of somatosensory neurons to different flutter frequencies: one depended on firing rate modulations, and the other on the periodicity of the firing patterns. These curves were compared to the actual psychometric curves from the monkey's behaviour. Only the neurometric curves that were based on mean firing rate matched the behaviour. Those based on periodicity predicted a much greater accuracy.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Romo, R., Hernández, A., Zainos, A. & Salinas, E. Somatosensory discrimination based on cortical microstimulation. Nature 392, 387–390 (1998). The first study in which microstimulation was used not just to bias a sensory percept, but to generate such percepts throughout a full parameter range. Microinjecting electrical current directly into S1 of behaving monkeys generated artificial sensations. The animals were able to compare artificial (electrical) and tactile sensations as if both were generated by natural, mechanical stimuli.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Romo, R., Hernández, A., Zainos, A., Brody. C. D. & Lemus, L. Sensing without touching: psychophysical performance based on cortical microstimulation. Neuron 26, 273–278 (2000). A follow-up of the previous study (reference 52) in which additional combinations of electrical and natural stimuli were tested. In the most extreme case, the two stimuli to be discriminated were generated artificially by microstimulating S1, and the monkeys were able to compare them, with just a small decline in psychophysical performance.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Salzman, C. D., Murasugi, C. M., Britten, K. H. & Newsome, W. T. Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance. J. Neurosci. 12, 2331–2355 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Britten, K. H. & van Wezel, R. J. Electrical microstimulation of cortical area MST biases heading perception in monkeys. Nature Neurosci. 1, 59–63 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Graziano, M. S., Taylor, C. S. & Moore, T. Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex. Neuron 34, 841–851 (2002). The authors microstimulated the premotor and primary motor cortices of awake monkeys using parameters different (perhaps more realistic) from those used previously. This did not generate simple, short movements, but complex, multi-limb motions, such that each stimulation site led to a specific, final body configuration. This forces a substantial revision of the traditional view of motor areas.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Wickersham, I. & Groh, J. M. Electrically evoking sensory experience. Curr. Biol. 8, R412–R414 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Mountcastle, V. B. Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat's somatic sensory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 20, 408–434 (1957).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Powell, T. P. S. & Mountcastle, V. B. Some aspects of the functional organization of the cortex of the postcentral gyrus of the monkey: a correlation of findings obtained in a single unit analysis with cytoarchitecture. Bull. Johns Hopkins Hosp. 105, 133–162 (1957).

    Google Scholar 

  60. Jones, E. G., Burton, H. & Porter, R. Commisural and cortico-cortical 'columns' in the somatic sensory cortex of primates. Science 190, 572–574 (1975).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Sur, M., Wall, J. T. & Kaas, J. H. Modular distribution of neurons with slowly adapting and rapidly adapting responses in area 3b of somatosensory cortex in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 51, 724–744 (1984).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Fuster, J. M. The Prefrontal Cortex (Raven, New York, 1989).

    Google Scholar 

  63. Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 331–349 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A. & Desimone, R. Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. J. Neurosci. 16, 5154–5167 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. D'Esposito, M., Postle, B. R. & Rypma, B. Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 3–11 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Kojima, S. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Delay-related activity of prefrontal neurons in rhesus monkeys performing delayed response. Brain Res. 248, 43–49 (1982).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Bruce, C. J. & Goldberg, M. E. Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 603–635 (1985).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Quintana, J. & Fuster, J. M. Mnemonic and predictive functions of cortical neurons in a memory task. Neuroreport 3, 721–724 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Asaad, W. F., Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. Neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during associative learning. Neuron 21, 1399–1407 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. D'Esposito, M., Ballard, D., Zarahn, E. & Aguirre, G. K. The role of prefrontal cortex in sensory memory and motor preparation: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 11, 400–408 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Luppino, G. & Rizzolatti, G. The organization of the frontal motor cortex. News Physiol. Sci. 15, 219–224 (2000).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Tanji, J. New concepts of the supplementary motor area. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 782–787 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Jones, E. G. & Powell, T. P. S. Connexions of the somatic sensory cortex of the rhesus monkey. II. Contralateral cortical connexions. Brain 92, 717–730 (1969).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Jones, E. G. & Powell, T. P. S. Connexions of the somatic sensory cortex of the rhesus monkey. I. Ipsilateral cortical connexions. Brain 92, 477–502 (1969).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Luppino, G., Matelli, M., Camarda, R. & Rizzolatti, G. Corticocortical connections of area F3 (SMA-proper) and area F6 (pre-SMA) in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 338, 114–140 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Jones, E. G., Coulter, J. D. & Hendry, S. H. Intracortical connectivity of architectonic fields in the somatic sensory, motor and parietal cortex of monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 181, 291–347 (1978).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Cipolloni, P. B. & Pandya, D. N. Cortical connections of the frontoparietal opercular areas in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 403, 431–458 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Kosslyn, S. M. et al. The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science 284, 167–170 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Super, H., Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. A neural correlate of working memory in the monkey primary visual cortex. Science 293, 120–124 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Zhou, Y. D. & Fuster, J. M. Mnemonic neuronal activity in somatosensory cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 10533–10537 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Zhou, Y. D. & Fuster, J. M. Neuronal activity of somatosensory cortex in a cross-modal (visuo-haptic) memory task. Exp. Brain Res. 116, 551–555 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Harris, J. A., Miniussi, C., Harris, I. M. & Diamond, M. E. Transient storage of a tactile memory trace in primary somatosensory cortex. J. Neurosci. 22, 8720–8725 (2002). This paper presents evidence that activity in S1 has an impact on working memory in a task that is practically identical to the flutter discrimination task discussed in this review. It provides an interesting comparison with the neurophysiological results from awake monkeys.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Salinas, E. Background synaptic activity as a switch between dynamical states in a network. Neural Comput. (in the press).

  85. Dodd, J. V., Krug, K., Cumming, B. G. & Parker, A. J. Perceptually bistable three-dimensional figures evoke high choice probabilities in cortical area MT. J. Neurosci. 21, 4809–4821 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Kim, J. N. & Shadlen, M. N. Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Nature Neurosci. 2, 176–185 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Shadlen, M. N. & Newsome, W. T. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 1916–1936 (2001). One of the most complete studies of neural activity recorded during the discrimination of random-dot visual motion. Neurons in the lateral intraparietal area have oculomotor characteristics, but their activity also depends on sensory information. So, they might reflect the ongoing visual discrimination process.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Horwitz, G. D. & Newsome, W. T. Target selection for saccadic eye movements: direction-selective visual responses in the superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 2527–2542 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Mountcastle, V. B., Atluri, P. & Romo, R. Selective output-discriminative signals in the motor cortex of waking monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 2, 277–294 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Salinas, E. & Romo, R. Conversion of sensory signals into motor commands in primary motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 18, 499–511 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Tanji, J. The supplementary motor area in the cerebral cortex. Neurosci. Res. 19, 251–268 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Picard, N. & Strick, P. L. Medial wall motor areas: a review of their location and functional activation. Cereb. Cortex 6, 342–353 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Geyer, S., Matelli, M., Luppino, G. & Zilles, K. Functional neuroanatomy of the primate isocortical motor system. Anat. Embryol. 202, 443–474 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  94. Shima, K., Mushiake, H., Saito, N. & Tanji, J. Role for cells in the presupplementary motor area in updating motor plans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 8694–8698 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Gold, J. I. & Shadlen, M. N. Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands. Nature 404, 390–394 (2000). Clever microstimulation experiment in which a population of neurons in the frontal eye fields was stimulated at different points in time during the presentation of a visual-motion stimulus. The magnitude of the ocular deviations evoked by microstimulation varied depending on time, therefore reflecting how much sensory information the neurons had accumulated to that point. This indicates that the accumulation of sensory evidence (or of a processed sensory signal) might be equivalent to the degree of motor preparation.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Gold, J. I. & Shadlen, M. N. The influence of behavioral context on the representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands. J. Neurosci. 23, 632–651 (2003). The follow-up to reference 95 showing that the equivalence between motor preparation and the accumulation of sensory evidence occurs only when the motor plan is to some extent predictable.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Shadlen, M. N. & Newsome, W. T. Motion perception: seeing and deciding. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 628–633 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Romo, R., Ruiz, S., Crespo, P., Zainos, A. & Merchant, H. Representation of tactile signals in primate supplementary motor area. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 2690–2694 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Salinas, E. & Romo, R. in Computational Neuroscience: Trends in Research 98 (ed. Bower, J.) 599–604 (Plenum, New York, 1998).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  100. Romo, R., Merchant, H., Zainos, A. & Hernández, A. Categorical perception of somesthetic stimuli: psychophysical measurements correlated with neuronal events in primate medial premotor cortex. Cereb. Cortex 7, 317–326 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Merchant, H., Zainos, A., Hernández, A., Salinas, E. & Romo, R. Functional properties of primate putamen neurons during the categorization of tactile stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 1132–1154 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Horwitz, G. D. & Newsome, W. T. Sensing and categorizing. Curr. Biol. 8, R376–R378 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Platt, M. L. & Glimcher, P. W. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400, 233–238 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Marder, E. & Calabrese, R. L. Principles of rhythmic motor pattern generation. Physiol. Rev. 76, 687–717 (1996). An extremely comprehensive review of the biophysical bases of circuits that can generate repetitive motor activity. The interaction between sensory and motor processes is viewed from a more integrated perspective than in the cortical literature.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Friesen, W. O. & Cang, J. Sensory and central mechanisms control intersegmental coordination. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 678–683 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Esch, T., Mesce, K. A. & Kristan, W. B. Evidence for sequential decision making in the medicinal leech. J. Neurosci. 22, 11045–11054 (2002). A fresh look at the mechanisms of decision making. The authors describe an interneuron in the leech that, when activated, triggers swimming or crawling. The type of behaviour observed depends on sensory information about water level. The authors propose that processes considered to be involved in decision making might be composed of a hierarchy of motor actions, such that a general plan is set at one level and its details are specified sequentially downstream.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  107. Zainos, A., Merchant, H., Hernández, A., Salinas, E. & Romo, R. Role of primary somatic sensory cortex in the categorization of tactile stimuli: effects of lesions. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 357–360 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Seidemann, E., Zohary, U. & Newsome, W. T. Temporal gating of neural signals during performance of a visual discrimination task. Nature 394, 72–75 (1999). PubMed

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Roitman, J. D. & Shadlen, M. N. Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task. J. Neurosci. 22, 9475–9489 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  110. Cook, E. P. & Maunsell, J. H. Dynamics of neuronal responses in macaque MT and VIP during motion detection. Nature Neurosci. 5, 985–994 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Thompson, K. G., Hanes, D. P., Bichot, N. P. & Schall, J. D. Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 4040–4055 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Schall, J. D. & Thompson, K. G. Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 241–259 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Hanes, D. P. & Schall, J. D. Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science 274, 427–430 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Hanes, D. P. & Carpenter, R. H. Countermanding saccades in humans. Vision Res. 39, 2777–2791 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Reddi, B. A. & Carpenter, R. H. The influence of urgency on decision time. Nature Neurosci. 3, 827–830 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Hertz, J., Krogh, A. & Palmer, R. G. Introduction to the Theory of Neural Computation (Addison-Wesley, New York, 1991).

    Google Scholar 

  117. Salinas, E. & Abbott, L. F. A model of multiplicative neural responses in parietal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 11956–11961 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Gold, J. I. & Shadlen, M. N. Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 10–16 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Wang, X. -J. Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron 36, 955–968 (2002). This modelling paper studies a fairly realistic circuit that can receive two input signals and generate one of two possible outputs. Several results are directly comparable to experimental observations made during the discrimination of random-dot visual motion.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Murray, E. A., Bussey, T. J. & Wise, S. P. Role of prefrontal cortex in a network for arbitrary visuomotor mapping. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 114–129 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Olson, C. R. & Gettner, S. N. Macaque SEF neurons encode object-centered directions of eye movements regardless of the visual attributes of instructional cues. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 2340–2346 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Wallis J. D., Anderson, K. C. & Miller E. K. Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature 411, 953–956 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The research of R.R. was partially supported by an International Research Scholars Award from the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, and grants from the Millennium Science Initiative-Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología and Dirección General del Personal Académico-Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. E.S. was supported by startup funds from the Wake Forest University School of Medicine. We appreciate the technical assistance of A. Hernández in preparing the figures.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ranulfo Romo.

Related links

Related links

FURTHER INFORMATION

Encyclopedia of Life Sciences

learning and memory

neural networks and behaviour

somatosensory system

touch

Glossary

DIFFERENCE LIMEN

In flutter discrimination, the difference limen is a measure of how small an increase in the frequency of a vibrotactile stimulus can be detected when compared to a standard stimulus frequency. A smaller difference limen implies a higher discrimination capacity.

WEBER FRACTION

Weber made the observation that, within a fairly large range, the increase in a stimulus that is just noticeable (ΔI) is a constant proportion of the initial stimulus (I) for any one sense. The proportion ΔI/I is the Weber fraction.

PSYCHOMETRIC CURVE

A plot of the percentage of correct behavioural responses as a function of changes in the properties of the test stimulus.

PHASE LOCKING

The preferential firing of neurons at a certain phase of an amplitude-modulated stimulus.

GRATING

An arrangement of parallel bars. The roughness of a surface may be varied by adjusting the width and spacing of the bars of an embossed grating.

NEUROMETRIC CURVE

 A plot of the percentage of correct behavioural responses that an ideal observer would make on the basis of observing the neuronal responses that are elicited by a given test stimulus.

INFORMATION THEORY

Shannon introduced the term 'mutual information' in a strict mathematical sense within a framework for studying communication channels. Mutual information is a statistic that measures the degree of association between any two quantities or sets of quantities. It is useful because it requires no assumptions about their mathematical form or behaviour, so it is in some sense objective.

INTRACORTICAL MICROSTIMULATION

 A neurophysiological technique that is used to activate a population of neurons within a restricted cortical locus. Pulses of electric current delivered through a microelectrode drive the activation.

TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION

(TMS). A non-invasive technique that is based on the application of a time-varying magnetic field near the surface of the head. The magnetic pulse generates electrical currents in the brain that affect the activity of the underlying superficial neurons. Pulses are intense but brief and relatively localized because the magnetic field decreases strongly with distance.

EFFERENCE COPY

A copy of a motor command that is sent back to the central nervous system to inform it of the executed movement.

RANDOM-DOT STIMULUS

A commonly used visual stimulus that consists of dots randomly moving on a screen. The experimenter can vary the coherence of their movement (the fraction of dots that move in the same direction), and the subjects are asked whether they can detect any movement coherence.

SACCADE

A rapid intermittent eye movement that occurs when the eyes fix on one point after another in the visual field.

RECEIVER-OPERATING CHARACTERISTIC INDEX

A measure that allows establishment of the sensitivity and specificity of a given test, enabling us to determine an optimal cut point to distinguish between true and false positives. It is particularly useful when the results of the test are a continuous measure, such as glucose concentration in a blood test.

CENTRAL PATTERN GENERATOR

(CPG). A circuit that produces self-sustaining patterns of behaviour.

REACTION TIME

The period of time between the detection of a stimulus at a sensory receptor and the performance of the appropriate response by the effector organ.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Romo, R., Salinas, E. Flutter Discrimination: neural codes, perception, memory and decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 203–218 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1058

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1058

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing