Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Categorical perception of colour signals in a songbird

Abstract

In many contexts, animals assess each other using signals that vary continuously across individuals and, on average, reflect variation in the quality of the signaller1,2. It is often assumed that signal receivers perceive and respond continuously to continuous variation in the signal2. Alternatively, perception and response may be discontinuous3, owing to limitations in discrimination, categorization or both. Discrimination is the ability to tell two stimuli apart (for example, whether one can tell apart colours close to each other in hue). Categorization concerns whether stimuli are grouped based on similarities (for example, identifying colours with qualitative similarities in hue as similar even if they can be distinguished)4. Categorical perception is a mechanism by which perceptual systems categorize continuously varying stimuli, making specific predictions about discrimination relative to category boundaries. Here we show that female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) categorically perceive a continuously variable assessment signal: the orange to red spectrum of male beak colour. Both predictions of categorical perception5 were supported: females (1) categorized colour stimuli that varied along a continuum and (2) showed increased discrimination between colours from opposite sides of a category boundary compared to equally different colours from within a category. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of categorical perception of signal-based colouration in a bird, with implications for understanding avian colour perception and signal evolution in general.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Munsell colours used to create stimuli.
Fig. 2: Categorization trials suggested a boundary between colours 5 and 6.
Fig. 3: Discrimination of stimuli that are two colour spaces apart increased across the 5–6 boundary.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Maynard Smith, J. & Harper, D. Animal Signals (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Searcy, W. A. & Nowicki, S. The Evolution of Animal Communication (Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, 2005).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Roff, D. A. The evolution of mate choice: a dialogue between theory and experiment. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1360, 1–15 (2015).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bornstein, M. H. in Categorical Perception: the Groundwork of Cognition (ed. Harnad, S. R.) 287–300 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1987).

  5. Studdert-Kennedy, M., Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S. & Cooper, F. S. Motor theory of speech perception: a reply to Lane’s critical review. Psychol. Rev. 77, 234–249 (1970).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S. & Griffith, B. C. The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. J. Exp. Psychol. 54, 358–368 (1957).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Baugh, A. T., Akre, K. L. & Ryan, M. J. Categorical perception of a natural, multivariate signal: mating call recognition in túngara frogs. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 8985–8988 (2008).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wyttenbach, R. A., May, M. L. & Hoy, R. R. Categorical perception of sound frequency by crickets. Science 273, 1542–1544 (1996).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Nelson, D. A. & Marler, P. Categorical perception of a natural stimulus continuum: birdsong. Science 244, 976–978 (1989).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Wright, A. A. & Cumming, W. W. Color-naming functions for the pigeon. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 15, 7–17 (1971).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. von Frisch, K. Bees: Their Vision, Chemical Senses, and Language (Cornell Univ. Press, Ithaca, 1964).

    Google Scholar 

  12. Sandell, J. H., Gross, C. G. & Bornstein, M. H. Color categories in macaques. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 93, 626–635 (1979).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Jones, C. D., Osorio, D. & Baddeley, R. J. Colour categorization by domestic chicks. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 2077–2084 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Hanley, D. et al. Egg discrimination along a gradient of natural variation in eggshell coloration. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20162592 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Harnad, S. R. Categorical Perception: the Groundwork of Cognition (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1987).

    Google Scholar 

  16. Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Perrett, D. I., Etcoff, N. L. & Rowland, D. Categorical perception of morphed facial expressions. Vis. Cogn. 3, 81–118 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Endler, J. A. Natural and sexual selection on color patterns in poeciliid fishes. Environ. Biol. Fishes 9, 173–190 (1983).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Hill, G. E. Female house finches prefer colourful males: sexual selection for a condition-dependent trait. Anim. Behav. 40, 563–572 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Hill, G. E., Montgomerie, R., Inouye, C. Y. & Dale, J. Influence of dietary carotenoids on plasma and plumage colour in the house finch: intra- and intersexual variation. Funct. Ecol. 8, 343–350 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Borel, P. Genetic variations involved in interindividual variability in carotenoid status. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 56, 228–240 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Blount, J. D., Metcalfe, N. B., Birkhead, T. R. & Surai, P. F. Carotenoid modulation of immune function and sexual attractiveness in zebra finches. Science 300, 125–127 (2003).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. George, D. B., Schneider, B. C., McGraw, K. J. & Ardia, D. R. Carotenoids buffer the acute phase response on fever, sickness behavior and rapid bill color change in zebra finches. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 2957–2964 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Collins, S. A. & ten Cate, C. Does beak colour affect female preference in zebra finches? Anim. Behav. 52, 105–112 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Burley, N. & Coopersmith, C. B. Bill color preferences of zebra finches. Ethology 76, 133–151 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Collins, S. A., Hubbard, C. & Houtman, A. M. Female mate choice in the zebra finch — the effect of male beak colour and male song. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 35, 21–25 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Birkhead, T. R., Fletcher, F. & Pellatt, E. J. Sexual selection in the zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata: condition, sex traits and immune capacity. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 44, 179–191 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Vorobyev, M. & Osorio, D. Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proc. R. Soc. B 265, 351–358 (1998).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Lind, O. Colour vision and background adaptation in a passerine bird, the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). R. Soc. Open Sci. 3, 160383 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Hempel de Ibarra, N., Giurfa, M. & Vorobyev, M. Discrimination of coloured patterns by honeybees through chromatic and achromatic cues. J. Comp. Physiol. A 188, 503–512 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Osorio, D. & Vorobyev, M. Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities in terrestrial animals: adaptations for luminance and colour vision. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 1745–1752 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Riggs, L. A., Blough, P. M. & Schafer, K. L. Electrical responses of the pigeon eye to changes in wavelength of the stimulating light. Vision Res. 12, 981–991 (1972).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Boogert, N. J., Anderson, R. C., Peters, S., Searcy, W. A. & Nowicki, S. Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures. Anim. Behav. 81, 1209–1216 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Endler, J. A. & Mielke, P. W. Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see them. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 86, 405–431 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Bennett, A. T. D., Cuthill, I. C., Partridge, J. C. & Maier, E. J. Ultraviolet vision and mate choice in zebra finches. Nature 380, 433–435 (1996).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Hunt, S., Cuthill, I. C., Swaddle, J. P. & Bennett, A. T. D. Ultraviolet vision and band-colour preferences in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim. Behav. 54, 1383–1392 (1997).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Balkenius, A. & Kelber, A. Colour constancy in diurnal and nocturnal hawkmoths. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 3307–3316 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Stoddard, M. C. & Prum, R. O. Evolution of avian plumage color in a tetrahedral color space: a phylogenetic analysis of new world buntings. Am. Nat. 171, 755–776 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing http://www.R-project.org/ (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, 2016).

  39. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Vorobyev, M., Osorio, D., Bennett, A. T. D., Marshall, N. J. & Cuthill, I. C. Tetrachromacy, oil droplets and bird plumage colours. J. Comp. Physiol. A 183, 621–633 (1998).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank D. Osorio for comments on the manuscript; L. Jin, K. Kochvar, J. Bollinger and L. Deehan for assistance running trials; and R. Mooney and M. Booze for providing access to zebra finches from their colony. Funding for this study was provided by the Duke University Office of the Provost.

Reviewer information

Nature thanks A. Kelber and the other anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.N. conceived and led the project, and secured funding; E.M.C., P.A.G. and M.N.Z. designed and carried out behavioural experiments, with assistance from S.P.; S.J. led the visual modelling and colour vision aspects of the study, with assistance from E.M.C. and P.A.G.; M.N.Z. conducted statistical analyses; P.A.G. prepared figures with assistance from M.N.Z.; S.P. managed the zebra finch population; E.M.C. wrote the initial draft of the manuscript, which was subsequently edited and approved by all authors.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen Nowicki.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Fig. 1 Downwelling vector irradiance at the level of the foraging grid.

Units are photons per cm2 per nm. Tungsten bulbs were used to illuminate each cage from a set distance. There was some variation in irradiance between cages. The blue line represents the mean absolute irradiance of our halogen bulbs, and the grey-shaded region indicates one standard deviation in either direction. The orange line illustrates standard Illuminant A, which we used throughout for visual modelling because it is a standard spectrum (and thus repeatable by other researchers) and closely matched the ambient lighting in our room.

Source Data

Extended Data Fig. 2 Reflected radiance of experimental stimuli under experimental lighting conditions.

Reflected radiance is in arbitrary energy units; stimulus is Munsell paper covered with an epoxy cover. Below 400 nm, the values are so low that they reach the noise floor of the spectrometer, suggesting that the use of a trichromatic model (versus one that includes ultraviolet sensitivity) to examine chromatic distance between colours is appropriate.

Source Data

Extended Data Fig. 3 Results from one-apart discrimination trials.

a, b, Results show data across all birds (a) and for each bird individually (b). Overall, birds performed more poorly at all one-apart tasks compared to two- or three-apart tasks, perhaps suggesting that colours that are one step apart neared the physiological limit of colour discrimination. However, the pass rate for the 5|6 comparison (which crossed the category boundary; green box, grey-shaded area) was significantly higher than for comparisons that did not cross the category boundary (white-shaded areas; blue boxes) (paired t-test, t24 = 4.09, P < 0.01). Numbers in parentheses inside boxes are number of birds that participated in each type of comparison. Box plots depict the median (horizontal line inside the box), 25th and 75th percentiles (box), 25th and 75th percentiles ±1.5× interquartile range (whiskers) and outliers (circles). The horizontal grey line represents the expected pass frequency if birds flip discs by chance. Numbers in square brackets are Michelson contrast for a given colour pair.

Source Data

Extended Data Fig. 4 Results from three-apart discrimination trials.

a, b, Results show data across all birds (a) and for each bird individually (b). Pass rate was significantly higher for the three comparisons that crossed the boundary (3|6, 4|7 and 5|8; green boxes, grey-shaded area) than for those that did not (1|4 and 2|5; blue boxes, white-shaded area) (paired t-test, t25 = 6.07, P < 0.001). Numbers in parentheses inside boxes are number of birds that participated in each type of comparison. Box plots depict the median (horizontal line inside the box), 25th and 75th percentiles (box), 25th and 75th percentiles ±1.5× interquartile range (whiskers) and outliers (circles). The horizontal grey line represents the expected pass rate if birds flip discs by chance. Numbers in square brackets are Michelson contrast values for a given colour pair.

Source Data

Extended Data Fig. 5 Results from greyscale (that is, hue information removed) one-apart discrimination experiments.

These greyscale experiments did not replicate the categories that we observed when hue information was included, indicating that categories are not structured based on brightness alone. Box plots depict the median (horizontal line inside the box), 25th and 75th percentiles (box), 25th and 75th percentiles ±1.5× interquartile range (whiskers) and outliers (circles). Numbers in parentheses below each comparison are Michelson contrast values. Linear mixed models showed that, in our greyscale experiments (that is, without chromaticity information), Michelson contrast between greyscale pairs significantly predicted pass rate. This finding is consistent with the possibility that category formation may be the result of both chromaticity and brightness information (see Table 1). Sample size was 18 birds for all comparisons.

Source Data

Extended Data Fig. 6 Wavelength discrimination of avian photoreceptors.

The wavelength discrimination function of the pigeon C. livia (black dashed line) plotted against the spectral sensitivity peaks of the zebra finch (green, red, blue lines). Original data for the electroretinography-derived wavelength discrimination function are from Riggs et al.31 and have been inverted so that higher numbers indicate greater discrimination.

Source Data

Extended Data Fig. 7 Schematic illustrating differences between continuous and categorical perception.

Under continuous perception (solid line), receivers perceive and respond in a continuous fashion to signal variation, meaning that any change in a signalling trait is associated with a concomitant change in receiver response. Under categorical perception (dashed line), such as described here for female zebra finches, receivers show enhanced discrimination of variants across a boundary (hash marks on x axis) compared to equally spaced variants within a category. Zebra finch line drawing by N. Silina licensed under a Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) licence (http://supercoloring.com/pages/zebra-finch).

Extended Data Table 1 Photon catch values for the short-, medium- and long-wave cones of the zebra finch
Extended Data Table 2 Chromatic distances between selected Munsell colours under different spectral sensitivities and lighting conditions
Extended Data Table 3 Results of a single linear mixed model containing all labelling and discrimination data

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

This file contains an SI Guide providing descriptions for sheets 1-6 in the Supplementary Table file.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Table

This file contains data regarding spectra and sensitivity curves.

Source data

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Caves, E.M., Green, P.A., Zipple, M.N. et al. Categorical perception of colour signals in a songbird. Nature 560, 365–367 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0377-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0377-7

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing