Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Short-term memory span: insights from sign language

Abstract

Short-term memory (STM), or the ability to hold information in mind for a few seconds, is thought to be limited in its capacity to about 7 ± 2 items. Notably, the average STM capacity when using American Sign Language (ASL) rather than English is only 5 ± 1 items. Here we show that, contrary to previous interpretations, this difference cannot be attributed to phonological factors, item duration or reduced memory abilities in deaf people. We also show that, despite this difference in STM span, hearing speakers and deaf ASL users have comparable working memory resources during language use, indicating similar abilities to maintain and manipulate linguistic information. The shorter STM span in ASL users therefore confirms the view that the spoken span of 7 ± 2 is an exception, probably owing to the reliance of speakers on auditory-based rather than visually based representations in linguistic STM, and calls for adjustments in the norms used with deaf individuals.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Short-term memory spans and reading rates in deaf native signers and hearing controls.
Figure 2: Short-term memory spans and recall rates in ASL signers and English speakers.
Figure 3: Short-term memory (STM) and working memory (WM) spans in ASL signers and English controls.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Martin, R.C. & Freedman, M.L. Neuropsychology of verbal working memory: ins and outs of phonological and lexical-semantic retention. in The Nature of Remembering: Essays in the Honor of Robert G. Crowder Vol. XIX (eds. Roediger, H.L. et al.) 331–349 (American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 2001).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Baddeley, A. Working memory and language: an overview. J. Commun. Disord. 36, 189–208 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) (The Psychological Corporation, New York, 1955).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Miller, G.A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 63, 81–97 (1956).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Alvarez, G.A. & Cavanagh, P. The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects. Psychol. Sci. 15, 106–111 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Cowan, N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 87–185 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Klima, E. & Bellugi, U. The Signs of Language (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1979).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Emmorey, K. & Lane, H. The Signs of Language Revisited. An Anthology to Honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima (Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, New Jersey, USA, 2000).

  9. Baddeley, A. Working Memory (Clarendon Press, Oxford, 1986).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Caplan, D., Rochon, E. & Waters, G.S. Articulatory and phonological determinants of word length effects in span tasks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A. 45, 177–192 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Caplan, D. & Waters, G.S. Articulatory length and phonological similarity in span tasks: a reply to Baddeley and Andrade. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A. 47, 1055–1062 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Service, E. The effect of word length on immediate serial recall depends on phonological complexity, not articulatory duration. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A. 51, 283–304 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Wilson, M. The case of sensorimotor coding in working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 44–57 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Conrad, R. Short-term memory processes in the Deaf. Br. J. Psychol. 61, 179–195 (1970).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Conrad, R. Short-term memory in the Deaf: a test for speech coding. Br. J. Psychol. 63, 173–180 (1972).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Wilson, M. & Emmorey, K. A visuospatial “phonological loop” in working memory: evidence from American Sign Language. Mem. Cognit. 25, 313–320 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Wilson, M. & Emmorey, K. A “word length effect” for sign language: further evidence for the role of language in structuring working memory. Mem. Cognit. 26, 584–590 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. MacSweeney, M., Campbell, R. & Donlan, C. Varieties of short-term memory coding in deaf teenagers. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 1, 249–262 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Losiewicz, B.L. A specialized language system in working memory: evidence from American Sign Language. Southwest J. Linguist. 19, 63–75 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  20. Wilson, M. & Emmorey, K. When does modality matter? Evidence from ASL on the nature of working memory. in The Signs of Language Revisited. An Anthology to Honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima (eds. Emmorey, K. & Lane, H.) 135–142 (Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, New Jersey, USA, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  21. Mayberry, R.I. & Eichen, E.B. The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: another look at the critical period for language learning. J. Mem. Lang. 30, 486–512 (1991).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Mayberry, R.I. First-language acquisition after childhood differs from second-language acquisition: the case of American Sign Language. J. Speech Hear. Res. 36, 1258–1270 (1993).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Wilson, M., Bettger, J.G., Niculae, I. & Klima, E.S. Modality of language shapes working memory: evidence from digit span and spatial span in ASL signers. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2, 150–160 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Marschark, M. & Mayer, T.S. Interactions of language and memory in deaf children and adults. Scand. J. Psychol. 39, 145–148 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Mueller, S.T., Seymour, T.L., Kieras, D.E. & Meyer, D.E. Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 29, 1353–1380 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Bellugi, U. & Fisher, S. A comparison of sign language and spoken language. Cognition 1, 173–200 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Ellis, N.C. & Hennelly, R.A. A bilingual word-length effect: implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. Br. J. Psychol. 71, 43–51 (1980).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Lau, C.W. & Hoosain, R. Working memory and language difference in sound duration: a comparison of mental arithmetic in Chinese, Japanese and English. Psychologia 42, 139–144 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  29. Perfetti, C.A. & Lesgold, A.M. Discourse comprehension and sources of individual differences. in Cognitive Processes in Comprehension (eds. Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A.) 141–183 (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, USA, 1977).

    Google Scholar 

  30. Daneman, M. & Merilke, P.M. Working memory and language comprehension: a meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 422–433 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Engle, R.W., Kane, M.J. & Tuholski, S.W. Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (eds. Miyake, A. & Shah, P.) 102–134 (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, 1999).

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  32. Daneman, M. & Green, I. Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. J. Mem. Lang. 25, 1–18 (1986).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Fortkamp, M.B.M. Working memory capacity and aspects of L2 speech production. Commun. Cogn. 32, 259–296 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  34. Elliott, J.M. Forward digit span and articulation speed for Malay, English and two Chinese dialects. Percept. Mot. Skills 74, 291–295 (1992).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Darwin, C.J., Turvey, M.T. & Browder, R.G. An auditory analogue of the Sperling partial report procedure: evidence for brief auditory storage. Cognit. Psychol. 3, 255–267 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Sperling, G. The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychol. Monogr. Gen. Applied 74, 1–29 (1960).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Metcalfe, J., Glavanov, D. & Murdock, M. Spatial and temporal processing in the auditory and visual modalities. Mem. Cognit. 9, 351–359 (1981).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Neath, I. & Crowder, R.G. Schedules of presentation and temporal distinctiveness in human memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 16, 316–327 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Kanabus, M., Szelag, E., Rojek, E. & Poeppel, E. Temporal order judgment for auditory and visual stimuli. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 62, 263–270 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  40. Hanson, V.L. Short-term recall by Deaf signers of American Sign Language: implications of encoding strategy for order recall. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 8, 572–583 (1982).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Hanson, V.L. Recall of order information by deaf signers: phonetic coding in temporal order recall. Mem. Cognit. 18, 604–610 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) edn. 3 (The Psychological Corporation, New York, 1997).

    Google Scholar 

  43. Cohen, J.D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M. & Provost, J. Psyscope: a new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 25, 257–271 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Brainard, D.H. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Pelli, D.G. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10, 437–442 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Keppel, G. Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook (Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, New Jersey, USA, 1991).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the students and the staff of the National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester, New York, and of Gallaudet University, Washington, DC, as well as the participants and organizing committee of CoDAWay 2002. We are also grateful to P. Clark and P. Hauser for their support and to M. Hall, O. Pouliot, J. Cohen, D. Metlay and R. Harris for their assistance. This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (DC04418 to D.B.; DC00167 to E.L.N. and T.S.) and by the James S. McDonnell Foundation (D.B.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daphne Bavelier.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Boutla, M., Supalla, T., Newport, E. et al. Short-term memory span: insights from sign language. Nat Neurosci 7, 997–1002 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1298

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1298

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing