Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Molecular analysis of flies selected for aggressive behavior

Abstract

Aggressive behavior is pervasive throughout the animal kingdom, and yet very little is known about its molecular underpinnings. To address this problem, we have developed a population-based selection procedure to increase aggression in Drosophila melanogaster. We measured changes in aggressive behavior in the selected subpopulations with a new two-male arena assay. In only ten generations of selection, the aggressive lines became markedly more aggressive than the neutral lines. After 21 generations, the fighting index increased more than 30-fold. Using microarray analysis, we identified genes with differing expression levels in the aggressive and neutral lines as candidates for this strong behavioral selection response. We tested a small set of these genes through mutant analysis and found that one significantly increased fighting frequency. These results suggest that selection for increases in aggression can be used to molecularly dissect this behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Population cage and territorial assay.
Figure 2: Fighting parameters in arena assay at Gen11 (white bars) and 21 (gray bars).
Figure 3: Aggression quantification in population cage at Gen22 and Aggr-Neutr mixed populations and pairs.
Figure 4: Control behaviors tested in selected lines at Gen22 and 23.
Figure 5: Validation of array expression differences by qPCR.
Figure 6: Fighting frequency of mutants in arena assay and mutant validation.

Similar content being viewed by others

Accession codes

Accessions

Gene Expression Omnibus

References

  1. Blanchard, D.C. & Blanchard, R.J. The colony model of aggression and defense. in Contemporary Issues in Comparative Psychology (ed. Dewsbury, D.A.) (Sinauer, Sunderland, Massachusetts, 1990).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Popova, N.K., Nikulina, E.M. & Kulikow, A.V. Genetic analysis of different kinds of aggressive behavior. Behav. Genet. 23, 491–497 (1993).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Baker, R.R. Insect territoriality. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 28, 65–89 (1983).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lorenz, K.Z. On Aggression (Harcourt, Brace and World, New York, 1963).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Sturtevant, A.H. Experiments on sex recognition and the problem of sexual selection in Drosophila. J. Anim. Behav. 5, 351–366 (1915).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Jacobs, M.E. Influence of light on mating of Drosphila melanogaster. Ecology 41, 182–188 (1960).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Dow, M.A. & von Schilcher, F. Aggression and mating success in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 254, 511–512 (1975).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Jacobs, M.E. Influence of β-alanine on mating and territorialism in Drosophila melanogaster. Behav. Genet. 8, 487–502 (1978).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Partridge, L., Hoffmann, A. & Jones, J.S. Male size and mating success in Drosophila melanogaster and Drosophila pseudoobscura under field conditions. Anim. Behav. 35, 468–476 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Hoffmann, A.A. A laboratory study of male territoriality in the sibling species Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans. Anim. Behav. 35, 807–818 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Hoffmann, A.A. Territorial encounters between Drosophila males of different sizes. Anim. Behav. 35, 1899–1901 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Hoffmann, A.A. Heritable variation for territorial success in two Drosophila melanogaster populations. Anim. Behav. 36, 1180–1189 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Hoffmann, A.A. Selection for territoriality in Drosophila melanogaster: correlated responses in mating success and other fitness components. Anim. Behav. 38, 23–34 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hoffmann, A.A. Geographic variation in the territorial success of Drosophila melanogaster males. Behav. Genet. 19, 241–255 (1989).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Hoffmann, A.A. The influence of age and experience with conspecifics on territorial behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Insect Behav. 3, 1–12 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hoffmann, A.A. & Cacoyanni, Z. Territoriality in Drosophila melanogaster as a conditional strategy. Anim. Behav. 40, 526–537 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hoffmann, A.A. Heritable variation for territorial success in field-collected Drosophila melanogaster. Am. Nat. 138, 668–679 (1991).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Lee, G. & Hall, J.C. A newly uncovered phenotype associated with the fruitless gene of Drosophila melanogaster: aggression-like head interactions between mutant males. Behav. Genet. 30, 263–275 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Ueda, A. & Kidokoro, Y. Aggressive behaviours of female Drosophila melanogaster are influenced by their social experience and food resources. Physiol. Entomol. 27, 21–28 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Baier, A., Wittek, B. & Brembs, B. Drosophila as a new model organism for the neurobiology of aggression? J. Exp. Biol. 205, 1233–1240 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Chen, S., Lee, A.Y., Bowens, N.M., Huber, R. & Kravitz, E.A. Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99, 5664–5668 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Nilsen, S.P., Chan, Y.B., Huber, R. & Kravitz, E.A. Gender-selective patterns of aggressive behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 12342–12347 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Nelson, R. & Chiavegatto, S. Molecular basis of aggression. Trends Neurosci. 24, 713–719 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Droney, D.C. Tests of hypotheses for lek formation in Hawaiian Drosophila. Anim. Behav. 47, 351–361 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Sivinski, J.M., Epsky, N. & Heath, R.R. Pheromone deposition on leaf territories by male Caribbean fruit flies, Anastrepha suspensa (Loew) (Diptera: Tetriphidae). J. Insect Behav. 7, 43–52 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Droney, D.C. & Hock, B.M. Male sexual signals and female choice in Drosophila grimshawi (Diptera: Drosophilidae). J. Insect Behav. 11, 59–71 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hamblen, M. et al. Germ-line transformation involving DNA from the period locus in Drosophila melanogaster: overlapping genomic fragments that restore circadian and ultradian rhythmicity to per0 and per- mutants. J. Neurogenet. 3, 249–291 (1986).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Shaw, P.J., Cirelli, C., Greenspan, R.J. & Tononi, G. Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 287, 1834–1837 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Harshman, L.G. & Hoffmann, A.A. Laboratory selection experiments using Drosophila: what do they really tell us? Trends Ecol. Evol. 15, 32–36 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Osborne, K.A. et al. Natural behavior polymorphism due to a cGMP-dependent protein kinase of Drosophila Science 277, 834–836 (1997).

  31. Lesch, K.P. et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274, 1527–1531 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Grumbling, G. & Strelets, V. The FlyBase Consortium FlyBase: anatomical data, images and queries. Nucl. Acids Res. 34, D484–D488 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Mackay, T.F. et al. Genetics and genomics of Drosophila mating behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 6622–6629 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Toma, D.P., White, K.P., Hirsch, J. & Greenspan, R.J. Identification of genes involved in Drosophila melanogaster geotaxis, a complex behavioral trait. Nat. Genet. 31, 349–353 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Greenspan, R.J. The varieties of selectional experience in behavioral genetics. J. Neurogenet. 17, 241–270 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Gibson, G. & Dworkin, I. Uncovering cryptic genetic variation. Nat. Rev. Genet. 5, 681–690 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Maibeche-Coisne, M., Merlin, C., Francois, M-C., Porcheron, P. & Jacquin-Joly, E. P450 and P450 reductase cDNAs from the moth Mamestra brassicae: cloning and expression patterns in male antennae. Gene 346, 195–203 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Wang, Q., Hasan, G. & Pikielny, C.W. Preferential expression of biotransformation enzymes in the olfactory organs of Drosophila melanogaster, the antennae. J. Biol. Chem., 274, 10309–10315 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Maibeche-Coisne, M., Nikonov, A.A., Ishida, Y., Jacquin-Joly, E. & Leal, W.S. Pheromone anosmia in a scarab beetle induced by in vivo inhibition of a pheromone-degrading enzyme. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 11459–11464 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. McDonald, M.J. & Rosbash, M. Microarray analysis and organization of circadian gene expression in Drosophila. Cell 107, 567–578 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Bray, S. & Amrein, H. A putative Drosophila pheromone receptor expressed in male-specific taste neurons is required for efficient courtship. Neuron 39, 1019–1029 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Broughton, S.J., Kitamoto, T. & Greenspan, R.J. Excitatory and inhibitory switches for courtship in the brain of Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 14, 538–547 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Hall, J.C. Control of male reproductive behavior by the central nervous system of Drosophila: dissection of a courtship pathway by genetic mosaics. Genetics 92, 437–457 (1979).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank J. Sullivan for help with the behavioral analysis, D. Robinson for technical assistance, A. McCartney for help with the figures, J. Fleisher for help with the video files, B. van Swinderen for discussions and for comments on the manuscript and R. Andretic and S. Pangas for comments on the manuscript. This material is based on work supported by the US National Science Foundation under grant no. 0432063 (R.J.G. and H.A.D.) as well as by a grant from the Wacker Foundation (R.J.G.). R.J.G. is the Dorothy and Lewis B. Cullman Fellow at The Neurosciences Institute, which is supported by the Neurosciences Research Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

This study was designed by H.A.D. and R.J.G. H.A.D. performed the experiments, analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Herman A Dierick.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1

Arena assay and aspiration hole. (PDF 86 kb)

Supplementary Figure 2

Array expression profiles of gene in Table 2. (PDF 44 kb)

Supplementary Table 1

Biological categories of significant genes32 (P < 0.002). (PDF 123 kb)

Supplementary Video 1

Main male fighting elements in population cage. The video (three clips spliced together) was shot in a population cage (see Fig. 1a) focusing on one single territory at a time (a blue food cup 2 cm in diameter). The first clip shows wing threat behavior where males lift their wings up at a 45° angle in a threatening posture (which has also been referred to as “wings erect” behavior). The second clip shows the most common male fighting element, referred to as charging or lunging, where the territorial male charges at any intruder on the territory. Often this ends in a lunge where the attacking male lifts his front legs and drops down on the attacked male, who will typically be running away and receive the blow on his side or back. Occasionally, an intruder will not run away, resist the attacks and reciprocate the lunging attacks of the territory holder. This is shown in the third clip and often occurs in short repeated bouts. The two males will then lunge at each other and often box, wrestle and tussle in an attempt to gain control over the territory. We refer to this last behavior as escalated fighting. Such escalations are rare and occur in less than five percent of all aggressive encounters10,21. In the Aggr selected lines they can last for up to 10 minutes. (MOV 609 kb)

Supplementary Video 2

Main male fighting elements in scaled-down territorial assay (Fig 1c). The movie was shot in scaled-down territorial assay with two males, one mated female and small food territory (eppendorf cap filled with fly food). Three clips are spliced together, showing wing threat (slow motion), charging & lunging and escalation (slow motion). (MOV 651 kb)

Supplementary Video 3

Main male fighting elements in arena-assay. The movie shows one arena in the arena chamber. Three clips are spliced together. The first clip shows wing threat followed by a charge and holding, where the attacker grabs the wings of the other male. This often leads to a “roll over”, which is quantified as the intensity parameter in Fig 2d. The second clip shows a fighting sequence. The third clip shows both males in escalation mode, boxing and tussling. (MOV 762 kb)

Supplementary Video 4

Abdomen dipping. The movie was shot in the population cage focusing a single territory. Male streaks his genital area over the food surface while walking as if to mark the territory. Six separate clips are spliced together. (MOV 295 kb)

Supplementary Video 5

Aspiration of escalating males. The movie shows two males escalating on a blue food territory in the population cage. Aspirator comes in from the left and gently sucks up both males from the territory. (MOV 126 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dierick, H., Greenspan, R. Molecular analysis of flies selected for aggressive behavior. Nat Genet 38, 1023–1031 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1038/ng1864

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/ng1864

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing