Main

Neuropsychiatric disorders severely compromise the well-being of those affected, with their negative effects on general health and on the ability of children to learn and of adults to work. These disorders have a relatively high prevalence1,2, can have an early onset (for example, autism in childhood and schizophrenia in young adulthood) or a relapsing–remitting course (as in mood and anxiety disorders and obsessive–compulsive disorder), and often have disabling symptoms. For these reasons, they exert, in aggregate, a devastating impact on the families involved and on the human capital of a society. To help guide countries in assessing the investment needed in the health sector, the World Health Organization (WHO) has adapted a measure of disease burden3, the Disability Adjusted Life Year (DALY), which is the sum of the years of life lost to premature mortality (YLL) in a population and the years lived with disability (YLD). Neuropsychiatric disorders, especially mood disorders, are the leading risk factors for suicide, but the major effect of these illnesses on disease burden is the disability they create. For the year 2000, the WHO estimated that neuropsychiatric disorders were responsible for 12% of all DALYs worldwide and 31% of YLDs4 (Fig. 1). For North America and Europe, where infectious disease and malnutrition are far less prominent than in the developing world, neuropsychiatric disorders were responsible for a striking 43% of all YLDs4.

Figure 1: Top ten causes of disability worldwide for individuals aged 15–44, as estimated for the year 2000.
figure 1

The contribution of each condition is quantified as years lived with disability (YLDs). Neuropsychiatric conditions are shown in green. Data are taken from ref. 4.

Despite the disease burden attributable to neuropsychiatric disorders, and despite significant research, their mechanisms of pathogenesis and precise genetic and non-genetic risk factors have remained stubbornly out of reach. Although sustained and clever exploitation of clinical observations has produced a useful pharmacopoeia, almost all recently introduced psychotherapeutic drugs are based on reverse engineering the mechanisms of existing drugs. Arguably, no new drug targets or therapeutic mechanisms of real significance have been identified for more than four decades5. This parlous state of affairs is finally beginning to improve, in part through the application of new genomic technologies coupled to advances in neuroscience. The articles in this Insight explore some of these advances. In this Introduction, I discuss the obstacles that have delayed progress, look at some recent successes in neuropsychiatric genetics and touch on the promise of these findings for neurobiology.

Challenges

There are many reasons for the slow progress in understanding neuropsychiatric disorders. First, it is a daunting challenge to delineate the neurobiology of higher cognition, emotion regulation and executive function ('top-down' control of cognition and behaviour). Yet these are precisely the functions impaired in neuropsychiatric disorders. Second, it has been difficult to produce convincing and useful animal models of human cognitive, social and emotional functions and their dysfunction. Third, the living human brain is protected from the prying eyes and instruments of investigators by its bony skull, and for both practical and ethical reasons, human experimental neurobiology is mostly limited to indirect, non-invasive methods of investigation. Fourth, although neuropsychiatric disorders are highly influenced by genes, the genetic risk factors for common neuropsychiatric disorders have proved fiendishly complex6,7. And last, there are no well-validated objective biological markers by which to delimit precise phenotypes to use in genetics or any other type of research8. The lack of objective tests has meant that diagnosis has been made on the basis of phenomenological criteria (symptoms, signs and course of illness) selected by expert consensus. The current diagnoses listed in the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV)9 are useful in a rough and ready way, and indeed necessary if clinicians and researchers are to communicate, but it would be hard to argue that they provide a mirror of nature8.

Genetics

Despite these obstacles, neurobiology is making steady progress in discovering the molecular, cellular, synaptic, neural-systems, computational and cognitive underpinnings of brain function and its infirmities. The complexity of the task suggests, however, that progress will be painfully slow from the point of view of public health. Measures of genetic influence, such as heritability and recurrence risk ratio (Table 1), suggest that autism10, schizophrenia11,12 and bipolar disorder12 are among the most 'genetic' of any common medical disorder13. Many other common neuropsychiatric disorders, including major depression14, also have a significant genetic component. Therefore, it has long been hoped that alleles conferring increased disease risk could be identified. These would, in turn, provide important tools for neurobiology and would ultimately aid in developing treatments. In Alzheimer's disease, for example, the discovery of disease-causing mutations within the amyloid precursor and presenilin genes identified molecular pathways involved in pathophysiology and thus led to potential drug targets15. Spatial and temporal patterns of expression of risk-conferring genes can identify cells and circuits involved in illness and the timing of disease onset; antibodies raised against the protein products of implicated genes can be used to probe pathogenesis; and risk-conferring genes can be used to construct mouse models of disease.

Table 1 Relative risk of selected disorders

Over the past two decades, however, efforts to identify risk-conferring alleles for the common forms of neuropsychiatric disorder have largely been unrewarding. Despite the significant role for genes highlighted by aggregate measures of their influence (Table 1), the underlying genetics of common neuropsychiatric disorders has proved highly complex, as attested by unpredictable patterns of segregation in families, lack of Mendelian ratios in twin studies and serious difficulties in replicating genetic linkage studies. There is much evidence that similar neuropsychiatric symptoms can result from different combinations of genetic risk factors7. Conversely, the same genetic variant might be associated with multiple DSM-IV diagnoses7,9. For example, a balanced translocation discovered in a large Scottish family disrupts two genes on chromosome 1: disrupted in schizophrenia 1 (DISC1) and (DISC2)16. DISC1 has become a well-studied positional candidate for risk of neuropsychiatric disorders, but even within the index family, the original translocation is found in individuals with different DSM-IV diagnoses: schizophrenia, schizo-affective disorder and recurrent major depression16. Other forms of sequence variation within DISC1 have subsequently been associated with schizophrenia, schizo-affective disorder, bipolar disorder, major depression and both broad and narrow diagnoses of autism17.

At one extreme, heterogeneous genetic pathways to shared phenotypes have been shown to result from interactions among different common genetic variants at multiple genomic loci, often described as the 'common disease–common variant hypothesis'. At the other extreme, they can result from diverse rare, highly penetrant mutations, with each individual mutation causing disease in a small number of families (as is the case for retinitis pigmentosa18). The variants that contribute to common diseases are typically single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) that are not individually deleterious but produce risk through interaction with variants at other loci and with non-genetic factors19.

Rare, highly penetrant disease-causing mutations are by definition deleterious, causing abnormalities by loss or gain of RNA or protein functions. Not all such mutations are fully penetrant, depending on their precise biology and, potentially, on other genetic and environmental factors (such as the avoidance of phenylalanine in phenylketonuria). The identification of rare, highly penetrant disease-causing alleles is relatively straightforward with modern technologies. When common variants produce large effects on risk, they are also fairly easily identified, as in the case of the effect of the apolipoprotein E variant allele APOE ɛ4 in late-onset Alzheimer's disease20 and a variant gene encoding complement factor H in age-related macular degeneration21. The identification of common risk-conferring variants has, however, proved extremely challenging in most cases, because of their relatively small contribution to the disease phenotype (typically a 1.1–1.3-fold increase in risk)19. Thus, the search for common variants that contribute to the risk of common disorders, such as type 2 diabetes22, inflammatory bowel disease and breast cancer23, has only begun to be successful with the advent of tools such as the human haplotype map of SNPs and genome-wide association studies using very large populations or meta-analyses19,22.

In some cases, individual SNPs can produce deleterious mutations, especially when they influence protein translation or messenger RNA splicing. Copy-number variation (CNV), which involves larger structural alterations in chromosomes, including deletions, insertions, duplications and more complex genomic rearrangements, is more likely to produce biological effects by disrupting one or more genes or by quantitatively altering gene expression. In addition to harbouring millions of SNPs, the human genome harbours substantial CNV, some of it occurring de novo rather than being inherited. Even when risk-conferring variants are found and validated, the task of relating them to pathophysiology or to predictive testing has only just begun. Large-scale epidemiological studies are needed to better define the relationship of gene variants to particular phenotypes. Genetic association studies identify risk-conferring alleles independently of knowledge of their biological roles, so there may be little information to guide subsequent laboratory investigations. Moreover, because of the diverse genetic, environmental and random factors that lead to these common diseases, what we think of as a single 'disease' is not in a strict sense a homogeneous entity for which there is a 'Platonic' ideal phenotype. Common diseases are more likely to represent families of diseases that share major pathophysiological and symptomatic features but can differ in important characteristics such as age of onset, severity of symptoms, rate of progression, and response to treatment24.

Phenotyping and clinical diagnosis

Untangling the genetic causes of neuropsychiatric conditions has proved difficult, not only because of intrinsic genetic complexity but also because of the lack of objective diagnostic tests. Despite a decades-long search for objective markers or for intermediary neurobiological phenotypes, such as patterns of cortical thinning in schizophrenia or stress-hormone secretion in depression, validated and widely accepted markers still remain more of a goal than a reality. By contrast, even though common conditions such as type 2 diabetes and hypertension are also heterogeneous and have had shifting diagnostic criteria over time25, their diagnoses are based on objective tests that limit the heterogeneity of population samples used in research.

The DSM-IV diagnoses of conditions such as autism, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depression and obsessive–compulsive disorder are not entirely without external validation: the high heritabilities of many of these disorders, and the remarkable cross-cultural similarity of symptoms, argues that the DSM-IV diagnosis is picking up a real signal. Of course, the editors of the DSM-IV did not ignore possible objective markers but had to develop diagnostic criteria in their absence. The reliance of the DSM-IV on clinical phenomenology means, however, that many patients do not fit any single diagnosis and that there are many anomalous observations, such as symptoms of schizophrenia and diverse mood disorders segregating within the same families7,26.

A complicating factor for phenotyping in genetic and neurobiological studies is that the DSM-IV classification rests on an arbitrary decision to treat all disorders as 'categorical' rather than considering some to represent quantitative deviations from normality. The DSM-IV thus posits clear boundaries between 'ill' and 'well' and between different disorders. There is, however, evidence that many neuropsychiatric disorders, such as autism, schizophrenia11 and major depression27, are better modelled quantitatively on multiple symptom dimensions, by analogy with hypertension. A consequence of the sharp boundaries drawn between 'ill' and 'well' is seen in family studies of schizophrenia: relatives of individuals with schizophrenia, who are considered well by DSM-IV criteria, may show cortical thinning in structural neuroimaging studies and may suffer from cognitive abnormalities7,11. A consequence of the sharp boundaries between disease categories is that many patients receive multiple DSM-IV diagnoses (described as co-morbidity), such as generalized anxiety disorder and major depression, even when the symptoms may be aetiologically related and may respond to the same treatment.

As work begins on DSM-V, the lack of objective markers remains a major problem. That said, there are proposals that might help the DSM-V fit better with scientific data. Such proposals include the addition of quantitative dimensions to the manual for some disorders, based on data derived from sources that include epidemiology, family studies and genetics. Other proposals include the possibility of updating diagnostic criteria for specific disorders without having to wait for an entirely new edition. It is probable that improvements in diagnosis will involve iterative processes in which, for example, genetic and neurobiological discoveries might suggest better disease groupings, which, in turn, might lead to additional genetic and neurobiological advances.

Early progress and future directions

Despite the challenges posed by the DSM-IV classification, as well as by a lack of objective markers for phenotyping and by genetic complexity, this year has seen significant progress in finding common variants that might contribute to bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and in finding rare, highly penetrant mutations that might give rise to schizophrenia and autism. The studies that searched for common variants contributing to bipolar disorder28 and schizophrenia29 assembled large numbers of subjects through the work of international consortia. Large sample sizes are needed because of the heterogeneity of the patients and because of the modest relative risk typically contributed by common variants. The study of bipolar disorder28 achieved significant associations with the genes ANK3 and CACNA1C only after combining two previously published studies and adding a new one. The ANK3 protein has a role in the assembly of voltage-gated Na+ channels; CACNA1C encodes the α1c subunit of the L-type Ca2+ channel. Interestingly, Na+ channels are targets of the anticonvulsant drugs used to treat bipolar disorder. In the study of schizophrenia29, the clearest evidence for association with a gene known as ZNF804A required a meta-analysis. Strikingly, the strength of an association with ZNF804A increased when the affected schizophrenia phenotype was extended to include bipolar disorder. If this finding holds up, it bears witness either to the pleiotropic effects of genes in neuropsychiatric disorders or to a pressing need to revise the DSM-IV with respect to its categorical split between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, a separation that dates back to the psychiatrist Emil Kraepelin (1856–1926).

The strategy of searching for highly penetrant disease-causing mutations has received much attention in neurology but perhaps too little in psychiatry, despite the fact that single-gene disorders with autistic or psychotic symptoms have long been known. Rett syndrome30, fragile X syndrome31 and tuberous sclerosis32 include symptoms of autism. Chromosome 22q11.2 deletions associated with DiGeorge syndrome and velocardiofacial syndrome produce psychosis in 30% of patients33,34. Historically, there had been a concern that study of these rare disorders would not shed light on the common neuropsychiatric disorders with which they shared similar symptoms. As advances in genetics have demonstrated the aetiological heterogeneity of diseases such as autism35 and schizophrenia7,11, it has become clear that useful clues should be seized wherever they arise. Just as several neurodegenerative diseases, including Alzheimer's disease15,36, Parkinson's disease37 and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis38, can result either from multiple common variants or from rare, highly penetrant mutations, it now seems that both schizophrenia29,39,40,41 and autism35,42,43 show both types of genetic risk as well. Indeed a spate of recent papers has correlated excess CNV with risk of schizophrenia39,40,41; others have closed in on mutations that might cause autism by exploiting methods to identify CNV42,43. Undoubtedly, there is also a wide variety of intermediate situations in which relatively penetrant mutations act against a background of multiple disease-modifier alleles to influence age of onset or to produce milder or more severe disease.

In this Insight, Edwin Cook and Stephen Scherer describe how the widespread CNV in the human genome44 might contribute to neuropsychiatric disorders, while reminding us of the difficulty of proving that a particular mutation has a role in the disease phenotype of interest (see page 919). This point deserves to be emphasized. Biologically interesting candidate genes, in the absence of statistical demonstration of a causal role, run the risk of creating a diversion of effort with which this field is only too well acquainted. That said, appropriately validated, highly penetrant mutations provide significant opportunities for neurobiological investigation. Because common disease-related variants contribute only small increments of risk, they are likely to be highly sensitive to genetic background and environmental factors, whether their effects are being studied in people with the disease or in transgenic mouse models. Rare disease-causing mutations are more likely to produce robust phenotypes in mouse models, as they have in models of Alzheimer's disease45 and Rett syndrome46, even if they cannot be said to perfectly mimic the human disease. Understanding the biology of neuropsychiatric disorders will demand the assembly of information from multiple sources and levels of analysis.

Also in this Insight, Thomas Südhof describes the kind of compelling neurobiological analysis that is possible when penetrant mutations are identified in genes known to have significant functions in the nervous system (see page 903). The fascinating story of how mutations in the neuroligin and neurexin genes might alter the balance between excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission is a major step forward. It is also a reminder of how far neurobiology must progress before we can understand how such mutations can produce specific deficits in social cognition. Melissa Ramocki and Huda Zoghbi focus on mutations that illuminate the importance of the regulation of gene and protein expression in the pathogenesis of neuropsychiatric disorders that begin during development (see page 912). They adduce several examples, including Rett syndrome (for which Zoghbi and colleagues were the first to identify a mutation in the MECP2 gene47) and fragile X syndrome, and put forward the hypothesis that loss or gain of protein and RNA functions cause remarkably overlapping cognitive and neuropsychiatric effects because of an effect on neuronal network homeostasis. In addition, Vaishnav Krishnan and Eric Nestler illustrate the sophistication required in the neurobiological investigation of what may be the most common serious neuropsychiatric disorder — depression (see page 894). They describe the utility of both genetic animal models and environmentally produced animal models.

What is the significance for neurobiology of these recent advances in neuropsychiatric genetics? Even variants that contribute a small increment of risk, as is likely to be the case for ANK3 and CACNA1C28, can point to pathways that might be involved in pathophysiology and can thus suggest biological hypotheses and possible drug targets. Where highly penetrant mutations can be identified, they can provide particularly powerful tools for neurobiology, as illustrated by the neurexin and neuroligin story or the investigations into fragile X syndrome and Rett syndrome that have produced animal models of treatment. There is, of course, still a long distance to travel in the identification of risk-conferring alleles, of which there will be many, and in the replication of association studies and demonstration of both statistical and biological significance. There is much challenging neurobiology to come, but it is biology that will ultimately have enormous significance for human health. Neuropsychiatric genetics has had many well-publicized false starts48. Perhaps not all the findings discussed here will hold up, but I feel real optimism brought about by the intelligent application of modern genomic technologies. Maybe the current glimmer of light will lead to a new dawn for the diagnosis and treatment of neuropsychiatric disorders.