INTRODUCTION

Interactions among conspecifics constitute an important basis for the organization of life and provide many health benefits to social species. Deficits in social behavior are linked to developmental abnormalities in children, such as autism spectrum disorders (ASD), and can also occur later in life as a sign of major psychiatric illnesses such as schizophrenia or major depression (Crespi and Badcock, 2008). Understanding the mechanisms of social behavior therefore has important implications for mental health.

Several neurotransmitter systems have been causally linked to the expression of social behavior, including those acting via major excitatory and inhibitory ion channel receptors (Coghlan et al, 2012; Yang and Chang, 2014), as well as those acting via G-protein-coupled receptors (Caldu and Dreher, 2007; Kiser et al, 2012). Two classes of Gq-protein-coupled receptors, metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 (mGluR5) and oxytocin receptor (Oxtr), are of particular interest: both have been found to contribute to social, affective, and stress-related behavior (Panksepp, 1992; Carter, 1998; Yap et al, 2005; Koros et al, 2007) and have been implicated as potential treatment targets for ASD symptoms (Insel et al, 1999; Dolen et al, 2010).

Abnormal mGluR5 function has been suggested as a contributing factor to ASD; thus, mGluR5 manipulations has been extensively studied by using systemic pharmacological manipulations in genetic mouse models of social deficits (Dolen et al, 2010; Carlson, 2012). Unfortunately, results from these studies have been inconsistent, depending on the genetic abnormality and behavioral phenotype. This is further complicated by findings that both positive (Clifton et al, 2013) and negative (Silverman et al, 2012) allosteric modulators of mGluR5 confer model- and phenotype-specific behavioral benefits. For example, impaired social novelty discrimination induced by neonatal treatment with phencyclidine is improved by activation of mGluR5 with positive allosteric modulators (Clifton et al, 2013). On the other hand, deficits of sociability in BTBR mice were improved by negative allosteric modulators of mGluR5 (Silverman et al, 2012). Interestingly, the role of mGluR5 in the social behavior of normal, genetically intact mice has also provided controversial evidence, with pharmacological approaches yielding strain-dependent effects. Antagonism of mGluR5 in Balb/c mice did not affect time spent in a chamber with a juvenile but it enhanced sniffing, whereas the same manipulation in Swiss Webster mice enhanced time spent in the chamber but reduced sniffing (Burket et al, 2011). Overall, the role of mGluR5 in social behavior has not yet been clarified, possibly because of the limited specificity of pharmacological approaches, reliance on acute treatments, or use of systemic manipulations.

Unlike mGluR5, oxytocin has traditionally been viewed as ‘the’ social hormone and a number of pharmacological manipulations of Oxtr has implicated this system in virtually every aspect of social functioning (Ross and Young, 2009). Surprisingly, genetic deletion of oxytocin or Oxtr has produced specific deficits of social memory while leaving other social functions intact (Ferguson et al, 2000; Crawley et al, 2007). However, even in genetic models, the data have not been consistent, and more general deficits have also been reported (Sala et al, 2013). It is not yet clear whether these discrepancies are due to insufficient specificity of pharmacological manipulations, compensatory mechanisms in constitutive Oxtr knockouts, or to the complex and possibly opposing contributions of different brain areas and circuits to social behaviors.

We attempted to establish the roles of mGluR5 and Oxtr on different aspects of social behavior in a comparative study, using conditional and region-specific genetic manipulations of these receptors. We selected this approach to achieve highly specific manipulations of each receptor while minimizing the possibility of compensation. We focused on the lateral septum because this area contains some of the highest brain levels of Oxtr (Lee et al, 2008) and mGluR5 (Shigemoto et al, 1993; Romano et al, 1995) and is therefore a likely target for their actions. In addition, the lateral septum has a well-established role in social behavior and it is directly connected (Risold and Swanson, 1997) to other key brain areas regulating social interactions, such as CA2 hippocampus (Hitti and Siegelbaum, 2014) and dopaminergic circuits (Gunaydin et al, 2014). To examine the specificity of Oxtr and mGluR5 in social versus nonsocial behavior, we also tested the mice in novel object recognition and fear conditioning paradigms. Our findings demonstrate highly specific roles of septal mGluR5 and Oxtr in social behavior, as indicated by a double dissociation of their effects on sociability and social novelty.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Nine-week-old male C57BL/6J mice with floxed mGluR5 (Xu, 2009, p 669) or Oxtr (Nishimori, 2008 p 1830) were backcrossed for over nine generations into wild-type C57BL/6J females (Jackson) and subsequently maintained by homozygous breeding in Northwestern University’s animal facility. After weaning, the mice were individually housed on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 0700 hours), and allowed ad libitum access to food and water. Nine- to eleven-week-old mice were used for the experiments. Wild-type C57BL/6N male mice (Harlan) of the same age served as the stimulus mice. One week before the beginning of the experiments, the mice were transferred to our satellite facility, which provides similar environmental conditions. In the facility, the mice were housed in laminar flow cabinets (Scanbur, Sollentuna, Sweden) adjacent to the behavioral testing equipment to avoid transport between housing and testing. Separate groups of mice were used for each behavioral paradigm. All procedures were approved by Northwestern University’s Animal Care and Use Committee in compliance with National Institutes of Health standards.

Surgery and Cannulation

Double guide cannulas (Plastics One) were implanted in the lateral septum as described previously (Guzman et al, 2013). Briefly, mice were anesthetized with 1.2% tribromoethanol (Avertin) and implanted with bilateral cannulas (26-gauge outer and 28-gauge inner diameter, 1 mm center-to-center distance) using a digital stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments). The coordinates used to target lateral septum were 0.2 mm anterior±0.5 mm lateral, and 3.0 mm ventral to bregma. Mice were allowed 1 week to recover before virus injections. Only mice with correct cannula placements, as determined at the end of the experiments with histological examination of brain tissue, were included in data analyses.

Infusion of Viral Vectors

The viral vectors AAV-GFP-Cre (AAV-Cre; driven by a synapsin promoter and obtained from Pavel Osten) or AAV-GFP (driven by a synapsin promoter, University of Pennsylvania Viral Vector Core) were infused intraseptally over 2 min (0.1 μl/min) under light isoflurane anesthesia. All viruses had a titer of 1–2 × 1012 particles per ml and were of the AAV2 serotypes. Before any behavioral training, viruses incubated for 14 days to allow for significant knockdown. After the end of each experiment, all brains were collected for histological verification of cannula placements or immunohistochemical or quantitative PCR (qPCR) validation of genetic knockdown. We demonstrated previously that the AAV-GFP and AAV-Cre do not exert toxic effects on lateral septal neurons, and that AAV-Cre did not affect memory-related or other behaviors of wild-type mice (Guzman et al, 2013). On this basis, and to minimize confounds of genetic factors (eg with comparisons of floxed versus wild-type mice), all experiments were performed with floxed mice receiving either AAV-GFP or AAV-Cre.

Social Interaction Paradigm

To assess sociability and social novelty, we used a three-compartment social behavior chamber (Moy et al, 2004; Sankoorikal et al, 2006). The chamber was a glass rectangular box (76.2 cm long × 30.5 cm wide × 30.5 cm tall) divided into three compartments with a mesh cylinder (11.4 cm diameter) placed in each side compartment. The mesh cylinder allowed for air exchange and for auditory, visual, and olfactory interaction, but prevented fighting, between experimental and stimulus mice. Testing consisted of a 5-min habituation phase, in which no stimuli were present; a 5-min sociability test, in which a stimulus mouse and an inanimate object (toy mouse) were present in separate chambers; and a 5-min preference for social novelty test, in which the toy mouse was replaced by a novel mouse. Animals’ position and motion were tracked by a top-mounted video camera connected to a computer and a DVD recorder and analyzed using Videomot II tracking software (TSE Systems). For analysis, time spent in each compartment during each phase of the test and time spent in close proximity (within 4 cm) of the stimulus mice during the test phase were calculated and expressed as percent of the total test time (5 min). Because these parameters yielded similar results, we present only the percent of time spent in each compartment relative to the total test time so that the habituation data (without a stimulus) can also be compared. Sociability was assessed by the time spent with a mouse versus an object, whereas preference for social novelty was assessed by the time spent with a new versus a familiar mouse, and each was presented as percent of the total test time.

Novel Object Recognition Paradigm

The novel object recognition paradigm consisted of an identical apparatus and involved a similar procedure as described above, except that instead of conspecifics, mice were exposed to different objects as described previously (Gao et al, 2011). Briefly, after a 5-min habituation period, the mice were exposed for 5 min to a cube in one, and a bottle in the other side compartment. Subsequently, one of the objects was randomly replaced with a novel object (a tape roll), and the time spent in the chamber of the novel versus familiar object was calculated relative to total test time.

Fear Conditioning Paradigm

Fear conditioning took place in a 35 × 20 × 20 cm3 Plexiglas chamber with a stainless-steel rod floor (4 mm diameter, 0.9 cm center to center) located within a sound-attenuating cabinet with black inner walls (TSE Systems). The box was cleaned after each mouse with 70% ethanol. Mice were placed in the chamber and 3 min later presented with a footshock (2 s, 0.7 mA, constant current). Mice were tested for fear to the conditioning context 24 h later by returning them to this chamber for 3 min tests. Freezing was scored every 5 s by a trained observer blind to the experimental conditions, and expressed as the percentage of total number of observations (Guzman et al, 2013).

RNA Extraction and qPCR for Determination of Oxtr mRNA Levels

Mice were injected intraseptally with methylene blue to visualize the viral injection site and then killed by cervical dislocation. Brains were dissected and the lateral septum was collected using a brain matrix. Tissue was homogenized in lysis buffer with β-mercaptoethanol and frozen in liquid nitrogen. RNA was extracted using miRCURY Total RNA Isolation Kit (Exiqon), reversely transcribed, and subjected to real-time PCR using SYBR Green master mix (Applied Biosystems) and primers for Oxtr or mouse hypoxanthine phosphoribosyl transferase 1 (mHprt1) as an internal control (Tronson et al, 2009). For Oxtr, the forward primer was 5′-GGAGCGTCTGGGACGTCAAT-3′ and the reverse primer was 5′-AGGAAGCGCTGCACGAGTT-3′. For mHprt1, the forward primer was 5′-GGGCTTACCTCACTGCTTTC-3′ and reverse 5′-TCTCCACCAATAACTTTTATGTCC-3′. The level of Oxtr expression in Oxtrflox/flox mice injected with rAAV-Cre was normalized to mHprt1 and shown relative to the rAAV-GFP control.

Immunoblot

Septal tissue was collected around the tips of the cannula and lysed in modified radioimmunoprecipitation buffer (20 mM Tris-HCl (pH 7.5), 150 mM NaCl, 1 mM Na2EDTA, 1 mM EGTA, 1% NP-40, 1 mM Na3VO4) containing a protease inhibitor cocktail (Pierce), incubated for 15 min on ice, and centrifuged for 15 min (15 000 g) at 4 °C. Samples were subjected to SDS-PAGE (10 μg per well) and transferred to polyvinylidene difluoride membranes (Millipore). Membranes were blocked with I-block (Tropix), incubated with primary antibody overnight at 4 °C, and with secondary antibody conjugated to alkaline phosphatase for 1 h at room temperature, followed by incubations with the chemiluminescent enhancer Nitroblock II and chemiluminescent substrate CDP star (Tropix). Bands were visualized after exposing the membranes to X-ray films. Primary antibodies used were against α-tubulin, 1:2000 (mouse; Sigma), and anti-mGluR5, 1:6000 (rabbit; Millipore), as shown previously (Tronson et al, 2010).

Immunohistochemistry

Brains were fixed with 4% PFA, dehydrated in 30% sucrose, and stained using the Vectastain system (Vector), as described previously (Guzman et al, 2013). Immunostaining with mouse anti-GFP (1:6000; Abcam) was visualized with fluorescein isothiocyanate, whereas rabbit anti-mGluR5 (1:16 000; Millipore) was visualized with Coumarin. Slices were mounted in Vectashield (Vector) and observed with a confocal laser scanning microscope (Olympus Fluoview FV10i) for double labeling at × 40. Septal sections were analyzed for double labeling at the coordinates (Paxinos and Franklin, 2004) AP +0.14 to +0.38. Quantification of mGluR5 signals was performed using the ImageJ software, by analyzing the density of mGluR5 signals or determining the number of Venus-positive neurons in the dorsal and lateral septum.

Statistical Analysis

We used 7–10 mice per experimental group for the behavioral studies. For immunohistochemical, qPCR, and immunoblot assays, we used biological replicates of 4–6 samples per group. This sample size gave us statistical power of 80% to detect knockdown effects. All data showed normal distribution and were analyzed by repeated-measures ANOVA with factors genotype and test condition (sociability versus social novelty). Two-tailed Student’s t-test was used to determine within-group side preference. Data are presented as mean±SEM.

RESULTS

Conditional Knockdown of mGluR5 and Oxtr by rAAV-Cre

Infusion of rAAV-Cre resulted in a conditional mGluR5 knockdown in the lateral septum in mGluR5flox/flox mice, with an average decrease of ~75% of mGluR5 protein levels when compared with the rAAV-GFP controls (Figure 1a; t(9)=8.081, P<0.001, N=5 per group). Validation of cannula placements by histological analysis confirmed the septal infusion sites (Figure 1b). Because specific Oxtr antibodies were not available, Oxtr knockdown in Oxtrflox/flox was validated with qPCR. Similar to our earlier observations (Tronson et al, 2009; Guzman et al, 2014), infusion of rAAV-Cre resulted in a conditional Oxtr knockdown in the lateral septum, with an average decrease of ~70% of Oxtr mRNA when compared with the rAAV-GFP control (Figure 1c; t(7)=7.65, P<0.001, n=4 per group). Cannula placements from these experiments are depicted in Figure 1d. These studies showed that rAAV-Cre caused a significant knockdown of mGluR5 and Oxtr in their corresponding floxed mouse lines.

Figure 1
figure 1

Conditional knockdown of mGluR5 and Oxtr in the lateral septum. (a) Protein levels of mGluR5 in septal lysates of mGluR5flox/flox mice injected with rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre. The immunoblot is shown below. (b) Cannula placement sites for mice used in the mGluR5 knockdown experiments. An example of GFP signals in the lateral septum is illustrated below. (c) Oxtr mRNA levels in septi of Oxtrflox/flox mice injected with rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre. (d) Cannula placement sites for mice used in the Oxtr knockdown experiments. Statistically significant differences: *P<0.001, rAAV-Cre versus rAAV-GFP controls.

PowerPoint slide

mGluR5 Knockdown in the Lateral Septum Disrupts Sociability but not Preference for Social Novelty

mGluR5flox/flox mice infused with rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre did not show innate preference for any side of the chamber during habituation (Figure 2a; rAAV-GFP: t(1,17)=0.118, P=0.903; rAAV-Cre: t(1,17)=0.112, P=0.913, n=9 per group). On tests of sociability and social novelty, repeated-measures ANOVA found significant effects of test (F(1,16)=107.66, P<0.001), group (F(1,16)=11.21, P<0.01), and test × genotype (F(1,16)=21.83, P<0.001) interaction. Repeated contrary to rAAV-GFP controls, who displayed a significant preference for the social versus the nonsocial stimulus (Figure 2b; t(1,17)=3.119, P<0.05)), rAAV-Cre group similarly explored the social and nonsocial stimulus (t(1,17)=0.219, P=0.830). These findings demonstrate decreased sociability in mGluR5 knockdown mice. Despite this deficit, the mice showed preserved preference for social novelty (Figure 2c) and spent increased time exploring the novel versus familiar stimulus mice (rAAV-GFP: t(1,17)=3.43, P<0.05; rAAV-Cre: t(1,17)=2.961, P<0.05). These findings revealed a specific deficit of sociability induced by mGluR5 knockdown.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effect of septal mGluR5 knockdown on sociability and social novelty. (a) Behavior recorded during habituation and expressed as % time spent in the side compartments of the chamber. (b) Sociability, expressed as % time spent with the conspecific when compared with the toy mouse. (c) Preference for social novelty, expressed as % time spent with the novel when compared with the familiar mouse. Statistically significant differences: *P<0.05, versus corresponding control (toy mouse or novel mouse). Note: the schemes on the left illustrate the general design; however, during experiments the side was counterbalanced.

PowerPoint slide

Oxtr Knockdown in the Lateral Septum Disrupts Preference for Social Novelty but not Sociability

Similar to mGluR5flox/flox mice, Oxtrflox/flox mice infused with rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre equally explored the different sides of the chamber (t(1,17)=0.149, P=0.955, n=9 per group) during habituation (Figure 3a). On tests of sociability and social novelty, repeated-measures ANOVA found significant effects of test (F(1,16)=76.01, P<0.001), group (F(1,16)=8.246, P<0.05), and test × genotype (F(1,16)=24.68, P<0.001) interaction. Lateral septal Oxtr knockdown did not affect sociability (Figure 3b), as revealed by significantly more time spent with the social versus nonsocial stimulus in both rAAV-GFP (t(1,17)=3.55, P<0.05) and rAAV-Cre groups (t(1,17)=3.24, P<0.05). However, preference for social novelty was significantly disrupted (Figure 3c), as revealed by the finding that rAAV-Cre mice similarly explored the novel and familiar stimulus mouse (t(1,17)=0.186, P=0.645), whereas rAAV-GFP controls showed significant preference for the novel stimulus mouse (t(1,17)=4.27, P<0.01). Thus, contrary to the mGluR5 knockdown, Oxtr knockdown did not affect sociability but selectively impaired preference for social novelty.

Figure 3
figure 3

Effect of septal Oxtr knockdown on sociability and social novelty. (a) Behavior recorded during habituation and expressed as % time spent in the side compartments of the chamber. (b) Sociability, expressed as % time spent with the conspecific when compared with the toy mouse. (c) Preference for social novelty, expressed as % time spent with the novel when compared with the familiar mouse. Statistically significant differences: *P<0.05, versus toy mouse; or novel mouse; **P<0.01, versus novel mouse.

PowerPoint slide

mGluR5 or Oxtr Knockdown in the Lateral Septum do not Affect Novel Object Recognition Memory

We used the novel object recognition task to examine whether the observed deficits were specific for social behavior or also extend to nonsocial stimuli. All objects were selected based on their similar preference by the mice (Gao et al, 2011). Accordingly, mice of each group expressed similar interest in the objects during the first exposure, for mGlur5flox/flox (Figure 4b; F(3,28)=0.225, P=0.72, n=7–8) and Oxtrflox/flox mice (Figure 4e; F(3,27)=0.134, P=0.682, n=7–8). On tests of exposure to a novel versus familiar object, repeated-measures ANOVA performed on mGluR5 knockdown found significant effects of test (F(1,13)=150.08, P<0.001) but not genotype (F(1,16)=0.851, P=0.373) or test × genotype (F(1,16)=2.98, P=0.104) interaction. Similar results were obtained for the Oxtr knockdown, with significant effects of test (F(1,13)=123.22, P<0.001) but not group (F(1,13)=0.714, P=0.325) or test × genotype (F(1,13)=2.44, P=0.113) interaction. An overall one-way ANOVA on the test day revealed that both mGluR5 and Oxtr knockdown mice, as well as controls, recognized the novel object in the last phase of the paradigm, as revealed by a significantly increased time spent in the chamber of the novel when compared with the familiar object (Figure 4c; F(3,26)=8.43, P<0.01, n=7–8) and Oxtrflox/flox mice (Figure 4f; F(3,26)=7.92, P<0.01, n=7–8). These findings show that interest in and recognition of inanimate objects was intact in mGluR5 and Oxtr knockdown mice, and thus their deficits were restricted to social behavior.

Figure 4
figure 4

Effect of septal mGluR5 and Oxtr knockdown on novel object recognition. (a and b) Behavior of rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre-injected mGluR5flox/flox mice during habituation and object exploration. (c) Preference for novel object, expressed as % time spent with object 3 when compared with the familiar object 1. (d and e) Behavior of rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre-injected Oxtrflox/flox mice during habituation and object exploration. (f) Preference for novel object, expressed as % time spent with object 3 when compared with the familiar object 1. Statistically significant differences: *P<0.01, versus familiar object. As in Figures 2 and 3, the schematics depicts the general setup, but the object side was counterbalanced within a single experiment.

PowerPoint slide

mGluR5 or Oxtr Knockdown in the Lateral Septum do not Affect Fear Conditioning

In addition to social behavior, the lateral septum mediates fear- and anxiety-related behaviors (Gray and McNaughton, 2000). Both mGluR5 and Oxtr have been implicated in the modulation of contextual fear by social or nonsocial stress (Tronson et al, 2010; Guzman et al, 2013), thus we next examined whether the septal knockdown of these receptors affected contextual fear conditioning. The results did not reveal any differences in freezing behavior between mGluR5 (Figure 5a; t(15)=0.223, P=0.85, N=8 per group) or Oxtr knockdown mice (Figure 5b; t(15)=0.257, P=0.88, N=8 per group) and their corresponding controls, suggesting that neurotransmitter systems other than mGluR5 or Oxtr regulate fear-motivated learning via the lateral septum.

Figure 5
figure 5

Effect of septal mGluR5 and Oxtr knockdown on contextual fear conditioning. (a) Behavior of rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre-injected mGluR5flox/flox mice during a context memory test expressed as % time spent freezing. (b) Behavior of rAAV-GFP or rAAV-Cre-injected Oxtrflox/flox mice during a context memory test expressed as % time spent freezing.

PowerPoint slide

mGluR5 and Oxtr are Expressed in Different Neuronal Populations of the Lateral Septum

The double dissociation of the effects of mGluR5 and Oxtr on sociability and preference for social novelty could be due to actions of these receptors on the same neuronal population (but using different signaling pathways) or by engaging different neuronal populations. To better understand the distribution of mGluR5 and Oxtr in the lateral septum, we performed a colocalization study. We first demonstrated that mGluR5 are abundant at the lateral septum (Figure 6a), at neuroanatomical coordinates showing strong Oxtr signals (Guzman et al, 2013). We next confirmed, using immunofluorescent labeling of Venus (green) in Venus/Oxtr reporter mice (Yoshida et al, 2009) and mGluR5 (blue) (Figure 6b), that both signals were the strongest in the caudal part of the lateral septum. However, whereas mGluR5 signals were most prominent in the dorsal and dorsolateral area, Oxtr-positive cells were predominantly found in the intermediate- and ventrolateral area. Our microscopic analyses did not reveal significant colocalization of mGluR5 and Oxtr (Figure 6c), given that mGluR5 signals were significantly higher in the dorsal versus lateral septum (t(7)=8.45, P<0.001), whereas Venus signals were predominant in the lateral versus dorsal septum (t(7)=7.96, P<0.001). This suggested that different neuronal populations of the lateral septum are engaged in the regulation of sociability and preference for social novelty.

Figure 6
figure 6

Localization of mGluR5 and Oxtr in the lateral septum. (a) Validation of the specificity of anti-mGluR5 antibody in wild-type and mGluR5−/− mice. The schematics show the anatomical level of the images shown in the right panel. The antibody gave strong signals throughout the dorsomedial neuropil of the septum, whereas only background staining was noted in the medial septum. (b) Similar mGluR5 signals (blue) were detected in Venus reporter mice (left panel). Sections of these mice also showed Venus-positive neurons in intermediate (LSI) but not dorsal (DS) lateral septum (middle panel). The mGlu5 and Venus signals did not overlap (right panel). (c) Quantification of mGluR5 density and number of Venus-positive neurons in the DS and LSI. Statistically significant differences: *P<0.001, dorsal versus intermediate lateral septum.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

We demonstrated that lateral septal mGluR5 and Oxtr has specific roles in the expression of social behaviors, with distinct contributions of mGluR5 and Oxtr to sociability and preference for social novelty. These findings suggest that rather than regulating social behavior in general, regional neurotransmitter systems mediated by G-protein-coupled receptors regulate different aspects of social functioning.

Research on social behavior was markedly boosted by its implications in ASD, and many genetic mouse models displaying abnormal social interactions have been used to explore potential therapeutics. One of the earliest models implicating mGluR5 in social behavior relevant for ASD is the fragile X syndrome model with Fmr1 mutant mice, in which enhanced mGluR5 activity has been linked to disease symptoms (Nakamoto et al, 2007). Systemic administration of the noncompetitive mGluR5 antagonist 2-methyl-6-phenylethynyl-pyridine (MPEP) normalized seizures, prepulse inhibition, and dendritic spine densities (Yan et al, 2005; de Vrij et al, 2008). Genetic manipulations, however, did not support the link between enhanced mGluR5 activity and impairments of social behavior, as revealed by subtle, if any improvements of social behaviors in Fmr1 mutant mice (Thomas et al, 2011). In a different model, the BTBR inbred mouse strain, MPEP improved stereotyped behavior without affecting sociability (Silverman et al, 2010), whereas a more specific mGluR5 antagonist, GRN-529, partially reversed the deficits of sociability (Silverman et al, 2012). Finally, other models yielded quite opposite findings, suggesting that activation of mGluR5 is needed for adequate social behavior. In the ASD model of Shank2 knockout mice, a positive allosteric modulator of mGluR5 improved the social interaction deficits (Won et al, 2012). Similarly, in a developmental model of ASD, positive allosteric modulation of mGluR5 improved adult onset social deficits (Clifton et al, 2013). Unlike disease models, physiological studies of the role of mGluR5 in social behavior have been scarce, and have mainly relied on systemic injection of MPEP. This drug blocks N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors (O'Leary et al, 2000; Movsesyan et al, 2001) and monoamine oxidase-A (Cosford et al, 2003) in addition to mGluR5, which could have contributed to the inconsistent effects on sociability in Balb and Swiss-Webster mice (Burket et al, 2011). Our data are in agreement with findings obtained using the more specific mGluR5 antagonist, 3-[(2-methyl-4-thiazolyl) ethynyl] pyridine, MTEP, which impairs sociability both after acute and subchronic administration without affecting locomotor hyperactivity or stereotypy (Koros et al, 2007). Here we provide genetic evidence supporting an important and specific contribution of mGluR5 to sociability, and identify the lateral septum as the neuroanatomical target of this action. Taken together, these findings suggest that, unless the receptor is overactivated, antagonists and negative allosteric modulators of mGluR5 are more likely to worsen rather than improve deficits of sociability, whereas positive allosteric modulators might prove beneficial.

Contrary to mGluR5, the physiological role of oxytocin and Oxtr has been extensively investigated, with the majority of findings supporting a role in affiliative behavior and sociability (Neumann, 2008; Caldwell, 2012). Based on these findings, oxytocin has been linked to ASD (Insel et al, 1999) with promising outcomes in animal models (Teng et al, 2013). Surprisingly, approaches with constitutive genetic knockouts of oxytocin and Oxtr did not provide unequivocal support to this view. Oxytocin and Oxtr knockouts exhibited profound deficits of social memory (Ferguson et al, 2000; Lee et al, 2008); however, studies on sociability and preference for social novelty provided inconsistent data. One study reported no effects in two knockout lines (Crawley et al, 2007), whereas others reported deficits of preference for social novelty in knockouts (Lee et al, 2008; Pobbe et al, 2012) or deficits of both sociability and preference for social novelty in Oxtr+/− mice (Sala et al, 2013). Compensation, in particular via the vasopressin system, could have contributed to the restricted behavioral phenotypes of genetic manipulations. Our findings, after inducible knockdown of Oxtr, fully support the specific role of Oxtr in social memory, as shown by intact sociability but impaired preference for social novelty, as well as social recognition, as shown earlier (Guzman et al, 2013).

Both sociability and preference for social novelty have been used to model the first of three core symptoms of ASDs: abnormal social interactions, impaired communication, and stereotyped behavior. However, these phenotypes engage different processes: sociability primarily reflects the motivation to interact with conspecifics, whereas preference for social novelty can be viewed as a short-term memory process where recognition of a familiar conspecific critically drives behavior toward the novel one (Kaidanovich-Beilin et al, 2011). Accordingly, the effects of mGluR5 and Oxtr knockdown observed in the lateral septum likely reflect disparate actions on social motivation versus social memory. In line with this view, the non-overlapping distribution of mGluR5 and Oxtr within separate lateral septal compartments suggests that in addition to different receptor types, different neuronal populations underlie the motivational versus cognitive aspects of social behavior. Given the tight relationship between the organization of septal neurons and their inputs, this further suggests that mGluR5- and Oxtr-positive cell populations are regulated by projections originating from different hippocampal subdivisions (Risold and Swanson, 1997) or other areas involved in the regulation of social behavior.

Our data on the distinct mechanisms of social behavioral phenotypes are supported by findings showing that mice containing NMDA receptors with reduced affinity for glycine have sociability deficits but normal preference for social novelty (Labrie et al, 2008). If confirmed in humans, these data suggest that treatment of social dysfunction in patients with mental disorders might require different approaches for individual components of social functioning. To target specifically the lateral septal receptor populations, potential mGluR5 and Oxtr-based therapeutics could take advantage of the rapid advances of technologies for guiding therapeutics to desired locations (Pilakka-Kanthikeel et al, 2013) or for activating defined neuronal circuits (Balconi and Canavesio, 2014).

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflict of interest.