INTRODUCTION

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is highly expressed in the brain and is implicated in several neural processes, including neurogenesis, differentiation, survival, and synaptic plasticity. Animal models and human studies indicate that BDNF secretion is activity dependent: Cognitive stimulation, physical activity, and antidepressant treatment increase BDNF secretion, whereas stress and mood disorders decrease it (Chen et al, 2001; Cotman and Berchtold, 2002; Martinowich and Lu, 2008; Mattson et al, 2004; Sen et al, 2008). There is also considerable evidence that the genetic variant Val66Met (rs6265) influences the BDNF system. This single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) results in a Val to Met substitution. The less-frequent Met variant has been associated with impaired memory and hippocampal function (Egan et al, 2003; Matsuo et al, 2009), smaller hippocampal volume (Bueller et al, 2006), and, in transgenic knock-in mice, defective BDNF secretion and increased anxiety-related behaviors (Chen et al, 2006).

The BDNF Val66Met has been studied extensively as a candidate gene for mood disorders and schizophrenia, but the results are mixed (Gratacos et al, 2007; Green et al, 2006; Muglia et al, 2008; Rybakowski, 2008; Sklar et al, 2002). Several studies (Hunnerkopf et al, 2007; Itoh et al, 2004; Lang et al, 2005; Sen et al, 2003; Tochigi et al, 2006; Willis-Owen et al, 2005a; Wray et al, 2008) have also examined the association between BDNF and the personality trait neuroticism, a major risk factor for depression, anxiety, and other psychiatric disorders (Bienvenu et al, 2004; Clark et al, 1994; Jylha et al, 2009; Kendler et al, 2006; Watson et al, 2005). A recent meta-analysis concluded that the less-frequent Met variant is associated with lower neuroticism (Frustaci et al, 2008), an unexpected finding, given the unfavorable effects of the Met variant (eg, abnormal hippocampal structure and function). However, a large study (Willis-Owen et al, 2005a) and two recent genome-wide association (GWA) scans (Terracciano et al, 2008; van den Oord et al, 2008) found no association between Val66Met and neuroticism. In light of these results, it remains an open question whether Val66Met has a direct effect on neuroticism.

Interestingly, the only published GWA study of extraversion reported that Met carriers were more introverted (Terracciano et al, 2008). This association was not seen in previous studies (Itoh et al, 2004; Sen et al, 2003; Tochigi et al, 2006), perhaps because of their small sample sizes (N<450). Owing to its relation to emotional processes, an association between BDNF and extraversion is of considerable interest. Extraversion is inversely and weakly associated with the experience of negative affect, but has a pervasive effect on positive emotions and life satisfaction (Costa and McCrae, 1980; Watson and Clark, 1992). Introverts are more likely to experience anhedonia and dysthymic/depressed mood (Clark et al, 1994; Jylha et al, 2009; Watson et al, 2005), important clinical features of depressive disorders.

The present research has two major goals. The first goal is to further test the association between BDNF Val66Met and both neuroticism and extraversion in samples from the United States and Italy. To provide a quantitative summary of the association between BDNF and neuroticism, we conducted a meta-analysis combining this study with the published literature.

The second goal of this study was to test the hypothesis that BDNF Val66Met and a polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) interact to influence neuroticism scores. This hypothesis derives from biological evidence that BDNF functions as a synaptic modulator that regulates multiple neurotransmitter systems, including serotonin, dopamine, and glutamate (Carvalho et al, 2008; Hunnerkopf et al, 2007; Mossner et al, 2000). In particular, BDNF stimulates the sprouting and morphological differentiation of serotonin neurons and enhances their survival (Mamounas et al, 1995; Martinowich and Lu, 2008; Mattson et al, 2004). Infusion of BDNF into the brain increases activity in monoaminergic systems, whereas depletion of serotonin downregulates BDNF mRNA (Martinowich and Lu, 2008; Zhou et al, 2008). The interdependence of the BDNF and serotonin systems is also supported by findings that BDNF has antidepressant-like effects (Siuciak et al, 1996). Chronic antidepressant treatment, including selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), upregulates BDNF expression (Nibuya et al, 1995).

The interaction of serotonin and BDNF systems has generated hypotheses of genetic epistasis, and recent studies have reported interactions between BDNF Val66Met and 5-HTTLPR. In these studies, the outcomes of epistatic effects were measures of depressive symptoms in the presence of environmental adversity (Kim et al, 2007; Wichers et al, 2008), differences in mood-related brain structures (Pezawas et al, 2008), or response to mood stabilizers (Rybakowski et al, 2007). In this study, we examine the interaction between the serotonin transporter and BDNF genes in the largest sample tested so far on a depression-related phenotype.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Sample Description: SardiNIA

SardiNIA is a study conducted in Sardinia, Italy, supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA). The study has been described in detail elsewhere (Costa et al, 2007; Pilia et al, 2006; Terracciano et al, 2008; Terracciano et al, 2009b). We recruited about 62% of the population aged 14–102 years (n=6148) from a cluster of four towns in the Lanusei Valley, Sardinia. As part of a founder population, the recruited individuals are highly interrelated and most have common ancestors from the same isolated region. Valid personality data were obtained from 5693 subjects at their first assessment, of which 3972 were genotyped as part of a GWA study (Terracciano et al, 2008). Here, we report association analyses for 1560 individuals who are a part of the SardiNIA cohort, but were not included in the original GWA analyses (Terracciano et al, 2008). We refer to this sample as SardiNIA 2. In this sample, age ranged from 14 to 94 years (M=42.7; SD=17), with 59.9% women.

To examine the interaction between BDNF Val66Met and 5-HTTLPR, we combined the two SardiNIA samples. We included individuals who were genotyped for both polymorphisms (as part of the GWA study or SardiNIA 2) and assessed for personality traits (n=2333). In this sample, age ranged from 14 to 94 years (M=45.8; SD=17), with 57% women. The SardiNIA project was approved by institutional review boards in Italy and the United States, and research was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki Principles and other international guidelines.

Sample Description: BLSA

The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) is an ongoing multidisciplinary study of community-dwelling volunteers. Here, we examine data from 1131 subjects who were successfully genotyped for Val66Met and completed the personality questionnaire at least once. In this sample, age at first assessment ranged from 20 to 93 years (M=57.3; SD=15.5), with 48% women. About 74% of the participants were of European descent, 25% African American, and 1% were Asian American. Personality traits were assessed from 1989 to 2009, and multiple assessments were available for 86% of the participants. Although personality traits are generally stable over time (Terracciano et al, 2006, in press), to provide more robust estimates, we used the average across all available assessments. These average scores are likely to provide a more reliable estimate of the individuals' personality profile. When multiple assessments were available, average age was used as covariate. The sample included a similar number of men and women with multiple assessments. The BLSA study was approved by the local institutional review board, and research was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki Principles and other international guidelines.

Personality Assessment

Personality traits were assessed using the English (Costa and McCrae, 1992) and Italian (Terracciano, 2003) versions of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R). The questionnaire consists of 240 items answered on a 5-point Likert scale, from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Scores followed a normal distribution and were standardized (M=50, SD=10) using American combined gender norms (Costa and McCrae, 1992).

The NEO-PI-R has a robust factor structure that has been replicated in Italy (Terracciano, 2003) and in more than 50 cultures (McCrae and Terracciano, 2005a), even at the genetic level (Costa et al, 2007; Pilia et al, 2006). In both samples, the NEO-PI-R showed good psychometric properties: internal consistency reliabilities for the five factors were at or above 0.80. In the BLSA, available longitudinal data indicate that stability coefficients for the five factors are in the range of 0.80 over intervals of 10 years (Terracciano et al, 2006). In addition, twin studies have shown that both neuroticism and extraversion are heritable, suggesting a genetic basis for these two traits (Bouchard and Loehlin, 2001). In the SardiNIA sample, family-based estimates of broad heritability were 0.44 for neuroticism and 0.39 for extraversion (Pilia et al, 2006).

Genetic Assays

The BDNF SNP rs6265 was genotyped using an Affymetrix custom chip in the SardiNIA 2 sample, and with the 550k Illumina platform in the BLSA sample. Genotype data from both samples passed quality controls, including Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium. In the SardiNIA 2 sample, genotype frequencies were 45.3% for GG (Val/Val), 44.5% for AG (Val/Met), and 10.2% for AA (Met/Met). The frequency of the G (Val) allele was 68%, which is lower than the frequencies found in other European populations (81%). In the BLSA sample, genotype frequencies were 68.1% for GG (Val/Val), 28.6% for AG (Val/Met), and 3.3% for AA (Met/Met). The frequency of the G (Val) allele was 82%.

As described elsewhere (Terracciano et al, 2009a), in the SardiNIA sample, 5-HTTLPR genotypes were determined using the MegaBACE 1000 fluorescence-based genotyping methodology. PCR was successful in >95% of the samples, and there were no assay-specific problems with the MegaBACE 1000 automatic DNA sequencer. In the samples that passed quality controls, allele frequencies were 49% for the short (S) allele and 51% for the long (L) allele, and genotype frequencies were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (P>0.05). Some evidence suggests that the effect of the 5-HTTLPR genotype on the serotonin transporter gene expression is further influenced by a polymorphism of the 5-HTTLPR long allele (rs25531), but we did not genotype this variant. However, several studies that genotyped the rs25531 polymorphism have found no association with neuroticism, anxiety, depression, or obsessive compulsive disorder (Gunthert et al, 2007; Wachleski et al, 2008; Wendland et al, 2007; Wray et al, 2009).

Statistical Analyses

To take advantage of the family information in the SardiNIA sample, we used the program MERLIN (Chen and Abecasis, 2007) to perform association analyses between Val66Met and personality traits. In MERLIN, regression coefficients are estimated in the context of a variance component model to account for relatedness among individuals (Chen and Abecasis, 2007). The same analyses were carried out for BLSA. Sex, age, and age2 were included as covariates in all analyses to account for sex differences and the linear (age) and curvilinear (age2) effects of age on personality traits (Costa et al, 2001; Terracciano et al, 2005).

Although it is difficult to account for the family structure and more complex genetic effects in power calculations, on the basis of a simulation of statistical power performed elsewhere (Willis-Owen et al, 2005a), both samples should have adequate power (>80%) to detect the main genetic effect observed in the original SardiNIA GWA study.

In the SardiNIA sample, we checked the genomic control value for our GWA analyses (Devlin and Roeder, 1999), and carried out a principal component analysis of genome-wide SNP data in a subset of unrelated individuals (Price et al, 2006). Neither analysis suggested evidence of population substructure nor did genetic outliers in this sample. To account for the multiethnic composition of the BLSA sample, we included eigenvalues derived from the full GWA data set (Price et al, 2006) as covariates and we repeated the analyses restricting them to the European-American subsample.

Regression analyses were carried out to examine the interaction effect (SPSS 13.0). A first set of analyses examined interactions of all Val66Met (Val/Val, Val/Met, and Met/Met) and 5-HTTLPR (SS, SL, and LL) genotypes. Following previous studies (Pezawas et al, 2008; Rybakowski et al, 2007; Terracciano et al, 2009a), we also contrasted the Val/Val genotype with any-Met genotypes, and LL with any-S genotypes. The regressions for the three- and two-way interactions controlled for sex, age, and age2. Interaction effects were further evaluated by MERLIN association analyses of BDNF with neuroticism separately for the 5-HTTLPR LL and any-S genotype groups.

Meta-Analysis

A recent meta-analysis reported an association between neuroticism and the BDNF Val66Met variant (Frustaci et al, 2008). We update this meta-analysis by adding data from the samples in this study and from the previous GWA study (Terracciano et al, 2008). We searched (up to 20 July 2009) for additional articles through the PubMed and Scopus search engines using the following keywords: ‘Neuroticism,’ ‘BDNF,’ ‘brain derived neurotrophic factor,’ ‘rs6265,’ and ‘Val66Met.’ We also examined references and citations of relevant studies. We included all samples genotyped for Val66Met and assessed with a measure of neuroticism. Surprisingly, the largest study we found (Willis-Owen et al, 2005a) was published in 2005, but was not included in the 2008 meta-analysis (Frustaci et al, 2008). We carried out the meta-analysis, examining fixed and random effect models, using the program Comprehensive Meta Analysis (version 2). In line with previous studies (Gatt et al, 2009; Pezawas et al, 2008; Rybakowski et al, 2007; Sen et al, 2003), given the small number of Met/Met genotypes among European populations, we contrasted the standardized mean difference on neuroticism between the Val/Val genotype vs the combined Val/Met and Met/Met genotypes.

RESULTS

Table 1 presents neuroticism and extraversion scores by the BDNF Val66Met (rs6265) genotype for the SardiNIA and BLSA samples. Consistent with the results of the GWA scan (Terracciano et al, 2008), individuals with the BDNF Met variant scored significantly lower on extraversion in both the SardiNIA (allele: G; β=0.093; p=0.005) and BLSA (allele: G; β=0.114; p=0.026) samples. The association was slightly stronger (allele: G; β=0.153; p=0.008) in the BLSA sample when analyses were restricted to participants of European ancestry (n=839). In both samples, the Val/Met group tends to score roughly 0.2 SD lower on extraversion compared with the Val/Val group.

Table 1 Neuroticism and Extraversion Scores by BDNF Val66Met Genotypes

We found no association between Val66Met genotypes and neuroticism in either SardiNIA (p=0.19) or BLSA (p=0.94) samples. Because these results are inconsistent with the previous meta-analysis based on 5 studies and 1633 subjects (Frustaci et al, 2008), we carried out a new meta-analysis combining data from the published literature to date and this study, for a total of 13 samples and 15 251 subjects. Figure 1 presents a forest plot of standardized mean differences on neuroticism between Val/Val and Val/Met + Met/Met BDNF genotypes. Apart from the few small studies that show trends in either direction, the largest studies show no evidence of an association between neuroticism and BDNF Val66Met. There was no significant heterogeneity (Q=16.9; p>0.05), and the pooled effect sizes using the fixed- and random-effects model were 0.01 (95% CI: −0.02 to 0.05; p=0.39) and 0.02 (95% CI: −0.03 to 0.06; p=0.51).

Figure 1
figure 1

Forest plot of studies examining the association between neuroticism and the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism. Met/Met and Val/Met were contrasted with the Val/Val genotypes. SMD, standardized mean differences.

Interaction of BDNF and 5-HTTLPR

Recent studies have reported interactions between Val66Met and 5-HTTLPR on depression-related phenotypes (Pezawas et al, 2008; Rybakowski et al, 2007; Wichers et al, 2008). We tested for such an interaction on neuroticism, a major risk factor for mood disorders. We first examined the interaction of the SS, SL, and LL 5-HTTLPR genotypes with Val/Val, Val/Met, and Met/Met BDNF variants. Consistent with previous studies, we also repeated the analyses combining the genotype groups with any S allele of the 5-HTTLPR and the genotype groups with any Met variant of BDNF. When the three genotypes of the two genes were examined, the interaction term was not significant. However, when a 2 × 2 interaction was tested, we observed a significant effect (p<0.05). Figure 2 presents neuroticism scores stratified by BDNF and 5-HTT variants. Among individuals with LL 5-HTTLPR, BDNF Met carriers scored higher on neuroticism compared with those with the Val/Val BDNF genotype (allele: G; β=−0.169; p=0.005). Among those with the S 5-HTTLPR allele, no differences were observed among BDNF genotypes (allele: G; β=0.00; p=0.98). For extraversion, we found no statistically significant interaction between BDNF and 5-HTTLPR (β=−0.029; p=0.52).

Figure 2
figure 2

Interaction of BDNF Val66Met and 5-HTTLPR on NEO-PI-R neuroticism scores in the SardiNIA sample (N=2333). Among the carriers of the 5-HTTLPR LL genotype, individuals with one or two copies of the BDNF Met variant tend to score higher on neuroticism (p=0.005). Among individuals with the 5-HTTLPR S variant, there are no significant differences in neuroticism scores between BDNF genotypes (p=0.98).

DISCUSSION

This study tested whether the BDNF Val66Met variant had a main effect on extraversion and neuroticism, and whether the Val66Met variant interacted with 5-HTTLPR to influence neuroticism scores. Consistent with a relatively large GWA study in the SardiNIA cohort (Terracciano et al, 2008), we found an association between BDNF and extraversion in a replication sample from SardiNIA and one from the United States. The effects were small but consistent across samples, with the Met carriers scoring roughly 0.2 SD lower on extraversion compared with individuals with the Val/Val genotype. Previous research has shown that the BDNF Met variant is linked to poor intracellular trafficking, decreased BDNF secretion, and abnormal hippocampus structure and function (Bueller et al, 2006; Egan et al, 2003). To this literature we add that Met carriers are also more likely to be introverted. This is of interest from a clinical perspective, as introverts tend to be socially withdrawn and more often experience anhedonia and dysthymic/depressed mood (Bienvenu et al, 2004; Clark et al, 1994; Jylha et al, 2009; Watson et al, 2005). These findings can provide new insights into animal and clinical studies that examine the effects of BDNF on mood, cognition, and behaviors.

The association between introversion and the Met variant is consistent with the idea that genetic factors contribute to the personality differences observed across populations. Indeed, cross-cultural studies of personality have consistently found that individuals from Asian populations tend to score slightly lower on extraversion compared with individuals of European ancestry (McCrae and Terracciano, 2005b; McCrae et al, in press; Schmitt et al, 2007). The rs6265 (Val66Met) allele frequencies differ across populations, with the A (Met) allele estimated to be about 20% among Europeans but about 40% among Asians (Itoh et al, 2004; Tochigi et al, 2006). As such, the higher frequency of the A (Met) allele among Asian populations may contribute to their lower scores on extraversion. Of course, cultural factors underlie these cross-cultural differences, but if confirmed in additional large samples, this association could be, to our knowledge, the first ascertained genetic variant that contributes to personality differences across populations.

We found no evidence of an association between BDNF Val66Met and neuroticism in two independent samples and in the meta-analysis inclusive of the published literature (see Figure 2). Although there is no evidence for a main effect on neuroticism, the effect of BDNF may depend, in part, on interactions with either environmental factors (gene × environment) or other genetic variants (gene × gene).

Animal models and clinical studies suggest that BDNF and the serotonin system are interdependent (Henningsson et al, 2009; Martinowich and Lu, 2008; Mattson et al, 2004), which has generated the hypothesis that the two genes interact (Pezawas et al, 2008; Rybakowski et al, 2007; Wichers et al, 2008). We tested for epistatic effects of Val66Met and 5-HTTLPR on neuroticism using the largest sample examined so far for depression-related outcomes. Despite the lack of main effects for BDNF Val66Met (see Figure 1) and 5-HTTLPR on neuroticism (Munafo et al, 2008; Terracciano et al, 2009a; Willis-Owen et al, 2005b), we found an interaction between these two genetic variants. Among the carriers of the 5-HTTLPR LL genotype, individuals with the BDNF Met variant scored higher on neuroticism compared with those with the Val variant. No differences were observed among 5-HTTLPR S carriers. This effect differs from those reported in previous smaller studies (Pezawas et al, 2008; Rybakowski et al, 2007; Wichers et al, 2008). Nevertheless, higher scores on neuroticism seem consistent with a synergistic effect of the BDNF Met variant, which reduces BDNF secretion and leads to anxiety-related behaviors in mice (Chen et al, 2006; Egan et al, 2003; Matsuo et al, 2009), and with the 5-HTTLPR LL genotype, which is associated with a higher rate of serotonin re-uptake from the synaptic cleft and supposedly to serotonin depletion (Arbelle et al, 2003; Lesch et al, 1996; Terracciano et al, 2009a). Given our promising findings and the intuitive appeal of an interaction between variants that reduce the circulating level of BDNF and serotonin, larger studies should attempt to confirm this epistatic effect on this major risk factor for depression.

This study advances our understanding of the influence of BDNF and 5-HTT on neuroticism and extraversion, the two personality dimensions with the strongest links to emotional processing. Identifying the genetic underpinnings of neuroticism and extraversion can help elucidate the biological pathways that contribute to psychiatric disorders with strong affective components.