Main

Thirty years ago, a burning question for both scientists and the general public was the degree to which genes influence between-group variation in phenotypic traits, particularly behavioral traits such as IQ1. The question of whether mean differences in IQ test scores had the same basis as differences in skin color, hair form and facial features (understood to be heavily influenced by genes) was at the forefront. At that time, scholars found much to agree on regarding genetic and phenotypic variation. Traits (e.g., blood groups and enzyme polymorphisms) close to the DNA level indicated that there was a great deal of within-group variation, with much overlap between 'races' (as defined at the time)2. Although studies had already shown the geographic pattern of human genetic variation3, the proportion of variation between groups was estimated to be low, between 0.06 and 0.15 depending on the groups considered4. For traits often associated with 'racial' or 'ethnic' categorization (e.g., skin pigmentation, facial features and hair form), there was little information regarding the role of genes5. Although there were rough estimates of the number of genes underlying variation in skin color (at least four; ref. 1), no specific genes were known. In contrast, geneticists were well aware of the genetic basis for a number of mendelian diseases, where between-group differences in incidence reflected differences in allele frequency6. They were also aware of between-group differences in incidence of more complex traits, traits without demonstrated genetic components even for the within-group variation6. In addition, there was agreement that for behavioral traits, little was known regarding a role for genes. Physical anthropologists and geneticists 30 years ago took the position that although we couldn't rule out a genetic basis for between-group variation in behavioral traits, we had no evidence indicating such a genetic basis but did have evidence for a role for nongenetic factors1,5.

Today, the question of whether there is a biological or genetic basis to 'race' or 'racial' differences is once again in the popular and scientific press, indicative of public interest in the basis for between-group variation, especially for medical conditions and behavioral traits7,8,9. Two recent books have argued strongly for a genetic basis for what have been called 'race differences'10,11. As writer Steve Olson notes, “Many people continue to believe that human groups have fundamental biological differences. They believe that outward variations in skin color, facial features, or body shape reflect more consequential differences of character, temperament, or intelligence.”12 Several authors, including Olson, have made the opposite argument, stating that there is no biological basis or meaning to race differences7,9,12,13.

The controversy stems, at least in part, from the possibility that attitudes are influenced by whether people believe 'race' is a biological or social construct. Indeed, research has shown that attitudes differ depending on whether individuals conceive of 'race' as biological or social14. Many social scientists have suggested that by linking 'racial' or 'ethnic' categories to biology (especially genetics), one reifies those categories and thereby influences attitudes and behavior15. For these reasons, authors have argued against assuming a genetic basis without evidence5.

In this discussion, we define a social category or group as one determined by social factors; an individual is associated with such a category (or categories) based on a set of socially negotiated criteria. Given this definition, 'race' and 'ethnicity' are social categories, even though some inclusion criteria may be biological. The extent to which 'racial' or 'ethnic' labels correlate with biological traits varies through time and across the labels themselves. Much of that correlation is the outcome of human population history, tied closely to geography16. Here, unless otherwise indicated, we use the term 'group' to refer to a group of individuals associated with a 'racial' or 'ethnic' label.

Has the scientific community made any progress since the early 1970s towards addressing the question of whether there is a genetic basis for between-group phenotypic differences? Some have argued that the sequencing of the human genome and related research have provided evidence that the notion of race is biologically meaningless and therefore useless17,18. Others, however, have made the opposite argument, namely that recent studies show that genetic clusters correspond closely with groups defined by self-identified race or ethnicity or by continental ancestry19.

In light of these contradictory statements, we consider here current scientific understanding relevant to the question of the degree to which phenotypic variation has a genetic basis. We first note a distinction between the terms 'genetic' and 'biological'. Genetic (i.e., DNA) factors can be considered a subset of biological factors, whereas biological, including physiological and morphological, factors reflect chance occurrences during development and the impact of environment (e.g., diet or training) as well as DNA sequence. We next consider three approaches to the study of the basis for between-group phenotypic differences (Table 1).

Table 1 Three methods for assessing the genetic component to within- and between-group variation

Inference from familial correlations

Study designs from genetic epidemiology, including family, twin and adoption studies, have been used extensively to estimate the proportion of trait variance that is due to genetic factors, commonly known as heritability. Heritability estimates, however, are far from absolute and depend not only on the population studied but also on the time and location of study. Any differences in allele frequency between populations or differences in relevant environmental contributions will alter heritability estimates. In addition, simplified models often lead to overestimates of heritability20. Nonetheless, an estimate can provide some insight into genetic contributions to trait variation within groups.

Does a heritability estimate inform us of the extent to which a genetic explanation underlies a group difference in a trait? The answer depends on the nature of that trait. For most complex disease and behavioral traits of interest, heritability is uninformative regarding group differences. Such traits typically result from the interaction between genetic and environmental contributors. A trait that seems to be purely genetic (heritability of 1.0) in one group may also have a group difference that is entirely environmental because an environmental factor differentiates the two groups. For example, studies have shown that without musical training before age six, few individuals will develop absolute pitch (also called perfect pitch). With musical training before age six, some individuals will develop absolute pitch, and whether they do so seems to be strongly familial and possibly genetic21,22. Thus, if two groups (e.g., racial or ethnic groups) have, on average, different experiences with early musical training, a large difference in prevalence of absolute pitch will occur (Fig. 1a). Trait variation will seem to have a genetic contribution (at least in a group with early musical training), even when the group difference is largely environmental. Such a pattern has been observed in one study where individuals of East Asian ancestry were three to four times more likely to have absolute pitch than individuals of European ancestry22.

Figure 1: Two examples of group differences influenced by environmental or genetic factors.
figure 1

(a) Absolute pitch in individuals with early musical training (before age 6) versus those without early musical training. Absolute pitch manifests primarily in the group that received early musical training, is familial and may have a genetic component in the presence of this environmental factor22. This potential genetic influence does not manifest in the absence of early musical training, and therefore the heritability of absolute pitch in a group with no early musical training would presumably be low. The difference in distribution is primarily environmental. Data derived from ref. 21. (b) Taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide in Europeans versus Native Americans. The phenylthiocarbamide tasting gene (TAS2R38, also called PTC) on chromosome 7 is polymorphic in a European population sample, in which the low-sensitivity haplotype has a frequency of 0.50 and homozygotes are at frequency of 0.25. This haplotype is absent in a Native American population sample, in whom the heritability is presumably low. In this case, the difference in distribution is genetic. Data derived from ref. 56.

The opposite pattern is also possible. When the allele that influences the trait segregates in only one group, and the variation in the other group reflects only environmental variation, the trait will appear to have 0 heritability (in the latter group) even though the group difference is explained fully by genetic variation. We illustrate this pattern with the case of taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide (Fig. 1b). A low-sensitivity haplotype was observed at frequency 0.50 in a European sample but was not observed at all in a Native American sample. Genes are responsible for the difference in the distribution of the trait, even though heritability is low in one of the populations.

Inference from DNA analysis

Inferences of the role of genes from familial correlations, even using unusual and informative relationships such as twins and adoptees, are indirect because they are not based on the identification of specific DNA regions that contribute to trait variation. Identification of genes influencing trait variation can be more informative. Many genetic studies today are carried out with reference to a particular population to minimize the influence of genetic or environmental heterogeneity. When a gene (or genes) that influences a trait has been identified, it can then be studied in a broader set of populations, including racially or ethnically defined groups. One might even estimate the between-group difference in trait frequency (in the case of a categorical trait) or in average trait value (otherwise) due to that gene. Such an estimate will be inaccurate, however, unless the effect of the alleles at this locus is the same in the two groups being compared. Because of gene-environment or gene-gene interactions, differences between the two groups in environmental conditions or in genetic background can lead to different impacts of the locus under consideration (e.g., the APOE e4 allele and Alzheimer disease23).

Inference from admixture analysis

One approach that has the potential to further our understanding of group differences in the distribution of a trait is admixture analysis. When individuals with ancestry from geographically disparate regions form a new population (e.g., African Americans, Mexican Americans), it may be possible to correlate trait values with degree of admixture (i.e., individual admixture) from the contributing ancestral populations. A positive correlation in the direction of the original group difference provides support for some genetic contribution to the between-group variation. If individual admixture correlates with other, unmeasured (or omitted) environmental, social, cultural or behavioral traits, however, as is often the case, then the observed trait correlation with individual admixture will be confounded, and a genetic interpretation of the correlation will be unreliable. Although inclusion of relevant environmental factors in the regression analysis is helpful, it may still be impossible to know that all relevant environmental factors have been considered.

There are two ways a genetic conclusion from admixture analysis can be more robust. First, if the individual admixture of individuals in the population is cryptic (not discernible by themselves or others), then residual correlation of individual admixture with social or other environmental agents is less likely. Second, correlation of individual admixture at specific locations in the genome with the trait is strong evidence for a genetic role. This method is the basis of what has been called mapping by admixture disequilibrium. Environmental confounding is unlikely to contribute to a positive regression of the trait at a single or small number of genomic locations.

Inference depends on the nature of the trait

Assessment of genetic contributions to between-group differences depends heavily on the nature of the trait under consideration. Traits that are closely related to DNA sequence variation may show different patterns, and group differences may be more easily resolved, than is true for those traits more remotely related to sequence variation. In a classic paper, Penrose gave the example of phenylketonuria where traits closely related to the actual genetic variant (such as the DNA sequence itself, electrophoretic mobility and serum phenylalanine levels) showed large differences between homozygotes and nonhomozygotes, whereas other traits (e.g., hair color) had much smaller differences24. For the latter traits, many factors other than the gene underlying phenylketonuria also contribute to variation. In discussing how traits have been studied, and the inferences about group differences that have resulted, we therefore consider five sets of traits: (i) DNA-level traits, where genotype is equivalent to phenotype; (ii) mendelian traits; (iii) physical traits often used to categorize people in terms of race or ethnicity; (iv) complex diseases; and (v) other complex phenotypes including behavior, cognitive abilities and temperament (Table 2).

Table 2 Current understanding of genetic contribution to group differences for five different categories of traits

DNA-level traits

Overall, our understanding of the pattern of human genetic variation is quite consistent with that of authors three decades ago. We now know details of the distribution of FST (the proportion of genetic variation between groups) across thousands of DNA markers. Recent estimates of average FST are similar to early estimates25, with exceptions such as the human Y chromosome and mitochondrial DNA (somewhat higher FST values) and sets of STR loci (relatively low FST values)26,27,28. A low average FST for neutral genetic markers suggests that the power to define phenotypic differences in racial and ethnic categories is not typical of a single neutrally evolving locus. On the other hand, an FST estimate of 0.10–0.15 does not rule out a genetic basis for phenotypic differences between groups. In fact, a low FST estimate implies little about the degree to which genes contribute to between-group differences. Small differences in allele frequency indicated by a low average FST are still sufficient for generating clusters of individuals that correspond closely with groups defined by continental ancestry or self-identified race or ethnicity19,27,29,30,31. The joint effect of small differences in allele frequencies at multiple neutral genetic markers provides the power to cluster individuals19,32.

Mendelian traits

The scientific community knows a great deal more about the role of genes in disease, especially mendelian traits, today than it did in the early 1970s. More than 1,000 traits have been mapped to specific chromosomal regions in the last 20 years or so33. Among these traits are a number of drug-metabolizing enzyme activities34,35. These enzyme activities have relatively high differentiation among populations, possibly because of the role of the enzymes in detoxifying compounds in the diet36,37. Also, the extent to which genes underlying 'simple' traits have variable penetrance continues to be apparent; the correspondence between genotype and phenotype is far from simple.

As was true thirty years ago, loci with unusually high or low FST values receive a great deal of attention because such loci may be under selection38,39,40. Some of those markers are known to be associated with differences in disease incidence between groups (e.g., Duffy locus41). In general, however, estimates of FST are of little clinical utility in characterizing racial or ethnic differences. For example, numerous disease or resistance mutations (many associated with mendelian diseases and others not) are found almost exclusively in a single population (e.g., in individuals with European ancestry: hemochromatosis, HFE C282Y; HIV, CCR5 Δ32; and Crohn disease, three CARD15 mutations)42,43,44. Yet these variants have low FST values because there is little absolute variation in frequency across racial groups (FST typically less than 0.05 in these cases)19.

Physical traits

Physical traits (e.g., skin pigmentation, hair form and facial features) are sometimes used to categorize people according to race or ethnicity: We know a little more regarding the genetic basis underlying these traits now than we did in 1970, but not much in the case of humans. The gene MC1R accounts for a small proportion of the variation in human pigmentation45. Much more has been learned about mouse coat pigmentation. As Jackson46 notes, “Despite decades of research on coat-color genes, mutagenic screens continue to identify new dominant mutations that affect pigmentation.” Given the mouse data, it seems likely that more than four genes (a minimum estimate 30 years ago) contribute to variation in human pigmentation. Even though these physical traits may have large racial differences, we cannot at this time conclude that such differences reflect radically different allele frequencies at one or a few genes. It remains plausible that many genes contribute to these traits and to the differences among populations.

Complex diseases

Some progress has been made with respect to a number of diseases, including inflammatory bowel disease and neurodegenerative diseases47,48, but inference of genes influencing complex traits such as diabetes, blood pressure, heart disease, psychiatric disorders and glaucoma remains a challenge49. Reports of linkage of these complex diseases to specific genetic regions are rarely replicated. Therefore, the genetic basis for any between-group variation remains undetermined in most cases. The greater the environmental contribution, the more genetic factors and the more complex the interactions, the more difficult it is to characterize group differences in terms of genetic and environmental contributions.

Behavioral traits

Little progress has been made towards elucidating a genetic basis for any variation in traits such as cognitive ability, temperament and athletic ability. Many studies have made claims regarding the role of specific genes, but few of these have been replicated. The DRD4 polymorphism is a good example. Although initial reports claimed that the polymorphism had a role in variation of 'novelty-seeking'50,51, a recent meta-analysis failed to find consistent support52. This category is a more extreme version of the complex-disease category, possibly having even more important social implications.

Why so little progress?

For complex diseases, and particularly for behavioral traits, little progress has been made towards understanding any genetic basis for within- or between-group variation. Does the lack of progress reflect a lack of research? Complex diseases have certainly received a great deal of attention and funding. Authors of the early 1970s felt that there were good reasons not to spend money on research into the genetic basis of between-group variation of cognitive traits1. Meta-analyses of studies of traits such as novelty-seeking, however, suggest that the lack of progress is not simply a matter of a lack of research.

Under relatively simple models, small differences in allele frequency can lead to relatively large differences in trait frequencies. A trait influenced by several genes that interact with one another may be rare in one group and far less rare in another. Similar models involving interactions between multiple nongenetic factors might also be explanatory. Such models might account for different frequencies of individuals at the extremes of phenotypic distributions (e.g., marathon success of Kenyan runners8). If a model of interactions among many genes and nongenetic factors best fits the data, however, detecting any specific genetic contribution to trait variation will be challenging. Furthermore, our ability to predict phenotype accurately (even given genotypes for a subset of the genes) is much lower than for more simply inherited traits. Much larger studies are needed to measure the effects of genes and the interactions among them.

The relationship between differences in allele frequency and group-level phenotypic differences need not be consistent. We anticipate that in the future all possible relationships will be represented. There may even be cases where there are no reported between-group phenotypic differences but contributing alleles differ in frequency between groups.

Conclusions

During the last three decades, the scientific community has generated a large volume of data regarding human genetic and phenotypic variation. Recent analyses of multilocus genetic data have generated clusters that correspond closely with socially or geographically defined groups. There is the potential, therefore, for genetic factors to contribute to phenotypic differences between groups. But the skepticism of some scientists of the early 1970s regarding our ability to find genes (if such genes exist) underlying group differences in behavioral traits such as intelligence seems warranted. We are far from characterizing the contribution of genes to between-group variation of any complex trait and are likely to continue struggling in the future. The current lack of detailed understanding has two implications.

First, there is a need for stringent criteria for drawing conclusions regarding the contribution of genes to between-group differences. Generalizations and assumptions are unwarranted and may exacerbate group disparities. We therefore advocate standards for statements regarding genetic contributions to between-group differences. With that goal in mind, we discussed promising approaches that take advantage of identified candidate genes or recent population admixture.

Second, our lack of understanding of the etiology of many complex traits means that racial and ethnic labels remain useful in epidemiological and clinical settings. Given the potential for furthering racism by discussing race and genetics together in a scientific context, we might seek to eliminate the use of racial categories in these contexts. Given current health disparities, however, and assuming that our society values the goal of understanding the underlying basis of those disparities, the continued use of labels in epidemiological research and clinical practice seems justified19,53. Racial and ethnic categories are proxies for a wide range of factors, potentially genetic and nongenetic. Many nongenetic correlates of race and ethnicity have been identified. Other factors will probably be elucidated through future epidemiological and genetic research. Once these factors are known, we may no longer need to take those categories into consideration. Until then, however, an individual's racial or ethnic affiliation will continue to be valuable information in the context of epidemiological research54.

The key to successful use of information about group differences in disease incidence (health disparities) is accurate incidence statistics with as much ancestry detail as possible. For now, racial or ethnic categories may be the best we can do in some cases. Not all racial or ethnic labels are equally useful in an epidemiological or clinical context. The labels are useful to the extent that they predict phenotypic differences and are particularly useful if they are predictive over and above any potentially correlated factors, such as education level or health care access. Self-identified race and continental ancestry often have relatively high predictive value. Self-identified ethnicity typically has somewhat less predictive power.

In the future we can strive for more detailed databases, acknowledging missing data regarding the ancestry of many individuals, the fluidity of racial and ethnic categories and increasing rates of intermarriage55. As always, the most accurate diagnoses and predictions (including recommendations for treatment) will come from an individualized analysis of genetic, nongenetic and additional unspecified (in terms of the role of genes) factors. Racial or ethnic categorization will continue to be useful as long as such categorization 'explains' variation unexplained by other factors.