INTRODUCTION

Accumulating evidence suggests that a common feature of many drugs of abuse is that they produce alterations in excitatory signaling, both directly through the regulation of synaptic function (Lu et al, 1997, 1999; Lu and Wolf, 1999; Hyman and Malenka, 2001; Winder et al, 2002; Lovinger et al, 2003), and indirectly through the regulation of cellular excitability (Zhang et al, 2002; Hu et al, 2004, 2005) in reward circuitry. Evidence to date suggests that psychostimulants differentially regulate glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), two key components of the reward circuitry. Chronic intraperitoneal (i.p.) administration of cocaine increases strength at excitatory synapses onto midbrain dopamine neurons in the VTA (Borgland et al, 2004), while reducing excitatory synaptic strength in the NAcc (Thomas et al, 2001).

While these studies have demonstrated clear mechanisms by which cocaine may regulate brain function, a major question left unanswered is whether contingent administration of cocaine produces the same alterations. It is well-established that excitatory signaling plays a role in behaviors related to self-administration, in both acquisition (Suto et al, 2002) and reinstatement of drug seeking after extinction (Cornish et al, 1999; Cornish and Kalivas, 2000). In the cocaine self-administration model, mice learn the behaviors necessary to obtain the drug, as human drug users do. They also learn that the drug-seeking behaviors lead to a highly reinforcing result. This cognitive, learned aspect of self-administration may make it markedly different from i.p. administration. Furthermore, the route of delivery used for self-administration, intravenous infusion, achieves greater and faster brain levels of cocaine than i.p. administration. Indeed, rapid entry via this route into the brain may be very important for the development of addiction (Samaha et al, 2002, 2004; Samaha and Robinson, 2005). Finally, with self-administration, the mice choose their own frequency of intake, presumably to maintain the most reinforcing level of cocaine. All of these differences may affect the way that cocaine interacts with the drug-reward circuitry.

We have therefore performed experiments to begin to determine whether alterations in excitatory signaling occur following self-administration of cocaine in a manner analogous to i.p. administration. We compared extracellularly recorded synaptically evoked glutamatergic responses in the NAcc shell from adult mice that had self-administered cocaine or saline. Consistent with observations from i.p. cocaine experiments, we have found that self-administered cocaine depresses synaptic responses in the NAcc compared to saline.

METHODS

Self-Administration Methods

Mice

All procedures were approved by Vanderbilt University's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. A total of 41 male, C57Bl/6J mice at 5–6 weeks of age were received from Jackson Laboratories (Bar Harbor, ME). They were allowed to acclimate for at least 1 week in the animal care facility before the start of training. Mice were identified by ear tagging (National Band and Tag).

Food deprivation

To aid in training for operant conditioning, mice were food-deprived. Food and water were taken away between 1500 and 1700 hours, and then returned between 0900 and 1100 hours the following day. They were allowed to eat and drink for 6 h per day with this regimen for 2 days. This procedure (for only 2 days) did not cause significant weight loss in the mice.

Food training

Food-deprived mice were trained in the MedAssociates operant chambers on an FR-1 schedule of reinforcement with 25% Vanilla Ensure as the reinforcer and levers as the manipulanda, with one noncontingent reinforcer available at the start of the session. Sessions were 1 h each and were performed between 0900 and 1200 hours. Illumination of the house light indicated the start of the session, and cue lights indicated availability of the liquid reinforcer. Presses on the inactive lever were recorded but had no programmed consequences. Mice were trained in this way until they received 50 reinforcers for two consecutive sessions. This usually required 4–5 sessions. Mice that did not acquire 50 reinforcers within 12 training days were excluded from the rest of the study. Three mice were excluded for failing to learn to respond for food.

Self-administration training

Mice that successfully completed food training were catheterized in the right jugular vein with a silastic catheter. On the day of surgery, mice were anesthetized with Nembutal (50 mg/kg) injected intraperitoneally. After anesthetizing the mouse, the fur was removed from the intrascapula and the right and left side of the chest. The areas were then sterilized using a 10% povidone-iodine solution. A small longitudinal incision was made in the skin just over the anterior right jugular vein. The jugular vein was isolated from surrounding connective tissues. Two threads of 6-0 silk suture were passed under the vein with a separation of ∼3 mm. The thread placed anteriorly was tied to prevent bleeding. A small incision was made in the vein just below the ligature and a silicone catheter filled with 20% heparin saline was inserted 11 mm into the lumen. The catheter was secured into place with the second thread and the previous thread used to prevent bleeding. The mouse was turned dorsal side up and a small incision was made on the intrascapula. A 14 gauge blunt needle was carefully inserted into the ventral incision and pushed subcutaneously until the end exited through the intrascapula incision. The catheter was carefully pulled through the needle. The ventral incision was closed using 7.0 prolene suture. The catheter was then connected to an access port. The port was made of hypo tube stainless steel tubing (HTX-25-12, Small Parts), Micro-Renethane tubing (Braintree Scientific), silicone tubing, and silicone. The bent portion of the access port was implanted subcutaneously on the intrascapula and the incision sutured with 4.0 and 7.0 prolene suture. The implanted catheter was then flushed with 20% heparinized saline. The total duration of the surgical procedure was ∼50 min. Mice were individually placed on a postsurgical warming bed until fully awake. They were allowed to recover for 1–4 days with food and water available ad libidum before resuming self-administration training.

For self-administration training, the mice were food restricted: one pellet available per day, water available ad libitum. This method maintained the animals at approximately 90% of their free-feeding weight, and sufficiently motivated them to continue lever pressing for cocaine (Carroll et al, 1979, 1981). Self-administration was performed on an FR-1 schedule for 1-h sessions with 0.3 mg/kg/infusion as the training dose. Cue lights were illuminated during the 2.5-s infusion. Lever presses during the infusion were recorded but had no programmed consequences. As in food training, presses on the inactive lever were recorded but had no programmed consequences. Criteria for successful self-administration of cocaine were a modification of those published by Caine et al (2002): (1) stable daily responding (less than 20% variation across 2 consecutive days); (2) a minimum of 10 responses on the active lever; and (3) at least 70% of responses on the active lever. Mice that failed to meet these criteria were excluded from electrophysiological analysis. Two mice were excluded for this reason. Self-administration sessions were terminated if the mouse received 50 infusions. At the end of self-administration training, catheter patency was verified with ketamine. Mice that lacked patent catheters were excluded from further analysis. In all, 14 mice were excluded for this reason.

Mice in the saline control group were treated exactly as the cocaine group, with the exception that they received saline via the intravenous catheters instead of cocaine. No saline mice were excluded based on the self-administration criteria or on the basis of blocked catheters; saline was not expected to act as a reinforcer. However, in some preliminary experiments, saline mice maintained stable lever pressing across several days and performed more than 10 lever presses per hour, which caused us to question whether the cocaine mice were truly seeking cocaine or merely lever pressing out of habit or failure to extinguish food responding. Therefore, we added an additional criterion for successful self-administration. After the 10th day of steady-state administration, mice were placed in the chambers with only saline available after each lever press. Mice receiving cocaine performed an extinction burst, or sharp increase in number of active lever presses. We took this as an indication of active drug seeking in the cocaine mice, because it was markedly absent in the saline mice. One mouse was excluded for failing to meet this criterion.

Dose–response determination in self-administration

For dose–response determination within each mouse, training began as described above with 0.3 mg/kg/infusion cocaine. When each mouse reached stable lever pressing behavior at this dose, meaning (1) he performed at least 10 presses on the active lever, (2) at least 70% of lever presses were on the active lever, and (3) there was less than 20% variation across two consecutive sessions (modification of criteria established by Caine et al, 2002), the dose was varied in pseudorandom order to 0.1 and 1 mg/kg/infusion. Administration of subsequent doses was performed until each mouse achieved stable lever pressing at that dose. (For these doses, criteria were (2) at least 70% of presses on the active lever and (3) less than 20% variation across two consecutive sessions.) The reported activity at each dose is the average number of active lever presses across the two days of stable lever pressing. After completion of the three doses within each mouse, saline replaced cocaine in the syringe for determination of extinction behavior.

Electrophysiology

Electrophysiologists were blinded to the drug history of the mice. Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated. Brains were removed and placed in ice-cold ACSF for slicing (ACSF, in mM: NaCl, 124; KCl, 4.4; MgSO4, 1.2; NaH2PO4, 1; CaCl2, 2.5; NaHCO3, 26; glucose, 10) as previously described (Schramm et al, 2002). Coronal slices (300–350 μm thick) containing the NAcc shell were allowed to recover for at least 1 h before recording. Analysis was performed on an interface chamber (Fine Science Tools) with ACSF plus 0.1% DMSO at 28–29°C at a flow rate of 1.5–2 ml/min. The chamber was humidified and oxygenated with 95% oxygen/5% CO2. Recordings were made via borosilicate glass recording electrodes (∼1 MΩ) filled with ACSF/DMSO (0.1%) and placed in the NAcc shell. Stimuli were delivered via a Grass S88 stimulator. The stimulating electrode was also placed in the NAcc shell, about 500 μm from the recording electrode. pCLAMP 8.0 and 9.0 software was used for data acquisition and analysis. An input–output curve was performed, in which the stimulator was set at 5, 10, 15, and 25 V. The N1 (axonal, presynaptic) and N2 (glutamate-dependent, postsynaptic) responses were recorded at each voltage. The N2/N1 ratio was calculated for each slice at each voltage in triplicate. Triplicates were averaged within each slice, then the ratios for multiple slices from a single mouse were averaged before inclusion in the group average. Stimuli that elicited approximately 1/2 the maximal response were then used for paired-pulse analysis. Stimuli were delivered 20, 50, 80, 110, 140, 170, and 200 ms apart and the ratio of the second to first peak was determined. The second to first peak ratios at each interstimulus interval from slices from the same animal were averaged before inclusion in the group average.

Following paired pulse ratio determination, half-maximal stimuli were delivered every 20 s for establishment of a baseline and for testing the effect of bath-applied forskolin. A minimum of 20 min of stable baseline recording was obtained before application of forskolin. Forskolin (10 μM) was applied for 15 min. The percentage of baseline after forskolin application was determined for each slice, and percentage stimulations were averaged from slices from the same mouse before inclusion in the group average.

Statistical analysis was performed using Microsoft Excel and Statview and JMP 5.1 statistical software.

RESULTS

Cocaine Self-Administration

A within-subjects dose–response curve was generated and is shown in Figure 1. Mice were initially trained on an FR-1 schedule in 1-h daily sessions at 0.3 mg/kg/infusion. They were then switched to 0.1 and 1.0 mg/kg/infusion in pseudorandom order. These three doses revealed the descending arm of the dose–response curve, similar to published reports with mice (Deroche et al, 1997; Caine et al, 2002). After all three doses were completed within each animal, a single extinction test was performed, in which all animals exhibited extinction burst behavior.

Figure 1
figure 1

Within-subject cocaine dose–response relationship in self-administration. Mice were trained initially at 0.3 mg/kg/infusion, as described in Methods, then switched to 0.1 and 1.0 mg/kg/infusion in pseudorandom order. N=7 mice; dose effect, p<0.0001.

As shown in Figure 2, a second group of mice stably self-administered cocaine on an FR-1 schedule for 10 daily 1-h sessions. Stable lever pressing was achieved in the cocaine group after 2–3 days of self-administration training and persisted in the mice that were ultimately used for electrophysiological analysis through the 10th day. Mice receiving cocaine that did not have patent catheters were excluded from further analysis (14 total). In the saline group, lever pressing progressively decreased over the first few days, down to a low basal rate.

Figure 2
figure 2

Self-administration in mice that were used for slicing experiments. (a) Time line of behavioral training and slicing experiments. (b) Active lever pressing in mice that were used for slice experiments. (c) Inactive lever pressing in mice that were used for slicing experiments. Cocaine, N=6 mice; saline, N=10 mice.

Because of the slow extinction of lever pressing in the saline group, and because some members of the saline group maintained stable lever pressing behavior, we added an additional criterion to determine whether the mice in the cocaine group were seeking cocaine. We incorporated an extinction test, performed on the 11th day, in which both groups were given saline. All but one mouse from the cocaine group exhibited an extinction ‘burst’, or at least a 15% increase in lever pressing relative to the average number performed in the last 2 days of cocaine delivery. The one mouse that did not exhibit an extinction burst was excluded from further analysis.

Analysis of Glutamatergic Responses

After recovery from dissection, stimulating and recording electrodes were positioned within the shell of the NAcc, and extracellular field potentials were recorded in response to a range of stimulus intensities (5–25 V for 50 μs). Electrical stimulation yielded a stereotypical response consisting of a stimulus artefact followed by two negative-going peaks (Figure 3a), which have been referred to as N1 and N2 (Takagi and Yamamoto, 1978; Cordingley and Weight, 1986). The N1 reflects in part the recruitment of presynaptic fibers, as it is blocked by tetrodotoxin, while the N2 response is a depolarization initiated by glutamatergic synaptic transmission (Cordingley and Weight, 1986). To compare glutamatergic responses from mice administering cocaine vs saline, we compared the amplitudes of the N2 responses relative to N1 amplitudes across the two populations. Figure 3b shows the N2/N1 ratio from slices from mice exposed to cocaine or saline at stimulus intensities of 10, 15, and 25 V. Ratio results obtained at 5 V showed extreme variability due to the small peak size at that voltage and were therefore excluded from presentation. Values from multiple slices from a single mouse were averaged before inclusion in the group average. There is a main effect of drug treatment: N2/N1 ratios are smaller in slices from cocaine self-administering mice than from saline self-administering mice (RMANOVA, p=0.0035). By pairwise comparison, this effect is significant at the 15-V stimulus intensity (t-test, p<0.05).

Figure 3
figure 3

Electrophysiological data from the nucleus accumbens of mice after 1 day of extinction from self-administration. (a) Typical NAcc field response. Stimulus artefact, N1 and N2 deflections are indicated. (b) N2/N1 ratios at stimulus intensities indicated. N=6 cocaine mice, N=10 saline mice. *p<0.05 by paired t-test. (c) Scatterplot and linear regression relating N1 to N2 (saline N=64 points from 16 slices from 10 mice, cocaine N=40 points from 10 slices from six mice).

An alteration in the N2/N1 ratio could be the product of upregulation of the N1, downregulation of the N2, or a combination of these two. To more specifically address changes in the N2 amplitude, we performed a linear-regression analysis of the data set (Figure 3c). In this way, we could examine the variation in N2 response as a function of N1 response and of drug treatment. This analysis demonstrates that for every increase in N1, there is less of an increase in N2 in the cocaine-exposed slices than in the saline-exposed slices. The slopes of the relationship between N1 and N2 are significantly different between the cocaine and saline groups (by ANCOVA, the N1 by drug interaction term is significant with respect to N2 (p=0.0006)).

Mechanisms of Regulation of Glutamatergic Responses in Cocaine Self-Administering Mice

A reduction in the amplitude of the synaptically evoked excitatory response could occur through multiple pre- or post-synaptic mechanisms. To begin to determine underlying mechanisms, we analyzed paired-pulse ratios of synaptically evoked responses in slices from the cocaine and saline self-administering animals. As shown in Figure 4a, we found no difference in paired pulse ratios at the 1/2 maximal stimulus intensity across a range of interstimulus intervals between cocaine- and saline-administering mice. Slices from both groups of animals exhibited facilitation of the second pulse relative to the first at short interstimulus intervals, which diminished with increasing interstimulus intervals. These data suggest that presynaptic release properties are unchanged between these two conditions.

Figure 4
figure 4

Analysis of paired pulse facilitation and forskolin-induced potentiation in slices from mice after self-administration. (a) Paired pulse facilitation, slices averaged from N=9 mice exposed to saline, and N=6 mice exposed to cocaine. (b) Forskolin-induced potentiation, N=5 mice exposed to cocaine; N=5 mice exposed to saline.

One well-established response to cocaine administration is the upregulation of the cAMP signaling system (Self et al, 1998). To test whether this occurred in our mice and had functional consequences, we tested the response of slices from our mice to a cAMP-stimulating agent, forskolin. Previous studies have demonstrated that activation of adenylyl cyclase by forskolin markedly facilitates glutamatergic transmission in the NAcc (Schramm et al, 2002) through predominantly presynaptic mechanisms (Manzoni et al, 1998; Brundege and Williams, 2002). Therefore, we predicted that cAMP systems would be upregulated and therefore would occlude further stimulation by forskolin. To test this, we bath applied forskolin to slices from saline vs cocaine self-administering animals, and found that peak effects are unchanged in the cocaine self-administering mice. Forskolin potentiated 70±17% responses in slices from cocaine-administering mice, and 86±28% in slices from saline-administering mice (mean±SEM). In total, these data suggest that the depression of N2 responses is not likely through alterations in presynaptic function, and that, if cAMP responses are altered in these slices, it does not affect the EPSPs that we measured.

DISCUSSION

The importance of excitatory signaling pathways in responses to psychostimulants is well established. Psychostimulant administration alters both glutamatergic signaling and excitable properties of neurons, and pharmacologic agents that alter glutamatergic signaling can alter psychostimulant-related behaviors such as self-administration and locomotor sensitization (summarized below). In this study, we examined synaptically evoked excitatory responses in the NAcc shell from mice that had contingently administered cocaine and found that excitatory responses are depressed compared to those observed in saline-administering mice. This depression of excitatory responses likely represents an important change in the circuitry that mediates drug-seeking behavior.

Alterations in NAcc Glutamate Receptor Subunit Expression by Psychostimulants

Levels of the ionotropic glutamate receptor subunits of both the AMPA and NMDA subtypes are altered in the NAcc by psychostimulant administration. The changes are dependent upon the method of administration, brain region examined, and time after administration (withdrawal). After 5 days of i.p. psychostimulant administration followed by 14 days of withdrawal, levels of the AMPA-type GluR1 subunit are reduced in the NAcc shell but not the core, while GluR2 subunit expression is reduced in both the NAcc core and shell (Lu and Wolf, 1999). NMDA receptor expression in the NAcc is also altered by this drug-administration regimen. In the NAcc core and shell, NMDAR1 expression is reduced after 5 days of amphetamine administration followed by 14 days of withdrawal (Lu et al, 1999).

Glutamate receptor levels are also altered after cocaine self-administration in a region and time-sensitive manner. Perhaps most analogous to our experimental design, in the NAcc, GluR1, GluR2, and NMDA receptors are upregulated after 10 days of self-administration and 1 day of withdrawal. In the VTA, GluR2 and NMDA receptors are elevated, but not GluR1 receptors (Lu et al, 2003). This upregulation in the NAcc would seem to contradict our results. Reasons for this may lie in the control group used (food-trained in the Lu et al study, saline in our study), or in the specificity of the tissue punch, which contained both shell and core, and synaptic and extrasynaptic receptors, while we specifically recorded synaptic responses in the shell.

Interestingly, extinction of self-administration can also manipulate levels of AMPA-type receptors. After 1 week of extinction training, when drug-seeking behavior is extinguished, GluR1 and GluR2/3 levels are upregulated in the NAcc relative to withdrawn animals (Sutton et al, 2003). Simulating this upregulation using viral expression of GluR1 or GluR2 accelerates the process of extinction (Sutton et al, 2003). These reports, combined with our results and the results of sensitization studies mentioned earlier, suggest that reduced levels of glutamatergic signaling are associated with elevated drug craving, while elevated levels of glutamate receptors are associated with reduced drug craving. In total, results obtained to date from analysis of receptor expression levels generate a complex picture. Continued analysis of functional synaptic responses, as in this and other studies, will be necessary to clarify how psychostimulant intake alters glutamatergic transmission.

Alterations in Glutamate Release by Psychostimulants

In addition to the postsynaptic receptors mediating the fast synaptic actions of glutamate, evidence suggests that glutamate dynamics are also altered by psychostimulants. Extrasynaptic glutamate levels are altered by cocaine self-administration. In animals trained to self-administer and then subjected to extinction training, extracellular glutamate levels in the NAcc are reduced at baseline (McFarland et al, 2003). Then, upon cocaine challenge, extracellular glutamate sharply increases (McFarland et al, 2003). At present, it is difficult to compare directly our findings that measure the effects of synaptically released glutamate, to these microdialysis studies that are thought to sample primarily extrasynaptic glutamate (Baker et al, 2002).

Alterations in Excitatory Signaling and Behavioral Responses to Psychostimulants

A growing body of literature indicates that psychostimulants produce complex alterations in excitatory signaling systems, as assayed both biochemically and electrophysiologically. Consistent with these alterations, behavioral pharmacological experiments as well as genetic manipulations highlight the importance of excitatory signaling in responses to psychostimulants. For example, during the maintenance phase of self-administration, stimulation of either AMPA or NMDA-type glutamate receptors enhances the reinforcing efficacy of cocaine (Cornish et al, 1999). After extinction, agonism of AMPA receptors in the NAcc induces reinstatement of drug seeking. Conversely, blockade of AMPA, but not NMDA or dopaminergic receptors in the NAcc inhibits cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug seeking in self-administration (Cornish and Kalivas, 2000). Stimulation of AMPA receptors in the NAcc can induce reinstatement of drug seeking after extinction. Further, in addition to the ionotropic glutamate receptors, the metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) also function in behaviors related to psychostimulants (Kim and Vezina, 1998a, 1998b; Suto et al, 2003; Kim et al, 2005).

The NAcc is a site of integration of emotional and motivational stimuli which influences motor output. It receives glutamatergic inputs from the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala and receives dopaminergic input from the VTA. Its main outputs are medium spiny GABAergic neurons that send projections to areas involved in motor output including pallidum, midbrain, substantia nigra, and VTA (summarized in Mulder et al, 1998). In our study, we found reductions in glutamatergic synaptic strength onto the medium spiny GABAergic neurons that constitute over 90% of NAcc neurons (Zhou et al, 2002). These alterations could result from a complex array of changes in the inputs to the region. For example, several reports have shown that cocaine and other drugs of abuse strengthen glutamatergic signaling in the VTA (Ungless et al, 2001; Saal et al, 2003; Borgland et al, 2004), which may enhance inhibitory dopamine signaling in the NAcc as a result. Furthermore, the changes we observed would most likely reduce GABAergic output from the NAcc, and would thereby reduce inhibitory signaling in motor output areas, and could thereby produce diverse behavioral alterations.

Contingent and Noncontingent Psychostimulant Administration Produce Common Effects on Excitatory Responses in the NAcc Shell

Previously, Thomas et al (2001) showed that sensitization to cocaine (5 days of i.p. administration) caused a reduction in the AMPA/NMDA ratio in the NAcc shell (but not the core) and occlusion of LTD in NAcc shell. These two observations indicate an overall reduction in the responsivity of cells in this region to glutamatergic inputs. Other research examining electrophysiological changes after experimenter-administered cocaine suggests additional molecular alterations that may also contribute to an overall dampening of excitatory responses in the NAcc. The function of multiple types of ion channels within cells of the NAcc is reduced, leading to a dampening of cell excitability: i.p. cocaine followed by 3 days of withdrawal leads to a suppression of high voltage-activated calcium potentials and an enhancement of potassium channel activity (Hu et al, 2004). N- and R-type calcium channel activity is also reduced (Zhang et al, 2002). Sodium channel phosphorylation is enhanced (Hu et al, 2005), leading to a reduction in sodium channel activation (Zhang et al, 1998). These multiple alterations likely converge to regulate the net output from the NAcc after cocaine administration. Our results now demonstrate that, as with i.p. administration of cocaine, self-administered cocaine also produces a reduction in excitatory signaling in the NAcc shell. It is likely that many of the individual molecular alterations described above summate to produce the alterations we observed in the field EPSP.

The effects of dopaminergic inputs to the NAcc are also altered by i.p. administration. After 5 days of i.p. injection of cocaine followed by 10–14 days of withdrawal, the ability of dopamine to inhibit glutamatergic stimulation of NAcc neurons was enhanced relative to saline-treated controls (Beurrier and Malenka, 2002). In the VTA, a region which sends dopaminergic projections to the NAcc, a single i.p. injection of cocaine leads to an upregulation of the AMPA/NMDA ratio and an occlusion of LTP on dopaminergic neurons (Ungless et al, 2001). These findings indicate enhanced responsiveness of dopaminergic neurons of the VTA. Thus, the NAcc experiences potentially increased dopaminergic input as well as increased inhibitory responsiveness to that input, resulting in the net dampening of the excitable signaling that we observed.

It was important to directly examine whether alterations elicited by experimenter-administered cocaine occur after self-administered cocaine, because there are two major differences between the two modes of administration. First is the contingent nature of the administration. Contingency has been shown to alter uniquely some responses to drugs of abuse. For example, extracellular glutamate levels in the NAcc are uniquely upregulated upon cocaine priming in self-administering but not yoked control animals (McFarland et al, 2003). In addition, the turnover of acetylcholine in several brain regions (Mark et al, 1999; Smith et al, 2004), CRF receptor gene expression in the hypothalamus (Crespo et al, 2003), and the noradrenergic regulation of metabotropic glutamate receptor mediated inhibition of dopamine neurons (Paladini et al, 2004) are all altered in a contingency-dependent manner.

We would have predicted that glutamatergic transmission in brain regions that are crucial for the learning and conditional reinforcement processes (ie contingency) associated with drug seeking, such as the NAcc shell would be altered by contingently delivered drug, while those regions that function in the unconditioned pharmacological responses to the drug would be altered regardless of the method of administration. Our results, however, show that self-administration of cocaine alters the responsivity of NAcc neurons to glutamatergic signaling in a way which is qualitatively similar to what happens after repeated i.p. administration. This may suggest that the NAcc is a brain region that is less sensitive to contingency or route of administration, or that other aspects of NAcc signaling are contingently regulated.

The second major way in which self-administration differs from i.p. administration is the route of administration, and thereby the rate at which the drug reaches the brain. The route of administration used for self-administration, intravenous infusion, achieves greater and faster brain levels of cocaine than i.p. administration. Rapid entry via this route into the brain appears to be very important for the development of addiction (Samaha et al, 2002; Samaha et al, 2004; Samaha and Robinson, 2005). Thus, it was important that we compare our results to those obtained after i.p. administration.

It will now be important to examine what happens in other regions of the drug-reward circuitry that might influence drug-seeking behavior. For instance, in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), excitatory synaptic transmission is enhanced after cocaine self-administration relative to i.p. administered cocaine, passive i.v. infusion of cocaine or saline, and food self-administration (Dumont et al, 2005). Other brain regions may be similarly sensitive.