Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Executive control processes underlying multi-item working memory

Abstract

A dominant view of prefrontal cortex (PFC) function is that it stores task-relevant information in working memory. To examine this and determine how it applies when multiple pieces of information must be stored, we trained two subjects to perform a multi-item color change detection task and recorded activity of neurons in PFC. Few neurons encoded the color of the items. Instead, the predominant encoding was spatial: a static signal reflecting the item's position and a dynamic signal reflecting the subject's covert attention. These findings challenge the notion that PFC stores task-relevant information. Instead, we suggest that the contribution of PFC is in controlling the allocation of resources to support working memory. In support of this, we found that increased power in the alpha and theta bands of PFC local field potentials, which are thought to reflect long-range communication with other brain areas, was correlated with more precise color representations.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Multi-item color working memory task.
Figure 2: Task performance.
Figure 3: Percentage of explained variance (PEV) across time for all neurons, calculated using a sliding ANOVA.
Figure 4: Projections of the neural population.
Figure 5: Relative contribution of time, item color, item location (loc.) and nonlinear mixtures of these parameters to the variance in the population's firing rates for one-item trials to each principal component.
Figure 6: Standardized beta values from the sliding regression model.
Figure 7: Average time-frequency power spectrum relative to baseline (500 ms before sample onset).
Figure 8: Average difference in LFP power between electrodes with spatially selective neurons and electrodes without spatially selective neurons.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Constantinidis, C., Franowicz, M.N. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. The sensory nature of mnemonic representation in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 311–316 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Funahashi, S., Chafee, M.V. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. Prefrontal neuronal activity in rhesus monkeys performing a delayed anti-saccade task. Nature 365, 753–756 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Rao, S.C., Rainer, G. & Miller, E.K. Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 276, 821–824 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Asaad, W.F., Rainer, G. & Miller, E.K. Task-specific neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 451–459 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Cowan, N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 87–114 discussion 114–185 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Engle, R.W., Tuholski, S.W., Laughlin, J.E. & Conway, A.R. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 128, 309–331 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Conway, A.R., Kane, M.J. & Engle, R.W. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 547–552 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Rouder, J.N. et al. An assessment of fixed-capacity models of visual working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 5975–5979 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Wilken, P. & Ma, W.J. A detection theory account of change detection. J. Vis. 4, 1120–1135 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Zhang, W. & Luck, S.J. Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory. Nature 453, 233–235 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Bays, P.M. & Husain, M. Dynamic shifts of limited working memory resources in human vision. Science 321, 851–854 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Vogel, E.K. & Machizawa, M.G. Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature 428, 748–751 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Todd, J.J. & Marois, R. Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature 428, 751–754 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Vogel, E.K., McCollough, A.W. & Machizawa, M.G. Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature 438, 500–503 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Buschman, T.J., Siegel, M., Roy, J.E. & Miller, E.K. Neural substrates of cognitive capacity limitations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 11252–11255 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Siegel, M., Warden, M.R. & Miller, E.K. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 21341–21346 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Lara, A.H. & Wallis, J.D. Capacity and precision in an animal model of visual short-term memory. J. Vis. 12 (3), 13 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Conway, B.R. & Tsao, D.Y. Color-tuned neurons are spatially clustered according to color preference within alert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 18034–18039 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Gerbella, M., Belmalih, A., Borra, E., Rozzi, S. & Luppino, G. Cortical connections of the macaque caudal ventrolateral prefrontal areas 45A and 45B. Cereb. Cortex 20, 141–168 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Machens, C.K. Demixing population activity in higher cortical areas. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4, 126 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Machens, C.K., Romo, R. & Brody, C.D. Functional, but not anatomical, separation of “what” and “when” in prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 350–360 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Stokes, M.G. et al. Dynamic coding for cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 78, 364–375 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Meyers, E.M., Qi, X.L. & Constantinidis, C. Incorporation of new information into prefrontal cortical activity after learning working memory tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 4651–4656 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Palva, J.M., Monto, S., Kulashekhar, S. & Palva, S. Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 7580–7585 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. von Stein, A. & Sarnthein, J. Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 38, 301–313 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Schabus, M. & Doppelmayr, M. Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 97–103 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Rigotti, M. et al. The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks. Nature 497, 585–590 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Cromer, J.A., Roy, J.E. & Miller, E.K. Representation of multiple, independent categories in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron 66, 796–807 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Ester, E.F., Anderson, D.E., Serences, J.T. & Awh, E. A neural measure of precision in visual working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 754–761 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Christophel, T.B., Hebart, M.N. & Haynes, J.D. Decoding the contents of visual short-term memory from human visual and parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 12983–12989 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Emrich, S.M., Riggall, A.C., Larocque, J.J. & Postle, B.R. Distributed patterns of activity in sensory cortex reflect the precision of multiple items maintained in visual short-term memory. J. Neurosci. 33, 6516–6523 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Lee, S.H., Kravitz, D.J. & Baker, C.I. Goal-dependent dissociation of visual and prefrontal cortices during working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 997–999 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Romo, R., Brody, C.D., Hernandez, A. & Lemus, L. Neuronal correlates of parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Nature 399, 470–473 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kepecs, A., Uchida, N., Zariwala, H.A. & Mainen, Z.F. Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence. Nature 455, 227–231 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Hernández, A. et al. Decoding a perceptual decision process across cortex. Neuron 66, 300–314 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Crittenden, B.M. & Duncan, J. Task difficulty manipulation reveals multiple demand activity but no frontal lobe hierarchy. Cereb. Cortex 24, 532–540 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Tsao, D.Y., Schweers, N., Moeller, S. & Freiwald, W.A. Patches of face-selective cortex in the macaque frontal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 877–879 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Hagler, D.J. Jr. & Sereno, M.I. Spatial maps in frontal and prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 29, 567–577 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Kastner, S. et al. Topographic maps in human frontal cortex revealed in memory-guided saccade and spatial working-memory tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 3494–3507 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Serences, J.T. & Yantis, S. Spatially selective representations of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional priority in human occipital, parietal, and frontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17, 284–293 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Jerde, T.A., Merriam, E.P., Riggall, A.C., Hedges, J.H. & Curtis, C.E. Prioritized maps of space in human frontoparietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 17382–17390 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Treisman, A.M. & Gelade, G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit. Psychol. 12, 97–136 (1980).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Itti, L. & Koch, C. A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Res. 40, 1489–1506 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Wolfe, J.M. Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1, 202–238 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Johnson, J.S., Sutterer, D.W., Acheson, D.J., Lewis-Peacock, J.A. & Postle, B.R. Increased alpha-band power during the retention of shapes and shape-location associations in visual short-term memory. Front. Psychol. 2, 128 (2011).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Liebe, S., Hoerzer, G.M., Logothetis, N.K. & Rainer, G. Theta coupling between V4 and prefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 456–462 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Canolty, R.T. et al. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313, 1626–1628 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Daitch, A.L. et al. Frequency-specific mechanism links human brain networks for spatial attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110, 19585–19590 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Axmacher, N. et al. Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 3228–3233 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Remondes, M. & Wilson, M.A. Cingulate-hippocampus coherence and trajectory coding in a sequential choice task. Neuron 80, 1277–1289 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Luck, S.J. & Vogel, E.K. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390, 279–281 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Lara, A.H., Kennerley, S.W. & Wallis, J.D. Encoding of gustatory working memory by orbitofrontal neurons. J. Neurosci. 29, 765–774 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Green, D.M. & Swets, J.A. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics (Wiley, New York, 1966).

Download references

Acknowledgements

The project was funded by US NIDA grant R01DA19028 and US NINDS grant P01NS040813 to J.D.W.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

A.H.L. designed the experiment, collected and analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. J.D.W. supervised the project and edited the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Antonio H Lara.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral measures of covert attention.

We analyzed subjects' eye movements and were able to detect microsaccades made during the sample period. We used the filtered eye speed to determine the occurrence and direction of the microsaccades. We used a threshold crossing detection algorithm that detected when the eye speed exceeded two standard deviations of the eye speed during the initial fixation (when no visual stimuli were present on the display). (a) Example of a single trial where items were presented in the upper right and lower left locations. The animal makes a microsaccade towards the upper right item during the sample period (red trace). For this trial, the test item appeared in the lower left and the animal made a full saccade toward that location after he was no longer required to hold fixation (green trace). (b) Eye speed during the course of the trial. During the sample period, there is a small, brief increase in speed corresponding to the microsaccade. The blue triangle indicates the time when the animal made his response and the large peaks in speed correspond to a saccade to the test item. (c) Pattern of microsaccades for all trials in this session. Each plot illustrates a specific two-item configuration, with the position of the items indicated by the gray squares. Red arrows indicate the angular direction of the first microsaccade. The blue lines indicate the angular direction of the second microsaccade (if there was one). There was a clear pattern evident. The subject favored microsaccades to the top left item. If no item was present in the top left, he favored microsaccades to the top right, and if there no items on the top of the screen he favored microsaccades to the bottom left. Such a pattern was consistent with the subject covertly attending to the items, starting with the item in the top right and moving counter-clockwise through the items. Similar stereotyped patterns of covert attention have also been reported in monkeys performing visual search tasks (Buschman & Miller, 2009). Note that microsaccades were only made during the sample epoch; they were absent during the delay. Thus, we cannot be sure how attention was allocated during the delay period.

Supplementary Figure 2 Color grouping procedure.

In order to give the sliding-ANOVA analysis the greatest chance of detecting color-related information, we grouped the 20 colors in the four groups of five colors per group. For each neuron, we grouped colors in four groups of five colors per group (a) and calculated the amount of information encoded by the neuron using the percentage of explained variance (PEV) measure. (b) We changed the colors that comprised each group by rotating the group boundaries clockwise by one color and calculated the PEV once more. (c) We repeated this procedure until we had traversed all the color space. The false discovery rate for this procedure, calculated by assessing the proportion of neurons that reached the criterion during the fixation period, was 4.9%.

Supplementary Figure 3 Additional color selectivity measures.

(a) To examine whether neural responses to colors could be fit by a von Mises distribution we performed a linear regression that was modified for circular data: r = β0 + β1sinθ + β2cosθ, where r is the neuron's firing rate, and θ is the angle of the color in our color space (Figure 1). The plot shows the percentage of variance in neuronal firing rates that could be explained by the model for each neuron at each point in the trial. Each row represents the selectivity a single neuron and neurons are sorted based on the time they show significant color selectivity. Vertical white lines indicate the onset of the sample stimulus, the beginning of the delay and the onset of the test stimulus. We defined a neuron to be color tuned if the regression model significantly fit the data for three consecutive time bins. Using this criterion we found that only 13% (65/507) of neurons were color-tuned. Similar to our sliding PEV analysis, in order to correct for multiple comparisons, we calculated the number of neurons for which the p-value of the regression was larger than 0.005 for three consecutive time bins during the fixation period. This resulted in a false discovery rate of 4.3%. (b) To examine whether the incidence of color selectivity depended on the spatial tuning of the neuron, we restricted our sliding ANOVA analysis (as described in Supplementary Figure 2) to trials in which the color was shown in each neuron's preferred location. Color tuning remained evident in only a small minority of neurons (45/507 or 9% of neurons). Thus, despite using a variety of analysis methods, PFC neurons consistently showed little encoding of color.

Supplementary Figure 4 Recording locations.

To ensure that the lack of color encoding in PFC was not due to the fact that we had confined our recordings to the ventrolateral PFC, we extended our recordings in to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex and the frontal eye fields. The plots show flattened cortical representations illustrating recording locations from the two subjects, and the proportion of color-selective neurons in each location (as indicated by the color of each data point). Gray shading indicates the position of a sulcus. The anterior–posterior (AP) position is measured relative to the interaural line. The circle dashed line demarcates the recording locations of the neurons in the main analysis. Despite recording an additional 323 neurons from these areas, we did not see a significant number of color-selective neurons.

Supplementary Figure 5 Decoding of spatial and color information using a linear classifier.

We used a neural-population decoding algorithm (Meyers et al, 2008; Meyers et al, 2012) that uses a maximum correlation classifier to classify multivariate patterns of activity across the population. By determining how accurately the classifier predicts which experimental conditions are present, one can determine how much information about the experimental conditions is encoded by the neuronal population. We focused our analysis on the single item trials and we decoded the location and the grouped color (see Supplementary Figure 2) of the items using the entire population of PFC neurons. The decoding algorithm used a cross-validation procedure in which the data was randomly split into 10 separate subsets. Nine of these subsets were used to train the classifier and the remaining set was used as a testing set. This cross-validation procedure was repeated 10 times to ensure that each set was used as part of the training and testing sets at least once. This generated 10 classification estimates, which were then averaged to obtain a more robust single classification estimate. This whole procedure was repeated 20 times, each time randomly selecting different subsets of training and testing sets. The results from the 20 repetitions were then averaged and are shown separately for (a) location and (b) color decoding. Shading indicates the standard error of the mean for the 20 separate repetitions of the decoding scheme. The horizontal line at 25% indicates chance performance. Vertical lines indicate the onset of the sample, delay and test epochs, respectively. The results from this decoding analysis strongly support the results from the demixed PCA analysis. We were able to decode spatial information robustly throughout the trial and at a level approaching 100% accuracy during the sample epoch (chance performance was 25%). In contrast, we could barely decode any information about color: it briefly exceeded chance during the sample epoch and returned to chance levels throughout the delay.

Supplementary Figure 6 Two-item regression coding scheme. Illustration of how the x3 regressor was determined.

(a) Spike density histogram showing the firing rate of a single neuron for each of the four locations during the one-item trials. The color of the line corresponds to the location indicated by the legend. This neuron responded strongest to item in the lower left and weakest to items in the upper right. Vertical lines denote the time when the item was presented on the screen. (b) Response to single items presented at the four locations for a different neuron. This neuron responded more to items on the left. (c) Schematic of a two-item trial. In this trial items were presented in the upper and lower left. Using our behavioral estimate of attention, we determined that for this trial the subject first shifted attention to the item in the upper left (denoted by the gray circle). When these items were presented in isolation, the neuron in (a) fired with an average peak rate of 17-Hz to the item in the upper left location (blue trace) and 42-Hz to the item in the lower left (green trace). Thus, x1 = 42 and x2 = 17. The attended location was the neuron's less preferred location of the two and so x3 = x2x1 = (17 – 46) = -29. The neuron in (b) had a peak one-item response to items on the upper left that was higher (x1 = 19) than the one-item response to items on the lower left (x2 = 9). Thus, attention was directed to the neuron's more preferred location, and so we would say that x3 = x1x2 or (19 - 9) = 10. The upshot of this coding scheme is that the beta for x3 will be positive when the neuron's firing rate correlates with the location indicated by our behavioral indices of covert attention and negative when the neuron's firing rate correlates with the alternative location.

Supplementary Figure 7 Regression model with outcome and response parameters.

To examine whether there was any additional variance in the firing rate of individual neurons (over and above the information we had already decoded about space) that could predict whether or not the animal would correctly detect the change in color we added two additional parameters to the regression model: whether firing rate predicted the subject's response ('change' or 'no change') and the subject's performance (trial outcome: correct or incorrect). (a) β values for all neurons corresponding to the preferred location, non-preferred location, trial outcome and subject's response respectively. There was virtually no variance explained by either response or outcome until the test epoch. (b) and (c) show the β3 values separated according to whether the initial β3-value was positive or negative and whether the β3-value switched sign (b) or stayed constant through the trial (c). The regression was performed on spikes aligned to the onset of the sample up to 600-ms before subjects made their response (shown on the left hand side of the plot from time -500-ms to 2100-ms); and using spikes aligned to the time when animals made their response until 500-ms after the response (shown on the right hand side of the panel). Vertical lines indicate onset of the sample, delay and test epochs and the subjects' behavioral response, respectively. Only trials with a reaction time >600-ms were used in this analysis to avoid contamination of response related activity with test-epoch related activity. In practice, this excluded <1% of trials. We also note that this analysis does not necessarily contradict the fact that we were able to detect an effect on the precision of stored information in the local field potential of those electrodes containing spatially selective neurons (Figure 7d, 7e, 8). First, the LFP averages activity across many thousands of neurons and so may be more sensitive at detecting the effect of PFC neuronal firing on behavior. More importantly, however, the single neuron firing rate and the LFP are not necessarily directly related to one another. It is the timing of spikes relative to one another that increases the power of the LFP, not their overall rate. Thus, the firing rate of PFC neurons could remain unchanged, and yet the increase in coherence in spikes communicating with posterior sensory areas would give rise to increased power in certain frequency bands of the LFP.

Supplementary Figure 8 Regression model without the attention parameter.

The mathematical formulation of the biased competition model (Desimone & Duncan, 2005) is that a neuron's response to two items is the sum of the response to either item presented alone, weighted according to which item is currently being attended. To directly test whether this model predicted PFC neuronal responses, we removed the attention parameter from our original model (β3), sorted the trials according to whether the first attended stimulus was the neuron's (a) preferred or (b) unpreferred stimulus (i.e. whether β3 would have been positive or negative) and then examined the dynamics of β1 and β2. If the biased competition model were applicable to PFC, then β1 should be greater than β2 when the first attended stimulus is the neuron's preferred stimulus. In other words, the neuron's firing rate to the two item trials should be better predicted by the response to the preferred stimulus alone when the subject is attending to the preferred stimulus. In contrast, β2 should be greater than β1 when the first attended stimulus is the neuron's unpreferred stimulus. In other words, the neuron's firing rate to the two item trials should be better predicted by the response to the unpreferred stimulus alone when the subject is attending to the unpreferred stimulus. In fact, β1 and β2 encoding did not depend on whether the first attended stimulus was the neuron's preferred or unpreferred stimulus. This supports our original conclusion. PFC spatial signals consist of a static encoding of the spatial location of the stimuli and a dynamic modulation associated with the attentional locus. These findings are consistent with recent studies of attentional effects in PFC, which have emphasized dynamic modulation of spatial selectivity that does not necessarily reflect the original receptive field. For example, analysis of PFC activity at the population level during visual search shows that there is a rapid global allocation of resources to the target that is largely independent of whether the target is in the neuron's preferred or unpreferred location (Kadohisa et al., 2013). Similarly, analysis of single neurons in the frontal eye fields have shown that there is a dynamic increase in neural activity that correlates with the locus of attention and is largely independent of the neuron's receptive field (Buschman & Miller, 2009). Thus, PFC attentional mechanisms are distinct from those in earlier visual areas.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–8 (PDF 3625 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lara, A., Wallis, J. Executive control processes underlying multi-item working memory. Nat Neurosci 17, 876–883 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3702

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3702

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing