Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing

Abstract

Semantics is a key feature of language, but whether or not music can activate brain mechanisms related to the processing of semantic meaning is not known. We compared processing of semantic meaning in language and music, investigating the semantic priming effect as indexed by behavioral measures and by the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP) measured by electroencephalography (EEG). Human subjects were presented visually with target words after hearing either a spoken sentence or a musical excerpt. Target words that were semantically unrelated to prime sentences elicited a larger N400 than did target words that were preceded by semantically related sentences. In addition, target words that were preceded by semantically unrelated musical primes showed a similar N400 effect, as compared to target words preceded by related musical primes. The N400 priming effect did not differ between language and music with respect to time course, strength or neural generators. Our results indicate that both music and language can prime the meaning of a word, and that music can, as language, determine physiological indices of semantic processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Examples of the four experimental conditions preceding a visually presented target word.
Figure 2: The N400 effect was similar for language and music priming, and did not depend on emotional content.
Figure 3: Grand-average source reconstruction of the N400 effect elicited by target words after the presentation of sentences (top) and musical excerpts (bottom).
Figure 4: ERPs elicited under a condition in which participants performed a memory test, separately for targets presented after sentences (top) and music (bottom).

References

  1. Bernstein, L. The Unanswered Question: Six Talks at Harvard (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1981).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P. ERPs and language comprehension. in Electrophysiology of Mind. Event-Related Potentials and Cognition (eds. Rugg, M. & Coles, M.) 192–208 (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Kellenbach, M., Wijers, A. & Mulder, G. Visual semantic features are activated during the processing of concrete words: event-related potential evidence for perceptual semantic priming. Cog. Brain Res. 10, 67–75 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203–205 (1980).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Kutas, M. & Federmeier, K. Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 463–470 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Friederici, A.D., ed. Language Comprehension: a Biological Perspective (Springer, Berlin, 1998).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  7. Jones, M.R. & Holleran, S., eds. Cognitive Bases of Musical Communication (American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., 1992).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  8. Swain, J. Musical Languages (Norton, New York, 1997).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Raffmann, D. Language, Music, and Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1993).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Meyer, L.B. Emotion and Meaning in Music (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1956).

    Google Scholar 

  11. Hevner, K. The affective value of pitch and tempo in music. Am. J. Psych. 49, 621–630 (1937).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Peirce, C. The Collected Papers of C.S. Peirce (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1958).

    Google Scholar 

  13. Zbikowski, L. Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2002).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  14. Krumhansl, C.L. Perceptual analysis of Mozart's piano sonata KV 282: segmentation, tension, and musical ideas. Mus. Percept. 13, 401–432 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Krumhansl, C.L. An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 336–352 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Pinker, S. How the Mind Works (Norton, New York, 1997).

    Google Scholar 

  17. Friederici, A.D. Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 78–84 (2002).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Démonet, J. et al. The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. Brain 115, 1753–1768 (1992).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Price, C., Moore, C., Humphreys, G. & Wise, R. Segregating semantic from phonological processes during reading. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 727–733 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Friederici, A.D., Opitz, B. & von Cramon, D.Y. Segregating semantic and syntactic aspects of processing in the human brain: an fmri investigation of different word types. Cereb. Cortex 10, 698–705 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Ni, W. et al. An even-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 120–133 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Kuperberg, G. et al. Common and distinct neural substrates for pragmatic, semantic and syntactic processing of spoken sentences: an fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 321–341 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Baumgaertner, A., Weiller, C. & Büchel, C. Even-related fMRI reveals cortical sites involved in contextual sentence integration. NeuroImage 16, 736–345 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Halgren, E. et al. N400-like magnetoencephalography responses modulated by semantic context, word frequency, and lexical class in sentences. NeuroImage 17, 1101–1116 (2002).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Service, E. & Connolly, J.F. Distinct time courses of word and context comprehension in the left temporal cortex. Brain 121, 1133–1142 (1998).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Busnel, R. & Classe, A. Whistled Languages (Springer, Berlin, 1976).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  27. Seboek, T. & Umiker-Seboek, D., eds. Speech Surrogates: Drum and Whistle Systems (Mouton, The Hague, Paris, 1976).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  28. Cowan, G. Mazatco whistle speech. Language 24, 280–286 (1948).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Crystal, D. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  30. Juslin, P.N. Cue utilization in communication of emotion in music performance: relating performance to perception. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 1797–1813 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Juslin, P.N. & Laukka, P. Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: different channels, same code? Psychol. Bull. 129, 770–814 (2003).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Talairach, J. & Tournoux, P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging (Thieme, Stuttgart, Germany, 1988).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stefan Koelsch.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Methods (PDF 6 kb)

Supplementary Audio 1

Musical excerpt priming the word wideness (rather than narrowness). (MP3 159 kb)

Supplementary Audio 2

Musical excerpt priming the word narrowness (rather than wideness). (MP3 242 kb)

Supplementary Audio 3

Musical excerpt priming the word needle (rather than river). (MP3 240 kb)

Supplementary Audio 4

Musical excerpt priming the word river (rather than needle). (MP3 235 kb)

Supplementary Audio 5

Musical excerpt priming the word staircase (rather than blossom). (MP3 213 kb)

Supplementary Audio 6

Musical excerpt priming the word basement (rather than sun). (MP3 335 kb)

Supplementary Audio 7

Musical excerpt priming the word king (rather than bird). (MP3 192 kb)

Supplementary Audio 8

Musical excerpt priming the word illusion (rather than reality). (MP3 252 kb)

Supplementary Audio 9

Musical excerpt priming the word devotion (rather than mischief). (MP3 163 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D. et al. Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing. Nat Neurosci 7, 302–307 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1197

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1197

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing