INTRODUCTION

Ecstasy (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, MDMA, XTC) is a popular recreational drug, despite the fact that there is considerable concern about its neurotoxic potential. Studies in animals have shown deleterious effects of MDMA on the serotonin system, indicated by serotonergic axonal injury, decreases in the number of central serotonin transporters (SERT), and a significant depletion of serotonin in various cortical and subcortical regions (for review see Ricaurte et al, 2000; Green et al, 2003). On the basis of these animal data, it is likely that ecstasy might damage serotonin neurons in the human brain too. In humans, the literature on the potential neurotoxicity of ecstasy is extensive and complex and it is beyond the scope of this paper to discuss in detail the methods available for assessing the neurotoxic effects of ecstasy on the living human brain or differences in results across studies in different populations (for review see Parrott, 2001; Reneman, 2003; Morton, 2005). In general, however, neuroimaging studies (PET, SPECT) provide evidence for sustained serotonergic dysfunction in people who heavily use ecstasy, whereas the dopaminergic system appears to be unaffected (McCann et al, 1998, 2005; Kish, 2002; Buchert et al, 2004; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank and Daumann, 2006a, 2006b; Parrott, 2006; Reneman et al, 2001, 2006). Whether the alterations in serotonergic function are permanent is not yet clear. Some studies suggest that for instance reductions in SERT, a marker of serotonergic neurotoxicity, are transient in most but not all brain regions, as reversibility was shown in former ecstasy users (Semple et al, 1999; Reneman et al, 2001; Buchert et al, 2004).

Cognitive consequences of ecstasy use have been examined more extensively. Numerous cross-sectional studies reported impairments of learning and memory in moderate to heavy recreational users (for review see Morgan, 2000; Parrott, 2000; Verbaten, 2003). However, little is known about the effects of ecstasy on the neural systems involved in cognition. The few available functional MRI studies have primarily focused on working memory and generated inconclusive results concerning the effects of ecstasy on brain activity patterns and the specific brain areas that are affected (Daumann et al, 2003, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c, 2004d; Jacobsen et al, 2004; Moeller et al, 2004). One study investigated episodic memory function and suggested that memory deficits in ecstasy users arise from a hippocampal dysfunction (Daumann et al, 2005). Summarizing, there is some evidence of effects of ecstasy use on neurocognitive brain function, but this issue is clearly in need of further investigation.

Research in this area suffers from methodological problems for which there are no easy solutions (Turner and Parrott, 2000). Most human studies contend with interference of many potential confounders, such as polysubstance use and large heterogeneity in recreational ecstasy users (Gouzoulis-Mayfrank and Daumann, 2006a). Experienced ecstasy users not only consume more ecstasy than novice users but they are also more likely to consume other drugs such as cannabis, amphetamine, cocaine, LSD, and psilocybin (mushrooms) (Scholey et al, 2004). Consequently, cumulative ecstasy use almost invariably correlates highly with use of other drugs, making it almost impossible to differentiate between effects of ecstasy and of the other drugs. Indeed, several studies have indicated that signs of neurotoxicity in ecstasy users may well be associated with polydrug use in general or with the use of other drugs, such as cannabis and amphetamine (Daumann et al, 2004a). In some studies, an effort was made to control for the use of these other drugs, either by including a group of ‘pure’ ecstasy users (Halpern et al, 2004), a group of ecstasy-naive polysubstance users (Rodgers, 2000), or by statistically adjusting for polydrug use (Medina et al, 2005). Although statistical techniques help to separate the effects of ecstasy and of other drugs to some extent, they generally do not suffice if subjects are recruited at random, given the fact that ecstasy use almost invariably is associated with polydrug use.

The present study aims to clarify the specific effects of ecstasy on neurocognitive brain function, that is, effects that can be ascribed to ecstasy and not to the co-occurring use of other drugs. For this purpose, a large sample (n=71) with substantial variation in the amount and type of drug used was carefully composed. Multiple regression analysis with ecstasy and other drugs as separate regressors was applied to investigate the specific effects of ecstasy on working memory, selective attention, and associative memory, measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). We hypothesized that if deficits in memory or attention and/or related brain function would surface, ecstasy use would be the primary but not the only factor accounting for these abnormalities. We expected that amphetamines, cannabis, and/or cocaine would contribute to possible neurocognitive deficits.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

This study is part of the Netherlands XTC Toxicity (NeXT) study. A detailed description of the design and objectives of the NeXT study is provided in a paper on the methods (De Win et al, 2005). Besides fMRI, subjects underwent SPECT and MR imaging and cognitive testing; results of these measurements will be reported in separate publications (De Win et al, 2007; Schilt et al, in press). In two previous papers, we reported fMRI findings in a part of the non-ecstasy using subjects (n=27) on the effects of cannabis on cognitive brain function (Jager et al, 2006, 2007).

Subjects

A total of 71 subjects (44 male, 27 female; mean age 23 years (SD 3.8, range 18–37) were included based on specific variations in the amount and type of drugs they were using and with the objective to keep correlations between the drugs that were used as low as possible. Potential candidates interested to participate in the study were asked to fill out a questionnaire on their drug use, but were blind to the inclusion criteria. Besides the typical heavy polysubstance ecstasy users, preference was given to candidates who were either ‘selective ecstasy users’ (100 ecstasy pills or more lifetime, but no or almost no use of other drugs except for cannabis) or ‘polydrug-but-no-ecstasy users’ (extensive experience with amphetamine and/or cocaine, but (almost) no ecstasy use, that is <10 pills lifetime). Eventually, the sample included 33 heavy ecstasy users and 38 non-ecstasy users, with both ecstasy users and non-ecstasy users showing considerable variation in type and amount of other drugs taken, for example, some heavy ecstasy users reported minimal use of other drugs such as cannabis, amphetamine, or cocaine, whereas others were moderate or frequent users of one or more other psychoactive drug.

All subjects were right-handed and were excluded if they reported: major medical, neurological, or psychiatric disorders; current use of psychotropic medications; use of intravenous drugs; pregnancy; and contra-indications for MRI. Except for smoking, which was allowed until 2 h before scanning, subjects had to abstain from use of all psychoactive drugs for at least 2 weeks and from alcohol for at least 1 week before examinations. Compliance to abstinence was checked with urine drug screening (enzyme-multiplied immunoassay for amphetamines, ecstasy, opiates, cocaine, benzodiazepines, cannabis, and alcohol). Hair samples were collected for drug hair analysis on previous ecstasy use (gas chromatography/mass spectroscopy).

Subjects were paid for their participation (€ 150 for 2 days) and gave their written consent according to the Helsinki Declaration. The local medical ethics committee approved the study.

Procedure

All 71 subjects completed validated self-report questionnaires about their drug use (Van den Wijngaart et al, 1997). They were screened for axis I psychiatric disorders using the Dutch version of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for DSM-IV clinical disorders (Sheehan et al, 1998). Urine samples were collected on the day of scanning and verbal intelligence was estimated using the Dutch version of the National Adult Reading Test (Schmand et al, 1991).

Assessment of Working Memory, Selective Attention, and Associative Memory

Three fMRI tasks were administered: a modified Sternberg item recognition task (denoted STERN), a visuo-auditory selective attention task (SAT), and a pictorial associative memory task that depends on (para)hippocampal brain function (PMT). The STERN task has the following basic format: a set of five consonants is shown for 5340 ms (the target-set). After this, a series of 10 consonants is displayed in sequence (Figure 1). Subjects are instructed to memorize the target-set, and subsequently press a button as quickly as possible when a consonant belongs to the target-set (50% were targets). Two experimental tasks were administered, which differed only with regard to the target-set(s): a novel set and a practiced set. In the practiced set task (PT), the same set was used repeatedly. In the novel set task (NT), the composition of the target-set was changed after every run of 10 trials. An additional reaction time control task (CT) was included, as well as rest periods of equal epoch duration (for further details on the STERN task see Jansma et al, 2001; Ramsey et al, 2004).

Figure 1
figure 1

The temporal sequence of events is shown for the STERN task. Each epoch starts with presentation of the target-set, and is followed by ten trials. Subjects have to press a button as fast as possible, if the letter belongs to the target-set.

The second task SAT is a visuo-auditory oddball detection task. It involves detection of tones deviant in pitch from a baseline tone, and similarly, detection of dots deviant in size from a baseline dot (Figure 2). A threshold for detecting differences in pitch and dot-size was determined individually before the scan session by adjusting it until the subject detected 80% of the deviant stimuli. Tones and dots were presented simultaneously in epochs of 29 s each. Epochs differed only with regard to the task instruction. At the start of each epoch, subjects were instructed to attend either to the tones while ignoring the dots (TO), or vice versa (DO) (for details see Jager et al, 2006).

Figure 2
figure 2

The temporal sequence of events is shown for the SAT task. Each epoch (duration 29 s) starts with an instruction slide (5 s), indicating ‘rest’, ‘attend to the tones only’, or ‘attend to the dots only’. Both during ‘tones only’ and ‘dots only’, the instruction slide is followed by a series of 25 stimuli (simultaneous asynchronous presentation of tones and dots at a variable interstimulus interval rate) of which on average 20% deviant (targets). In case of a target, subjects have to press a button as fast as possible. Before fMRI scanning, the difference between standard and deviant tones and dots is determined for each individual by changing the contrast until a performance of 80% correct is obtained.

The PMT task is a pictorial associative memory task, modified from a task paradigm from Henke et al (1997) and involves three task conditions. First, an associative learning phase (AL) is conducted which requires subjects to remember a specific combination of pictures and to establish a meaningful connection between the two pictures. In the next phase, single item pictures have to be classified (SC), which serves as a control task. Finally, in the retrieval phase (RE), subjects have to recognize specific combinations previously presented during associative learning. The RE-task condition enables to measure recall accuracy, which serves as an indirect measure for adequate associative learning during the AL phase. In healthy volunteers, this task reliably reveals brain activity in the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus bilaterally, especially during the AL condition (Henke et al, 1997). Figure 3 depicts a schematic example of the PMT task. In the scanner, each picture, containing two color photographs on a white background, was presented for 5000 ms, followed by a 2340 ms fixation cross. Each task condition was presented in four epochs of eight stimuli (picture+fixation cross). Also, four rest periods (RS) were included of equal epoch duration. For further details on the PMT task, we refer to Jager et al (2007).

Figure 3
figure 3

The temporal sequence of events is shown for the PMT task. Each epoch starts with an instruction slide (5 s) followed by a fixation cross (2.5 s). This is followed by eight trials of 7.5 s each (picture pair 5 s, fixation cross 2.5 s). Subjects have to respond to the task by pressing one out of two buttons, according to the instruction in each task condition.

Scans were made on a clinical Philips ACS-NT 1.5 Tesla MR-scanner with PT 6000 gradients, using a standard scan protocol (navigated 3D PRESTO; Ramsey et al, 1998). Voxel size was 4 mm isotropic. For further details on the scan procedure and scan parameters, we refer to Jansma et al (2001), Ramsey et al (2004) and Jager et al (2006, 2007).

Dependent and Independent Variables

Drug use (independent variables)

Table 1 gives a description of the drug use history of the sample. Use of GHB (liquid ecstasy), LSD, psilocybin, and laughing gas were less commonly reported and cumulative dosages were much lower than self-reported lifetime use of ecstasy (number of tablets), cannabis (number of joints), amphetamine and cocaine (number of occasions), and alcohol and tobacco consumption (drinks/week vs cigarettes/week). Therefore, only ecstasy, amphetamine, cocaine, alcohol, and tobacco were used as independent variables.

Table 1 Demographic Features and Drug Use Characteristics

Self-report histories may be inaccurate and in addition, there is imprecision arising from variation in drug content in ecstasy tablets. As a result, inaccuracies in dosage calculations are likely to undermine the validity of dose–response measures (Bedi and Redman, 2006). Also, drug use variables were far from normally distributed, even after log-transformation. Therefore, drug use variables were dichotomized using cutoff scores, which were fixed to balance the distribution of users and non-users of a particular drug within the sample. For ecstasy, amphetamines and cocaine the cutoff score was >10 tablets/occasions lifetime. For cannabis, the cutoff score was set somewhat higher (>50 joints lifetime), because experimenting with cannabis without becoming a frequent and long-term user is much more common than with the other illicit drugs. To illustrate this point, about half of the sample reported no use at all of ecstasy, amphetamines or cocaine, whereas only eight subjects reported no use at all of cannabis (see Table 2 for a description of the sample after drug use variables were dichotomized).

Table 2 Description Sample After the Drug Use Variables were Dichotomised

Performance data (dependent variables)

Outcome measures included performance accuracy (for STERN, SAT, and PMT) and reaction times (STERN only). Task performance during STERN was measured by computing the decrease in reaction time (RT) owing to practice (mean RT during the novel task minus mean RT during the practiced task in ms), and the decrease in error rate owing to practice (errors novel task minus errors practiced task). Accuracy during SAT was calculated by dividing the number of correctly identified deviants by the total number of trials (percent correct). Recall accuracy during PMT was computed by averaging the percentage ‘hits’ (picture pairs correctly identified as seen previously) and ‘correct negatives’ (percentage of picture pairs correctly rejected as seen before) during the retrieval task.

fMRI (dependent variables)

For all three tasks (STERN, SAT, and PMT) analysis of fMRI data involved the following stages: first, after motion correction, statistical activity maps were generated for each individual, for each of the task conditions compared with the rest condition by means of multiple regression (Worsley and Friston, 1995). Next, these maps were smoothed (FWHM 8 mm) and normalized into standard MNI space (Collins et al, 1994), and were analyzed for the whole group, creating a contrast of interest for each task and using z-statistics (details are described in the given references for each task). We then identified regions of activity in the group-maps for each task, thresholded at p<0.05 Bonferroni corrected for the total number of voxels in the brain (Ramsey et al, 1996; Rypma et al, 2006). These group-maps were based on the following contrasts. For STERN, the regions of interest (ROIs) were derived from the difference between the novel and the CT. For SAT, the ROIs were obtained from the contrast between tone detection and rest. For PMT, the ROIs were based on the contrast between associative learning and simple classification.

For regression analyses (based on drug use), we used the mean levels of activity in each of these ROIs for each subject, for each task and for each condition within tasks, relative to resting state.

Statistics

Multiple regression analyses

Specific effects of ecstasy and the relative contributory effects of other substances (amphetamines, cocaine, cannabis, tobacco, and alcohol) on task performance and brain activity were assessed using multiple regression analyses with the different drugs as separate regressors. As non-drug predictors verbal IQ and gender were added to the model, because of the expected effect of IQ on cognitive task performance and because previous studies indicated a higher vulnerability to the effects of ecstasy in females (Reneman et al, 2001; Verheyden et al, 2002). Age was not included as a regressor owing to the relatively small age range within the sample, and to reduce the total number of regressors in the regression model (and thus maximizing statistical predictive power).

The strength of the effect of ecstasy use was estimated using two stepwise regression models. Model 1 estimated the crude or upper bound effect of ecstasy on neurocognitive function, that is, after adjustment for the effects of gender and IQ but without correction for the effects of other drugs. In the first step, gender and IQ were entered, then in a second step, ecstasy. The independent effect of ecstasy is quantified as the R-square change between the first and the second step of the model. This model resembles the approach in previous studies where ecstasy-users were compared with non-users without accounting for polydrug use. The effect of ecstasy in this model is likely to be an overestimation owing to the lack of correction for the impact of other drugs. Model 2 estimated the adjusted or lower bound effects of ecstasy with all other substance use, gender and IQ entered into the model first. Ecstasy use was entered as the second step, and its effect was expressed as the R-square change between the first and the second step. The effect of ecstasy in the second model is likely to be an underestimation.

Goodness-of-fit statistics were used to quantify the fit of the model and standardized regression coefficients-β were used to indicate the predictive power of the different regressors.

Phi correlations were used to explore associations between dichotomized substance variables and demographic variables (see Table 3). The validity of the regression model is not affected by the relatively low associations (in terms of phi values) between some independent variables because each regressor in the stepwise model is adjusted for the predicting effect of all other regressors in the model. The variance inflation factor (VIF), a measure of multicollinearity, had a maximum value of 1.1 (as a rule of thumb any VIF that exceeds 10 is a reason for concern; Stevens, 1996), also indicating that correlations between variables in the model did not cause over-specification of the regression model.

Table 3 Correlations between Use of Various Drugs and Demographic Factors in Selected Sample (N=71)a

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

As data from a few tasks were lost for some subjects owing to technical malfunction, results are reported for each task separately with the number of subjects included within brackets. On average, mean verbal IQ was 101 (SD 7.7, range 83–122). Drug use characteristics are shown in Table 1 and 2, and the correlations between use of various drugs are shown in Table 3.

Performance

Table 5 shows the results from multiple regression analyses for all three tasks. For performance during STERN model 2 yielded a marginal significant βecstasy (R-squareecstasy=0.05, p=0.08). However, the regression model as such failed to reach significance and, therefore, it was statistically not justified to interpret any of the separate standardized β-coefficients. For SAT performance neither regression model 1 nor model 2 reached significance. For PMT performance regression model 1 failed to explain a significant part of the variance in recall accuracy. Regression model 2 explained a marginally significant part (R-square=20.3%, p=0.08) of the total variance in recall accuracy in the PMT task. However, ecstasy use explained only 1% (not significant) of the variance, whereas amphetamine use significantly predicted a reduction in recall accuracy (βamphetamine=−0.39, p=0.007).

Table 5 Results Multiple Regression Analyses STERN, SAT and PMT

fMRI

The ROIs obtained with the contrast described in the methods are shown in Figure 4, and details are given in Table 4. The results of regression analyses are shown in Table 5. The key results are as follows: there were no significant effects of ecstasy or other illicit drugs on brain activity in the STERN (n=70) or the SAT (n=69) tasks. However, there were significant effects of tobacco use, gender, and IQ on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the STERN task: being a smoker was associated with higher levels of brain activity in this brain area during working memory processing, as was being female, whereas higher IQ scores were associated with lower brain activity. Use of drugs did affect associative learning-related activity during the PMT (n=69) task. Ecstasy use predicted lower activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, whereas in the right middle occipital gyrus it was the opposite, that is stronger brain activity. Amphetamine use predicted lower activity in the right middle occipital gyrus, higher activity in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and marginally lower activity in the left parahippocampal gyrus. Cocaine use predicted higher brain activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and marginally higher activity in the right middle occipital gyrus. Finally, use of tobacco predicted marginally lower activity in the right middle occipital gyrus. Strength and direction of the effects of drug use on associative learning-related brain activity are depicted in Figure 5.

Figure 4
figure 4

ROIs for STERN, SAT and PMT: A=fusiform gyrus, B=dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, C=superior parietal cortex, D=anterior cingulate cortex, E=inferior frontal cortex, F=auditory cortex, G=(para)hippocampal region, H=middle occipital gyrus. The numbers beneath the slices indicate the MNI z-coordinates. Slices are in radiological orientation (left side is right hemisphere and vice versa).

Table 4 Regions of Interest
Figure 5
figure 5

Overview of the results from the multiple regression analyses for PMT. The vertical axis of the graphs represents standardized β-coefficients: values reflect strength and direction of the association between use of the drug and magnitude of brain activity in a brain region. On the horizontal axis, colored bars represent the different drugs (see legend; number 1–8 displayed from left to right in the graphs). Abbreviations: L-PHR=left parahippocampal region, L-DLPFC=left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, R-DLPFC=right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, R-MOG=right middle occipital gyrus. The numbers beneath the slices indicate the MNI z-coordinates. Slices are in radiological orientation (left side is right hemisphere and vice versa).

DISCUSSION

The present study found working memory and attention to be intact in polysubstance ecstasy users. However, polydrug use was associated with worse performance on an associative memory task. Interestingly, when effects of drugs were teased apart, it was the use of amphetamine, and not the use of ecstasy, that largely accounted for reduced associative memory performance.

Ecstasy use was related to altered brain activity patterns during associative learning in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the right middle occipital gyrus. These effects were independent from those of cannabis and alcohol use, and appeared to be independent from those of amphetamine, cocaine, or tobacco use. These specific effects of ecstasy on brain activity during associative learning may reflect sustained, possibly long-term adaptation or compensatory reorganization in a fronto-visual network. Whether or not this signifies serotonin neurotoxicity in terms of neuronal damage cannot be concluded from the present findings. However, our results do not support the notion of widespread loss of serotonin axons, as the effects of ecstasy use were moderate, and selective for associative memory. It is therefore more likely that the network involved in associative memory is more sensitive to the effects of ecstasy on cognitive activation than other networks. Several underlying mechanisms may be involved. For one, ecstasy use could compromise serotonin function. Converging evidence from monkey and human studies suggests that the visual cortex is particularly sensitive to MDMA exposure. In a recent study by Brevard et al (2006), it was shown that a low dose of MDMA in marmoset monkeys had an intense effect on brain activity in the visual cortex without applied visual stimuli, measured with BOLD-fMRI. Similar BOLD-effects have been observed in human ecstasy users, that is, an increased visual cortex activation after photic stimulation where the magnitude of the increase was positively related to the degree of prior ecstasy exposure (Cowan et al, 2006). In addition, there is evidence of serotonergic modulation of functional brain activity in the prefrontal cortex and limbic structures during a cognitive challenge (Evers et al, 2005; Rubia et al, 2005). In one study, acute tryptophan depletion significantly reduced brain activation in the right orbito-inferior prefrontal cortex, whereas it increased activation in the superior and medial temporal cortex. It was suggested that reduced prefrontal activation reflects low serotonin turnover, whereas the increased engagement of temporal brain regions could reflect compensatory mechanisms (Rubia et al, 2005). These findings bear a resemblance to the current observations of and reduced activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, increased activation of the middle occipital gyrus during associative learning. Thus, it is conceivable that heavy ecstasy use induces sustained reduced serotonin turnover in the prefrontal cortex. However, this does not explain why lower prefrontal activation in ecstasy users was selective to one cognitive domain, that is associative memory; the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, activated in all tasks (see Figure 4, ROI ‘B’ for STERN and PMT, data not shown for SAT), was only affected during associative memory. Another mechanism could be mild damage to (part of) the associative memory system owing to ecstasy use, though serious enough to lead to reduced functioning. In this case, the (para)hippocampal region is the most plausible candidate as this region was only activated during associative memory, and not during working memory or attention. However, our results suggest that the (para)hippocampal region may not be a prime target for ecstasy-related neurotoxicity (Daumann et al, 2005), as we found only tentative evidence for enhanced brain activity in the left parahippocampal area.

In addition to the specific effects of ecstasy, the effects of amphetamine on activity in the network engaged during associative memory are of special interest as most of the heavy ecstasy users also take amphetamine. Amphetamine use was related to altered brain activity in the right middle occipital cortex, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and possibly the left parahippocampal region (statistical trend). It should be noted that the effects of ecstasy and amphetamine on brain activation were in the opposite direction in the prefrontal and middle occipital regions, suggesting that different mechanisms are at play. This could be related to the different neurotransmitter systems these drugs predominantly act upon, namely, the serotonin (ecstasy) and the dopamine (amphetamine) system. At present, there is no compelling evidence to suggest that ecstasy use damages dopamine neurons in the human brain (Colado et al, 2004), but there is evidence from neuroimaging studies that (meth)amphetamine can induce changes in dopaminergic brain circuits (Choi et al, 2006; Reneman et al, 2002; Uftring et al, 2001; Volkow et al, 2001). However, more research with other (neuroimaging) techniques is necessary to further elucidate the neurochemical mechanisms underlying the drug-induced alterations in the BOLD signal, as measured in the present study.

Our findings of impaired associative memory performance in polysubstance (ecstasy) users are consistent with many neuropsychological studies reporting deficits in mainly verbal memory tests in heavy ecstasy users. It is important to note, however, that our results showed that lower associative memory performance in ecstasy users was largely due to amphetamine, and not ecstasy use. As previous studies in heavy ecstasy users often contend with confounding effects of polysubstance use (including amphetamine), an alternative explanation could be that (part of) the previously observed impairments in memory may be related to concomitant use of amphetamine. Furthermore, our brain activity data challenge the notion that memory impairments in polysubstance ecstasy users reflect hippocampal dysfunction owing to a specific vulnerability of this brain region to the neurotoxic effects of ecstasy (Daumann et al, 2005; Jacobsen et al, 2004). Only amphetamine seemed to affect the parahippocampal region to some extent, but there was no effect of ecstasy.

With regard to working memory and attention our results are less straightforward. We found no evidence for robust effects of ecstasy or other illicit drugs on performance or brain activity in the networks engaged during working memory or selective attention. This seems at odds with findings from several previous fMRI studies on working memory in ecstasy users in which both decreased and enhanced brain activity has been reported in ecstasy users in a variety of brain regions, including the frontal and temporal cortex (Daumann et al, 2003, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c, 2004d; Jacobsen et al, 2004; Moeller et al, 2004). There are several possible explanations for the discrepancy between the current and previous findings: first, activity in brain areas other than those consistently reported as being activated in working memory studies in healthy controls, that is parietal and frontal areas (D'Esposito et al, 1998), may result from specific characteristics of the fMRI task paradigm used. Consequently, observed differences between ecstasy users and non-users in, for example, hippocampal activity, may not be specifically related to working memory, but may surface because the task paradigm used involves other (episodic) memory processes as well. Second, differences in statistical analyses impede comparisons between the studies. In the present study, we used a whole brain analysis correcting for multiple comparisons, whereas other studies used more liberal statistical thresholds (Daumann et al, 2003) or performed a ROI analysis restricted to the (para)hippocampal region (Jacobsen et al, 2004). This may have biased results to certain brain areas that are not involved in working memory per se.

Several limitations of the current study should be noted. For one, a consistent critique of a cross-sectional design is that neurocognitive abnormalities might actually predate and place individuals at risk for drug abuse rather than being the result of abuse. In this regard, it is important to note that animal research has reported MDMA-induced neurotoxicity in several species, including primates (Ricaurte et al, 2000). Furthermore, several human studies have demonstrated dose–effect relationships between cumulative lifetime ecstasy use and memory deficits, which support the idea of ecstasy use causing neurocognitive impairments (Fox et al, 2001). A second limitation is that we had to rely on statements by the subjects themselves on their current and earlier consumption habits, with questionable reliability. Unfortunately, in a naturalistic design there is no obvious solution to this problem. Drug hair analyses confirmed previous ecstasy use in 86% of the subjects who reported to have used ecstasy. In addition, the results from hair analyses showed no evidence for previous use of ecstasy in 96% of the subjects who had reported to be ecstasy-naive. Drug hair analysis yields no information on patterns of ecstasy use, that is frequency, dosage (eg multiple pill ingestion per occasion resulting in nonlinear MDMA plasma levels), or cumulative dose lifetime, but the results support the plausibility of self-reported data on ecstasy use in the current study. Pill purity is related to reliability of self-reported drug use data. Pills containing pure MDMA are probably rare. Nevertheless, between 2002 and 2004, when subjects were recruited, pill-testing confirms that in The Netherlands more than 95% of the tablets sold as ecstasy contain MDMA as the sole (91.2%) or main (4.2%) psychoactive component (Netherlands National Drug Monitor, 2004 (http://www.trimbos.nl/)). A third limitation is that despite the substantial time and effort spent on recruitment, sample stratification was not completely satisfactory. The resulting correlations between use of ecstasy and amphetamine and cocaine were reduced but still substantial and statistically significant. Thus, we cannot claim full independence between predictors. Nonetheless, correlations between use of ecstasy and use of other illicit drugs were lower than usually found after random recruitment among frequent ecstasy users (Scholey et al, 2004; Parrott et al, 2002) and multicollinearity diagnostics indicated that the regression model allowed for reliable estimation of the effects of the various drugs. In contrast, the association between ecstasy use and its most commonly used co-drug cannabis was successfully removed as a result of sample stratification, thereby controlling for an important potential confounder.

In conclusion, this study does not show strong effects of use of ecstasy or other drugs in the domains of working memory and attention. However, polysubstance ecstasy users exhibit reduced associative memory performance, but this impairment is largely due to concomitant amphetamine use, and not to ecstasy use. Moreover, ecstasy and amphetamine have differential and partly opposite effects on brain activity in the network engaged during associative memory, suggesting that different mechanisms are at play that could be related to differential neurochemical effects on serotonin and dopamine systems. The effects of ecstasy and amphetamine are not specific for brain structures; the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, activated in all tasks, is only affected during associative memory. It seems more likely that the network as a whole is affected, and that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex responds differently to alterations in serotonergic vs dopaminergic neurotransmission within certain brain systems.