INTRODUCTION

Multiple sclerosis (MS) causes cognitive deficits since the early stages of the disease, by altering memory, attention, and executive functions (Amato et al, 2004, 2006a, 2008a, 2008b; Chiaravalloti and DeLuca, 2008; Genova et al, 2009).

The synaptic and molecular mechanisms at the basis of cognitive impairment in MS are still poorly understood. A large body of evidence converges in indicating that learning and memory processes are encoded in brain circuits as experience- and activity-dependent long-term synaptic changes. In particular, long-term potentiation (LTP) is a form of synaptic plasticity in which repeated stimulation connects neurons by making synapses more responsive to future activation, and is commonly considered a major candidate for representing the substrate of learning and memory processes (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Malenka, 2003).

LTP induction is regulated by a variety of factors, able to interfere with critical receptor and post-receptor events at excitatory synapses (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Malenka, 2003; Feldman, 2009; Kessels and Malinow, 2009; Minichiello, 2009). Among these factors, amyloid-β, particularly the isoform amyloid-β1–42, that was consistently found to be reduced in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, has a major function in LTP deficits and cognitive impairment in AD. More specifically, amyloid-β1–42 aggregates in oligomers that impair synaptic plasticity mechanisms (Klyubin et al, 2005; Shankar et al, 2008; Walther et al, 2009; Townsend et al, 2010). Its role in other disorders associated with mental deterioration is, however, still unclear.

Amyloid-β peptides are formed by proteolytic cleavage of a transmembrane protein, the amyloid precursor protein (APP) by β-site APP-cleaving enzyme 1 (BACE). BACE cleaves APP, causing the secretion of an extracellular soluble fragment of APP, termed sAPP-β, and the retention of a 99-residue C-terminal fragment. This fragment can undergo further cleavage by γ-secretase to release 40 or 42 amino-acid long amyloid-β fragments into the cytoplasm. However, APP may undergo at least two and most likely several different processing pathways. In the non-amyloidogenic pathway, APP is cleaved at the α-site in the middle of the amyloid-β domain. This cleavage obliterates amyloid-β formation, instead yielding an N-terminal sAPP-α fragment (Andreasson et al, 2007).

We postulated that, in MS, acute inflammation is able to interfere with amyloid-β1–42 or τ metabolism, and that, like in AD, this abnormal metabolism could be associated with cognitive dysfunction and synaptic plasticity. Notably, although both amyloid-β1–42 and τ protein alter LTP induction and cognitive performances in animal models (Oddo et al, 2003; Klyubin et al, 2005; Rosenmann et al, 2008; Shankar et al, 2008; Polydoro et al, 2009; Walther et al, 2009; Townsend et al, 2010), their role in human synaptic plasticity has never been explored.

In humans, LTP can be explored in a safe and non-invasive manner by using paradigms of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) (Ziemann, 2004; Cooke and Bliss, 2006; Di Lazzaro et al, 2008). A novel LTP-inducing protocol has been recently developed, in the attempt to mimic the physiological activity of hippocampal neurons during learning episodes (Huang et al, 2005; Di Lazzaro et al, 2008). This new protocol has been termed θ burst stimulation (TBS) and, according to in vitro experiments in hippocampal preparations, it produces an NMDA receptor-dependent LTP in the human cortex, by enhancing corticospinal excitability for several minutes after the end of the stimulation (Huang et al, 2005).

Thus, in this study, we investigated whether altered CSF concentrations of amyloid-β1−42 metabolism are associated with abnormal patterns of cognitive performances and of cortical plasticity in a sample of MS patients that were submitted to an extensive neuropsychological examination and to neurophysiological investigations of cortical plasticity by means of TBS.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Hospital Tor Vergata, Rome.

Abbreviations used throughout the text are provided in the Supplementary Text.

MS Patients

We collected CSF from 42 patients (30 females and 12 males, aged 23–54 years) admitted to the neurological clinic of the University Hospital Tor Vergata of Rome, and later diagnosed as suffering from clinically isolated syndrome (CIS) (n=7), relapsing-remitting (RR) MS (n=30), or primary progressive (PP) MS (n=5). After their admittance, all patients underwent, in sequence, brain (and in selected cases also spinal) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan, neuropsychological assessment, TMS, and CSF withdrawal within 24 h. Corticosteroids or other MS-specific immunoactive therapies were initiated later when appropriate.

The diagnosis of CIS or MS was established by clinical, laboratory, and MRI parameters, and matched published criteria (McDonald et al, 2001; Polman et al, 2005). In all instances, patients underwent detection of oligoclonal banding in the CSF (positive in 92% of cases). Expanded disability status scale (EDSS) scores were between 0 and 5.

As controls, we used CSF from 12 age- and gender-matched individuals (eight females and four males, aged 25–52 years) without inflammatory or degenerative diseases of the central or peripheral nervous system. These subjects underwent lumbar puncture because of a clinical suspect of acute peripheral neuropathy, meningitis, or subarachnoidal hemorrhage, which were not confirmed. All the subjects gave their written informed consent to the study.

MRI Acquisition and Analysis

Three Tesla MRI scans consisted of dual-echo proton density, FLAIR, T2-weighted spin-echo images, and pre-contrast and post-contrast T1-weighted spin-echo images. All images were acquired in the axial orientation with 3 mm-thick contiguous slices. T2 lesion volume was determined by manual tracing and the number of gadolinium-enhancing (Gd+) (0.2 ml/kg e.v.) lesions was counted by a neuroradiologist who was unaware of the patient's clinical details (Centonze et al, 2007).

CSF Determination of Amyloid-β1–42 and Total τ

Immediately after its collection, CSF was centrifuged to eliminate cells and cellular debris and stored at −80°C until analyzed (Centonze et al, 2007). Levels of both amyloid-β1–42 and total τ were determined according to standard procedures, using commercially available sandwich enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays (Innotest β-Amyloid 1–42, Innotest h-τ Ag, Innogenetics, Ghent, Belgium) (Sancesario et al, 2010). Briefly, 25 μl of the CSF from each patient were dispensed into corresponding 96-well ELISA plates, pre-coated either with the monoclonal antibody 21F12 for amyloid-β1–42, or AT120 for total τ, and incubated, respectively, with the biotinylated antibody 3D6 or HT7. Bound antibodies were then detected by a peroxidase-labeled streptavidin, after addition of a substrate solution. The reaction was stopped by sulfuric acid. The absorbance of the reaction product was read at 450 nm. The biomarker concentrations in the samples were calculated based on the amyloid-β1–42 and τ standard sigmoid curve equation.

Neuropsychological Assessment

Cognitive functions were assessed through the Brief Repeatable Neuropsychological Battery (BRB) (Rao, 1990) by an expert trained clinician in a subgroup of 21 MS patients (4 CIS and 17 RR MS) who gave consent to the examination. The BRB assesses the cognitive domains most frequently impaired in MS (Amato et al, 2004), and incorporates tests of verbal memory (selective reminding test (SRT)); visual memory (10/36 Spatial Recall Test); attention, concentration, and speed of information processing (paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT), symbol digit modalities test (SDMT)); and verbal fluency (word list generation). Performance on each test of the BRB was assessed by applying the available Italian normative values (Amato et al, 2006b). Failure of a test was defined when the score was at least 2 SD below the mean normative values.

Consistently with previous works (Amato et al, 2006a, 2008b), those patients who failed at least three tests were considered cognitively impaired (CI), and those who failed less than three tests were considered cognitively preserved (CP). No subjects were taking psychoactive drugs or substances that might interfere with neuropsychological performance.

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

TMS protocols were performed in a subgroup of 30 MS patients (5 CIS, 23 RR MS, and 2 PP MS) who gave consent to the examination and were asymptomatic in the upper right limb. Electromyographic traces were recorded from the right first dorsal interosseus muscle (FDI) with surface cup electrodes. The active electrode was placed over the muscle belly and the reference electrode over the metacarpophalangeal joint of the index finger. Responses were amplified with a Digitimer D360 amplifier (Digitimer, Welwyn Garden City, Hertfordshire, UK) through filters set at 20 Hz and 2 kHz with a sampling rate of 5 kHz, then recorded by a computer with SIGNAL software (Cambridge Electronic Devices, Cambridge, UK).

MEPs were evoked through a figure-of-eight coil with external loop diameter of 70 mm connected to a Magstim 2002 magnetic stimulator (Magstim Company, Whitland, Wales, UK). Coil position was adjusted to find the optimal scalp site to evoke motor responses in the contralateral FDI, the motor ‘hot spot,’ at the beginning of each experimental session and marked over the patient's scalp with a pencil. The coil was held tangentially to the scalp surface with the handle pointing posteriorly and laterally at about 45° with respect to the mid-sagittal axis of the head.

Intermittent TBS

Intermittent TBS (iTBS) was delivered over the motor ‘hot spot’ of the right FDI through a Magstim Rapid2 stimulator. The active motor threshold (AMT) was defined as the minimum stimulation intensity required to evoke a liminal motor potential from the FDI during voluntary contraction (about 200 μV in 50% of 10 trials). Stimulation intensity was 80% of AMT. The iTBS protocol consisted of 10 bursts, each burst composed of three stimuli at 50 Hz, repeated at a θ frequency of 5 Hz every 10 s for a total of 600 stimuli (200 s). Sixty MEPs were collected before iTBS (baseline) and at two different time points (0 and 15 min) after the end of iTBS. Stimulation intensity was set to induce a stable MEP of 1 mV peak-to-peak amplitude in the relaxed right FDI at baseline and remained unchanged until end of recordings. MEP's amplitudes were then averaged at each time point and normalized to the mean baseline amplitude.

Intracortical Circuits in Right M1

We also tested, through paired-pulse (pp) TMS, short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI; mediated by intrinsic GABAAergic circuits) (Kujirai et al, 1993), intracortical facilitation (ICF; believed to follow the preferential recruitment of intrinsic excitatory fibers) (Kujirai et al, 1993), short intracortical facilitation (SICF; likely mediated by excitatory cortical interneurons) (Ziemann et al, 1998), and long-interval intracortical inhibition (LICI; mediated by local GABAB pathways) (Valls-Solé et al, 1992) of the left M1. One figure-of-eight coil, external diameter 70 mm was held tangentially to the scalp over the motor ‘hot spot’ for right FDI muscle. Stimulation intensity for TS was adjusted in each experiment to evoke an MEP of 1 mV peak-to-peak amplitude in the relaxed right FDI.

SICI and ICF were tested using ppTMS with a subthreshold CS preceding a suprathreshold TS (Kujirai et al, 1993; Rothwell, 1997). CS stimulus was set at 80% AMT. Three conditions were presented in a random order: control (TS given alone) and two pp conditions (TS preceded by CS) at one of two different ISI (2 and 10 ms).

For SICF, the intensity of CS was set to 90% RMT. Six randomly intermixed conditions were presented in a random order: TS given alone and five conditions with the TS followed by CS at one of five different ISI (1.5, 2.1, 2.7, 3.7, and 4.5 ms) (Hanajima et al, 2002).

For LICI, the intensity of CS was set at 120% RMT. Two conditions were presented in a random order: control (TS given alone) and one paired-pulse condition (TS preceded by CS) at 100 ms ISI (Valls-Solé et al, 1992). For each experiment, 10 responses were collected for the test stimulus alone and for conditioned MEPs at each ISI. Changes in MEP amplitude at each ISI were expressed as the percentage of the mean unconditioned MEP amplitude.

Data Analysis

Differences in τ and amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels were assessed using analysis of variance, followed by post hoc comparison using the Tukey highest significant difference procedure, to account for multiple comparisons and by t-test or Mann–Whitney test for comparisons between the two groups. Relationships between τ and amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and MRI findings were assessed using Spearman's correlation test for non-parametric values and Pearson's correlation test for parametric values. Correlation analysis between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and cognitive variables was performed by calculating Spearman's correlation coefficients.

Correlations between τ and amyloid-β1−42 CSF levels with iTBS at each time point, SICI, ICF, LICI, and SICF at each ISI were calculated using Pearson's correlation coefficient. Differences between groups were assessed using repeated measures analysis of variance for iTBS, SICF, SICI, ICF, and LICI. Bonferroni's correction was used to correct for multiple comparisons. Data, expressed as mean±SEM, were considered significant at the 0.05 level.

RESULTS

CSF Contents of Amyloid-β1–42 and total τ Protein in MS Patients and Their Correlation with Neuropsychological Parameters

We first measured amyloid-β1–42 and τ protein levels in the CSF of MS patients. Amyloid-β1–42 levels were significantly reduced in the population of MS subjects, compared with control individuals (MS group: 267±22 pg/ml, n=42; control group: 390±45 pg/ml, n=12, p<0.05) (Figure 1a). Conversely, τ protein levels were not altered in the CSF of MS patients (MS group: 128±11 pg/ml, n=42; control group: 143±14 pg/ml, n=12, NS) (Figure 1b).

Figure 1
figure 1

CSF contents of amyloid-β1–42 are reduced in MS patients and correlate with their cognitive functions. (a) Amyloid-β1–42 levels were significantly reduced in the CSF of MS subjects, compared with control individuals. (b) The τ protein levels were not altered in the CSF of MS patients. (c) In a subgroup of 21 MS patients, who gave consent to the neuropsychological assessment, amyloid-β1–42 levels were lower in cognitively impaired (CI) MS patients than in both cognitively preserved (CP) MS patients and controls. (d) The τ protein CSF levels were normal also in CI MS patients. (e) Correlation plot between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and PASAT score in MS subjects. (f) Correlation plot between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and SDMT in MS subjects. In this figure and in the following ones, amyloid-β1–42 is abbreviated in ‘amyloid-β*p<0.05 vs control **p<0.01 vs control #p<0.05 vs CP MS. PASAT, paced auditory serial addition test; SDMT, symbol digit modalities test.

PowerPoint slide

MS patients were, then, classified as CI (n=11) or CP (n=10) on the basis of their neuropsychological performance, without differences in terms of demographic characteristics (CI: 7 females and 4 males, aged 25–40 years with 11±7 years of education; CP: 6 females and 4 males, aged 23–37 years with 11±3 years of education).

The reduction of amyloid-β1–42 levels was related to cognitive dysfunction (CI MS group: 145±25 pg/ml, n=11; CP MS group: 297±46 pg/ml, n=10; control group: 390±45 pg/ml, n=12; F=9.81, p=0.0005), as in CP MS amyloid-β1–42 levels were higher than in CI MS patients (p<0.05), and indistinguishable from controls (NS) (Figure 1c). In CI MS patients, τ protein CSF levels were conversely still indistinguishable from those measured in controls and in CP patients (CI MS group: 110±18 pg/ml, CP MS group: 176±37 pg/ml, control group: 143±14 pg/ml, F=2.22, p=0.12) (Figure 1d).

The most frequently failed tests by examined MS patients were those assessing sustained attention and concentration (SDMT, PASAT) and verbal memory (SRT-LTS, SRT-CLTR). Interestingly, a moderate positive correlation was found between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and scores on measures of attention/concentration (SDMT: n=21, r=0.51, p=0.02; PASAT: n=21, r=0.54, p=0.01) (Figures 1e and f). The scores on verbal memory tests were, conversely, not significantly related to amyloid-β contents (SRT-LTS: n=21, r= −0.08, p=0.61; SRT-CLTR: n=21, r= −0.11, p=0.58). None of the neuropsychological measurements correlated to τ CSF levels in MS patients (n=21, p=NS for all parameters, not shown).

As pathology of corpus callosum (CC) may have a major role in cognitive impairment of MS patients (Ozturk et al, 2010), we further compared scores in all neuropsychological tests between patients showing lesions within the CC (n=6) and patients without CC lesions (n=15) at conventional MRI. Comparison of neuropsychological scores between CIS and RRMS was also performed. No significant differences emerged between groups (n=21, all p=NS) (not shown).

Correlation Between Amyloid-β1–42 and Total τ Protein with Gd+ Lesions in MS Patients

Upon grouping of MS patients according to the presence of Gd+ lesions at the MRI (Gd+MS group: n=22 (14 patients complaining new symptoms since 10 days), Gd−MS group: n=20 (four patients with new symptoms since 10 days), control group: n=12, F=7.98, p=0.0009), amyloid-β1–42 levels were lower in Gd+MS patients (209±26 pg/ml) than in both Gd−MS patients (331±25 pg/ml, p<0.05) and in controls (390±45 pg/ml, p<0.01) (Figure 2a). No significant alteration of τ protein CSF content was revealed by grouping Gd− and Gd+ patients (Gd+MS group: 129±11 pg/ml, Gd−MS group: 124±20 pg/ml, control group: 143±14 pg/ml F=0.3, p=0.74) (Figure 2b).

Figure 2
figure 2

Disease activity at MRI correlates with reduction of amyloid-β1–42 levels in the CSF of MS subjects. (a) The graph shows that, upon grouping of MS patients according to the presence of Gd+ lesions, amyloid-β1–42 levels were lower in Gd+MS subjects than in both Gd−MS subjects and controls. (b) The τ protein CSF levels were normal also in Gd+MS patients. (c) Correlation plot between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and the number of Gd+ lesions in Gd+MS subjects. (d) Correlation plot between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and T2 lesion volume in MS subjects. (e, f) Correlation plots between τ CSF levels and the number of Gd+ lesions (e) or T2 lesion volume in MS subjects (f). **p<0.01 vs control #p<0.05 vs Gd−MS. Gd+, Gadolinium enhancing; Gd−, non-Gadolinium enhancing.

PowerPoint slide

These results might indicate that acute brain inflammation is associated with alterations of amyloid-β metabolism, leading to decreased CSF detection of this biologically active peptide. We further explored this possibility by correlating both amyloid-β1–42 and τ protein levels with the number of Gd+ lesions seen at the MRI. These analyses uncovered a significant negative correlation between amyloid-β1−42 content and Gd+ lesions (n=22, r= −0.45, p=0.03) (Figure 2c), whereas no correlation was seen between amyloid-β1–42 and total lesion load in T2 MRI sequences (n=42, r=0.06, p=0.69) (Figure 2d). No correlation was also found between τ levels in the CSF of MS patients and both Gd+ lesions (n=22, r= −0.21, p=0.34) and T2 lesion load (n=42, r = −0.08, p=0.57) (Figures 2e and f).

Correlation Between TBS-Induced LTP with Amyloid-β1–42 and Total τ Levels in MS Patients

Amyloid-β dimers isolated from AD brains completely block LTP induction in the rodent hippocampus, and consequently cause cognitive defects (Shankar et al, 2008). In an attempt to see if also in MS brains amyloid-β1–42 is able to alter LTP induction, we explored whether any correlation could be uncovered between CSF content of amyloid-β1–42 and the magnitude of LTP induced in MS patients with TBS. A significant positive correlation was found between the magnitude of LTP measured 15 min after the TBS protocol and the levels of amyloid-β1–42 in the CSF (r=0.48, p=0.007), a finding that may suggest that also in MS, as in AD, reduced CSF contents of amyloid-β1–42 may be associated with defective memory-related synaptic plasticity (Figures 3a and b).

Figure 3
figure 3

TBS-induced LTP magnitude correlates with amyloid-β1–42 levels in the CSF of MS subjects. (a) Correlation plot between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and MEP amplitude changes, expressed as percentage of baseline, immediately after TBS, and (b) 15 min after TBS. (c, d) Correlation plots between τ CSF levels and TBS-induced LTP. LTP, long-term potentiation; TBS, θ burst stimulation.

PowerPoint slide

Correlation Between Neuropsychological Assessments and TBS-Induced LTP

To investigate whether cognitive impairment was associated with reduced cortical LTP in MS subjects, we compared the mean percentage values of peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes before and after TBS in CI and CP MS patients. A repeated-measures ANOVA with TIME (3 levels: baseline, 0, and 15 min post-TBS) as within subjects and GROUP (2 levels: CI and CP) as between subjects main factors showed a significant effect of time (F=8.04, p=0.002), a significant effect of GROUP (F=6.82, p=0.023) and a significant TIME × GROUP interaction (F=5.96, p=0.031). At post hoc contrasts CI patients showed a lower effect of TBS at both time points (0 min post-TBS: 1.09±0.14; 15 min post: 1.18±0.12; all p<0.05) compared with CP patients (0 min post-TBS: 1.67±0.16; 15 min post: 1.71±0.16) (Figure 4). We also performed a t-test for independent data on the peak effect of TBS represented by the post-TBS time point with the highest mean MEP amplitude. A significantly lower peak effect of TBS in CI (n=10, mean±SE=1.39±0.15) compared with CP (n=10, mean±SE=2.32±0.23) MS patients (p=0.008) was observed (not shown). These findings, showing a lower effect of TBS in CI patients, are in line with previous observations that rTMS has an altered effect in AD patients (Inghilleri et al, 2006; Battaglia et al, 2007) and show for the first time that cognitive impairment in MS patients is paralleled by an alteration of NMDAR-dependent synaptic plasticity. Comparison of TBS effect between CIS, RRMS, and PPMS did not show any significant difference.

Figure 4
figure 4

TBS-induced LTP differs between cognitively impaired and cognitively preserved MS patients. Differences between the two groups were observed at both 0 and 15 min after TBS time points. *p<0.05.

PowerPoint slide

Role of Amyloid-β1–42 in the Modulation of Excitatory and Inhibitory Synaptic Transmission in MS

LTP is a complex phenomenon, and changes in glutamate-mediated synaptic drive or GABA-mediated inhibition alter its induction. Accordingly, blockade of NMDA glutamate receptors prevents LTP (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Cooke and Bliss, 2006), as also does the enhancement of GABA-mediated inhibition (Matsuyama et al, 2008; Gong et al, 2009; Pan et al, 2009). Thus, we tried to investigate whether amyloid-β1–42-mediated inhibition of LTP could be secondary to impaired basal synaptic transmission in cortical neurons of MS patients. In ppTMS experiments, however, we found that the degree of SICI, ICF, SICF, and LICI was not related to the CSF levels of amyloid-β1–42 in MS patients, suggesting that this protein affects synaptic plasticity but not basal excitatory or inhibitory transmission (Figure 5). Comparisons between CI and CP MS patients also did not reveal any significant difference (not shown).

Figure 5
figure 5

Correlation plots between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and MEP amplitude changes, expressed as percentage of baseline, induced by (a) SICI at 2 ms ISI, (b) ICF at 10 ms ISI, (c) LICI at 100 ms ISI, (d) SICF at 1.5 ms ISI, and (e) at 2.7 ms ISI. MEP, motor-evoked potential; SICI, short-interval intracortical inhibition; ISI, interstimulus interval; ICF, intracortical facilitation; LICI, long-interval intracortical inhibition; SICF, short intracortical facilitation.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

Gray matter pathology, brain atrophy, and loss of anatomical connectivity are the most accredited determinants of progressive cognitive decline in MS (Calabrese et al, 2009, Calabrese et al, 2010; Staff et al, 2009; Tiemann et al, 2009). Acute and reversible mental deterioration also occurs during clinical relapses, independently of the primary functional system involved (Foong et al, 1998). The presence of Gd+ lesions at the brain MRI also correlates with poor cognitive performance in asymptomatic and symptomatic MS patients (Foong et al, 1998; Bellmann-Strobl et al, 2009), suggesting that focal inflammation is able to diffusely disrupt the neuronal substrate of information processing, leading to acute cognitive impairment. Given that amyloid-β1–42 metabolism is regulated by inflammation (Griffin et al, 2006; Hickman et al, 2008; Schmidt et al, 2008), a recent study performed in a large sample of subjects reported that in MS patients there is an overall decrease of CSF amyloid-β1–42 in comparison with healthy controls (Mattsson et al, 2009). These results seem to indicate that in MS there could be an altered expression of amyloid-β1–42, possibly related to the underlying inflammatory processes.

This study shows that during the inflammatory phase of MS, amyloid-β1–42 metabolism is altered leading to its reduced detection in the CSF. We found, in fact, that amyloid-β1–42 levels were significantly lower in subjects with Gd+ lesions at the MRI. Interestingly, these findings are in good agreement with previous studies showing that IL-1β, an important pro-inflammatory cytokine, alters amyloid-β metabolism in the cerebral cortex, increasing APP levels in rat brain homogenate (Fan et al, 2009). There is no information, conversely, about IL-1β effect on CSF amyloid-β1–42 levels. However, inflammation increases BACE activity (Hong et al, 2003), and BACE activity levels were found to be higher in patients with low amyloid β1–42 levels in the CSF (Mulder et al, 2010). Anti-inflammatory cytokines, on the other hand, enhance the activity of amyloid-β degrading enzymes, thus favoring amyloid-β tissue deposit clearance (Shimizu et al, 2008).

This study shows, for the first time, a difference in cognitive profile of MS patients related to CSF levels of amyloid-β1–42. Interestingly, the cognitive domains that seemed to be affected in association with low amyloid-β CSF levels were those regarding attention, concentration, and information-processing speed, the same influenced by the presence of radiological disease activity (Bellmann-Strobl et al, 2009). Gd enhancement has been associated with poor PASAT performance in otherwise physically stable MS patients (Bellmann-Strobl et al, 2009) and to CSF amyloid-β1–42 levels (this work), in line with the idea that CSF amyloid-β1–42 expression, altered by brain inflammation, may cause a diffuse impairment of cerebral connectivity with a negative impact on cognitive functioning. Conversely, neuropsychological test scores examining memory did not correlate with amyloid-β1–42 levels.

Cortical atrophy was not measured in this study as imaging was performed for clinical purposes, thus only conventional MRI scans were collected. However, CSF τ levels, already reported to be associated with neurodegenerative damage (Vemuri et al, 2009; Ghoshal et al, 2002) were found to be normal in our patients. A further limitation of this study may be represented by anxiety, because of diagnosis communication or expectancy for the disease. Anxiety is known to influence the performance in the neuropsychological tests (Peretti, 1998; Tombaugh, 2006), especially those addressing concentration and attention. However, all participants were diagnosed as suffering from CIS or MS during hospitalization, and diagnosis was communicated after all data collection.

The synaptotoxic properties of amyloid-β1–42 have long been recognized, as well as its ability to interfere with LTP and with cognitive abilities (Klyubin et al, 2005; Battaglia et al, 2007; Shankar et al, 2008; Nygaard and Strittmatter, 2009; Walther et al, 2009; Townsend et al, 2010). A recent study reported that amyloid-β dimers isolated from AD patients can impair hippocampal LTP and memory in mice and induce dendritic spine retraction in neurons (Shankar et al, 2008). Of note, although the role of amyloid-β in LTP disruption has been shown in a variety of experimental models of AD (Klyubin et al, 2005; Battaglia et al, 2007; Shankar et al, 2008; Walther et al, 2009; Townsend et al, 2010), its correlation with LTP measured in humans have never been shown before. Thus, the correlation between amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels and LTP amplitude reported in this study is a first indication that amyloid-β is a potent regulator of synaptic plasticity also in the human brain, with potentially relevant consequences for understanding the mechanisms of cognitive deficits not only in MS subjects but also in other neurological disorders, obviously including AD. Importantly, although both altered cognitive performances and LTP induction was found in mice overexpressing τ protein (Polydoro et al, 2009), τ levels did not correlate with TBS-induced LTP amplitude in MS patients (Figures 3c and d). In this respect, it is noteworthy that only LTP induced by high-frequency stimulation but not that induced by TBS was altered in mice overexpressing τ (Polydoro et al, 2009).

Previous investigations found that in MS patients SICI was lower in secondary progressive MS (SPMS) in comparison with RRMS patients in the remitting phase, and that SICI correlated significantly with EDSS scores (Conte et al, 2009). These results are in apparent contrast with our findings. However, our study group did not include patients with SPMS, and the mean age and disease duration of these patients (age: 51±0, disease duration: 21.0±2) are also very different in comparison with our study group (age: 34.3±1.4 years, disease duration: 2.7±0.8 years). Other investigators found that fatigue in MS is associated with lower SICI compared with MS patients without fatigue and healthy subjects (Liepert et al, 2005). In our study group, only very few patients complained fatigue and also in this case our group differed in terms of mean age (41.4±4.75 years). Moreover, one of the inclusion criteria in the study from Liepert was that patients had to be in the remitting phase of the disease, thus presumably also had longer disease durations. Finally, a similar reduction of SICI was found in the relapsing phase of the disease in comparison with remitting patients in a study by Caramia et al (2004). Moreover, in this case, the study group had longer disease duration (mean 4.29±3.26 years), included only patients with a definite diagnosis of RRMS. Thus, differences in the results may be explained by the different clinical characteristics of the samples studied.

A recent investigation based on a different TMS protocol to induce neuroplastic changes, suggested that motor plasticity in MS patients is comparable with that found in healthy subjects (Zeller et al, 2010). Neuroplasticity in this study was, however, assessed only in patients in the remitting phase of the disease, with a stable clinical condition, whereas the alterations observed in our study were evident in patients with cognitive impairment and the presence of Gd+ lesions.

The reduction in CSF amyloid-β1–42 in AD has been postulated to reflect deposition of the aggregated insoluble fibrillar amyloid-β peptides in senile plaque, with lower levels of diffusion into the CSF. A limitation of this study is that we do not provide any evidence of amyloid-β tissue deposition in our patients. We cannot argue, therefore, that the decrease in amyloid-β1–42 CSF levels observed is consequent to tissue deposition. However, AD neuropathology apart from amyloid-β deposition includes variable amounts of soluble species that show much stronger correlations with severity of dementia than fibrillar amyloid-β (Terry, 1996; Lue et al, 1999; McLean et al, 1999; Gandy et al, 2010). Synthetic amyloid-β dimers are extremely potent inhibitors of synaptic plasticity both in vivo and in vitro, whereas even relatively high concentrations of amyloid-β monomers are inactive (Selkoe, 2008; Hu et al, 2008; Shankar et al, 2008). Moreover, human ex vivo samples of CSF that contain amyloid-β oligomers but not monomers potently inhibit LTP (Klyubin et al, 2005). The lowering of natively measured amyloid-β1–42 in the CSF of our patients may be consequent to oligomerization as observed in AD (Englund et al, 2009). Finally, a transgenic mice overexpressing APPE693Q develops learning and memory deficits in association with the formation of amyloid-β oligomers without generating amyloid plaques in their brains at any age (Gandy et al, 2010). These observations, even though only at a speculative stage, could help explaining the findings of our iTBS experiments. Another possibility is that the observed findings could be related to inflammation-induced downregulation of β-site APP cleavage. Accordingly, BACE activity is modulated by inflammation (Mattsson et al, 2009) and is also involved in the promotion of myelination by oligodendrocytes (Hu et al, 2006).

Nevertheless, the mechanism by which amyloid-β1–42 is reduced during inflammation in MS remains unclear, and further studies are needed to better clarify this issue.