Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Artists on Science: Scientists on Art
  • Published:

The artist as neuroscientist

Artistic licence taps into the simplified physics used by our brain to recognize everyday scenes, says Patrick Cavanagh.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: By 1467, artists such as Fra Carnevale had mastered consistent perspective but not consistent lighting.

CREDIT: ROGERS AND GWYNNE ANDREWS FUNDS, 1935 (35.121) PHOTOGRAPH © 1983 THE METROPOLITAN MUSEUM OF ART

Figure 2: Signorelli takes great liberty with shadows, but goes too far here in making the guard's shadow cross over the satyr's shadow as if it were paint.

CREDIT: PURCHASE, J. PULITZER BEQUEST, 1929 (29.164) PHOTOGRAPH © 2004 THE METROPOLITAN MUSEUM OF ART

Figure 3: The dark red areas of the two-tone image of a man's face on the left include both regions of dark shadow and dark pigment (eyebrows, hair, moustache).

CREDIT: G. KIENERK, FLORENCE

Figure 4: An array of cubes all lit from the same direction except one.
Figure 5

CREDIT: ©BETTMANN/CORBIS

Figure 6: No optical distortion of the lemon in the water is shown here and yet the glass and the water appear convincingly transparent.

CREDIT: ©DACS 2005 PHOTOGRAPH: C. SNEE FOR AGNSW

Figure 7: Egyptian artists were the first to depict transparency.

CREDIT: THE ART ARCHIVE/MUSÉE DU LOUVRE PARIS/DAGLI ORTI

Figure 8: When a transparent surface covers a contour in the object behind it, the contour of the transparent surface and the underlying contour cross to form an X-junction.
Figure 9: Two dancers are made up of some of the isolated swatches of colour.

CREDIT: COURTESY OF THE FOGG ART MUSEUM, HARVARD UNIVERSITY ART MUSEUMS, GIFT OF MR AND MRS J. H. HAZEN

Figure 10: When a flat picture is viewed from different angles, the 3D scene can still be perceived without jarring distortions.

CREDIT: ©ADAGP, PARIS AND DACS, LONDON 2005

Figure 11: The blurry global shapes and colours may convey emotional content directly to emotional centres of the brain while the irrelevant fine detail typical of Impressionist pieces distracts conscious perception.

CREDIT: LUNCH AT THE RESTAURANT FOURNAISE (THE ROWERS' LUNCH), PHOTOGRAPH BY R. HASHIMOTO, THE ART INSTITUTE OF CHICAGO

Figure 12: Vuilleumier et al.17 found that the blurry, fearful face on the right activated the amygdala more than the sharply detailed or unfiltered versions.
Figure 13: Try to determine where the woman is looking.

CREDIT: THE ROKEBY VENUS BY VELASQUEZ/NATIONAL GALLERY, LONDON/HTTP://WWW.BRIDGEMAN.CO.UK

Figure 14: These green billiard balls are all shown to reflect the scene that surrounds the ball on the left20.

References

  1. Cavanagh, P., Leclerc, Y. G. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 15, 3–27 (1989).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Casati, R. The Shadow Club (Little Brown, New York, 2004).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Yonas, A., Cleaves, W. T. & Pettersen, L. Science 200, 77–79 (1978).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Bovet, D. & Vauclair, J. Behav. Brain Res. 109, 143–65 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Bruno, V. J. Form and Color in Greek Painting (W. W. Norton & Company, New York, 1977).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Hills, P. The Light of Early Italian Painting (Yale Univ. Press, New Haven, 1987).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Gombrich, E. H. Shadows (Natl Gallery Pub., London, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Ostrovsky, Y., Sinha, P. & Cavanagh, P. Perception (in the press).

  9. Kennedy, J. M. & Silver, J. Perception 3, 313–322 (1974).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Yonas, A. & Arterberry, M. E. Perception 23, 1427–1435 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Kennedy, J. M. & Ross, A. S. Perception 4, 391–406 (1975).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Itakura, S. J. Gen. Psychol. 121, 189–197 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Metelli, F. Sci. Am. 230, 90–98 (1974).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Busey, T. A., Brady, N. P. & Cutting, J. E. Percept. Psychophys. 48, 1–11 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Koenderink, J. J. & van Doorn, A. J. in Looking into Pictures (eds Hecht, H., Schwartz, R. & Atherton, M.) 239–300 (MIT, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  16. Cavanagh, P., von Grünau, M. & Zimmerman, L. IEEE Proc. 2nd Intl Symp. 3D Data Processing, Visualization and Transmission 35–41 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  17. Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J. & Dolan, R. J. Nature Neurosci. 6, 624–631 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Miller, J. On Reflection (Yale Univ. Press, New Haven, 1998).

    Google Scholar 

  19. Croucher, C. J., Bertamini, M. & Hecht, H. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 28, 546–562 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Fleming, R. W., Dror, R. O. & Adelson, E. H. J. Vis. 3, 347–68 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Pegna, A. J., Khateb, A., Lazeyras, F. & Seghier, M. L. Nature Neurosci. 8, 24–25 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Savarese, S., Fei-Fei, L. & Perona, P. ACM Intl Conf. Proc. Series: Proc. 1st Symp. on applied perception in graphics and visualization 115–118 (2004).

  23. Wasserstein, J., Zappulla, R., Rosen, J., Gerstman, L. & Rock, D. Brain Cog. 6, 1–14 (1987).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Morris, J. S., Ohman, A. & Dolan, R. J. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 1680–1685 (1999).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank M. Bernson, E. Besancon, M. Carrio, A. Dietrich, H. Farid, L. Gay, A. Kiely, A. Lonyai, C. Pemberton, D. Wang and R. M. Shapley for their contributions to this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cavanagh, P. The artist as neuroscientist. Nature 434, 301–307 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/434301a

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/434301a

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing