Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Classical conditioning in the vegetative and minimally conscious state

Abstract

Pavlovian trace conditioning depends on the temporal gap between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. It requires, in mammals, functional medial temporal lobe structures and, in humans, explicit knowledge of the temporal contingency. It is therefore considered to be a plausible objective test to assess awareness without relying on explicit reports. We found that individuals with disorders of consciousness (DOCs), despite being unable to report awareness explicitly, were able to learn this procedure. Learning was specific and showed an anticipatory electromyographic response to the aversive conditioning stimulus, which was substantially stronger than to the control stimulus and was augmented as the aversive stimulus approached. The amount of learning correlated with the degree of cortical atrophy and was a good indicator of recovery. None of these effects were observed in control subjects under the effect of anesthesia (propofol). Our results suggest that individuals with DOCs might have partially preserved conscious processing, which cannot be mediated by explicit reports and is not detected by behavioral assessment.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Stimulus design and different stages of the EMG response.
Figure 2: Learning during the anticipatory interval.
Figure 3: Single-subject measures of learning during the anticipatory interval.
Figure 4: Learning, clinical measures and prediction of recovery.
Figure 5: Nonassociative learning in patients and control subjects: changes in the response to the aversive stimulus.
Figure 6: Intact latencies, but smaller amplitudes, in event-related auditory potentials in DOCs.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Koch, C. The Quest for Consciousness: A Neurobiological Approach (Roberts and Company, Englewood, Colorado, 2004).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Laureys, S. The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 556–559 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Clark, R.E. & Squire, L.R. Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. Science 280, 77–81 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Clark, R.E., Manns, J.R. & Squire, L.R. Trace and delay eyeblink conditioning: contrasting phenomena of declarative and nondeclarative memory. Psychol. Sci. 12, 304–308 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Knight, D.C., Cheng, D.T., Smith, C.N., Stein, E.A. & Helmstetter, F.J. Neural substrates mediating human delay and trace fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 24, 218–228 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. McGlinchey–Berroth, R., Carrillo, M.C., Gabrieli, J.D., Brawn, C.M. & Disterhoft, J.F. Impaired trace eyeblink conditioning in bilateral, medial-temporal lobe amnesia. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 873–882 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Clark, R.E., Manns, J.R. & Squire, L.R. Classical conditioning, awareness and brain systems. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 524–531 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Dawson, M.E. & Furedy, J.J. The role of awareness in human differential autonomic classical conditioning: the necessary-gate hypothesis. Psychophysiology 13, 50–53 (1976).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Lovibond, P.F. & Shanks, D.R. The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 28, 3–26 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Jennett, B. Thirty years of the vegetative state: clinical, ethical and legal problems. Prog. Brain Res. 150, 537–543 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Jennett, B. & Plum, F. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage. A syndrome in search of a name. Lancet 299, 734–737 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Royal College of Physicians. The Vegetative State: Guidance on Diagnosis and Management (Royal College of Physicians, London, 2003).

  13. Multi–Society Task Force on PVS. Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state. N. Engl. J. Med. 330, 1572–1579 (1994).

  14. Giacino, J.T. et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 58, 349–353 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Baars, B.J. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness (Cambridge University Press, New York, 1988).

  16. Dehaene, S. & Naccache, L. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79, 1–37 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Boly, M. et al. Auditory processing in severely brain injured patients: differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state. Arch. Neurol. 61, 233–238 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Bekinschtein, T.A. et al. Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 1672–1677 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Bekinschtein, T. et al. Emotion processing in the minimally conscious state. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 75, 788 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Kotchoubey, B. et al. Information processing in severe disorders of consciousness: vegetative state and minimally conscious state. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 2441–2453 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Laureys, S. et al. Cerebral processing in the minimally conscious state. Neurology 63, 916–918 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Schiff, N.D. et al. fMRI reveals large-scale network activation in minimally conscious patients. Neurology 64, 514–523 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Schnakers, C. et al. Voluntary brain processing in disorders of consciousness. Neurology 71, 1614–1620 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Laureys, S. et al. Auditory processing in the vegetative state. Brain 123, 1589–1601 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Menon, D.K. et al. Cortical processing in persistent vegetative state. Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre Team. Lancet 352, 200 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Boly, M. et al. Perception of pain in the minimally conscious state with PET activation: an observational study. Lancet Neurol. 7, 1013–1020 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Coleman, M.R. et al. Do vegetative patients retain aspects of language comprehension? Evidence from fMRI. Brain 130, 2494–2507 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Owen, A.M. et al. Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science 313, 1402 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Alkire, M.T., Hudetz, A.G. & Tononi, G. Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 322, 876–880 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Tononi, G. & Edelman, G.M. Consciousness and complexity. Science 282, 1846–1851 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Manns, J.R., Clark, R.E. & Squire, L.R. Standard delay eyeblink classical conditioning is independent of awareness. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 28, 32–37 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Veselis, R.A., Reinsel, R.A. & Feshchenko, V.A. Drug-induced amnesia is a separate phenomenon from sedation: electrophysiologic evidence. Anesthesiology 95, 896–907 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Boly, M. et al. When thoughts become action: an fMRI paradigm to study volitional brain activity in noncommunicative brain-injured patients. Neuroimage 36, 979–992 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Bekinschtein, T.A. Cognitive Processes in the Vegetative and Minimally Conscious State. (PhD thesis, Univ. Buenos Aires, 2006).

    Google Scholar 

  35. Carew, T.J., Hawkins, R.D. & Kandel, E.R. Differential classical conditioning of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica. Science 219, 397–400 (1983).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Morris, J.S., Ohman, A. & Dolan, R.J. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature 393, 467–470 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Öhman, A. & Soares, J.J. Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 127, 69–82 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Carter, R.M., O'Doherty, J.P., Seymour, B., Koch, C. & Dolan, R.J. Contingency awareness in human aversive conditioning involves the middle frontal gyrus. Neuroimage 29, 1007–1012 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Voss, H.U. et al. Possible axonal regrowth in late recovery from the minimally conscious state. J. Clin. Invest. 116, 2005–2011 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Giacino, J.T., Kalmar, K. & Whyte, J. The JFK coma recovery scale–revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 2020–2029 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Shiel, A. et al. The Wessex head injury matrix (WHIM) main scale: a preliminary report on a scale to assess and monitor patient recovery after severe head injury. Clin. Rehabil. 14, 408–416 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Bekinschtein, T.A. et al. Can electromyography objectively detect voluntary movement in disorders of consciousness? J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 79, 826–828 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Galton, C.J. et al. Temporal lobe rating scale, application to Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 70, 165–173 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Soriano, S.G., McCann, M.E. & Laussen, P.C. Neuroanesthesia. Innovative techniques and monitoring. Anesthesiol. Clin. North America 20, 137–151 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Siegel, S. & Castellan, N.J. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences (MacGraw-Hill, New York, 1998).

    Google Scholar 

  46. Harrell, F.E. Jr. et al. Regression modeling strategies for improved prognostic prediction. Stat. Med. 3, 143–152 (1984).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the care homes and rehabilitation centers in the UK and Argentina, the Cambridge Impaired Consciousness Research Group, the staff of the Wellcome Trust Research Facility for their contribution, and all the study's participants. We especially thank F. Klein and the Anesthesia Favaloro Team. We also thank L. Naccache and C. Koch for comments on an early version of the manuscript. This study was funded by an Antorchas Foundation grant (T.A.B.), a Marie Curie IIF grant (T.A.B.), a StartUp grant (F.F.M.), the Human Frontiers Science Program (M.S.) and a Medical Research Council Acute Brain Injury Collaborative grant (G0600986).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

T.A.B. and C.F. designed the study. T.A.B. and F.F.M. conducted the behavioral and neurological assessments. T.A.B., C.F., M.R.C., M.H. and D.E.S. conducted the eye-blink conditioning task in the normal volunteers group and T.A.B., M.R.C. and C.F. conducted the task in the patient group. T.A.B., D.E.S., C.F., M.H., M.R.C., F.F.M. and M.S. analyzed and interpreted the data. T.A.B., D.E.S. and M.S. performed the statistical analysis. T.A.B., D.E.S. and M.S. drafted the manuscript. All of the authors revised the manuscript for important intellectual content.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tristan A Bekinschtein.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–3 and Supplementary Tables 1–4 (PDF 1092 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bekinschtein, T., Shalom, D., Forcato, C. et al. Classical conditioning in the vegetative and minimally conscious state. Nat Neurosci 12, 1343–1349 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2391

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2391

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing