Introduction

Environmental cues are important moderators of ongoing instrumental behavior. Via Pavlovian learning, previously neutral cues can acquire motivational value by predicting the occurrence of rewarding or punishing events. In substance dependence, stimuli previously associated with drug reward enhance craving,1 promote approach,2 and may thus lead to relapse.3

Experimental approaches to study the influence of Pavlovian conditioned cues on instrumental responses are Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (PIT) paradigms (for example, Holmes et al.4). They combine instrumental learning, where a reward enhances or decreases the occurrence of a specific action, with Pavlovian, where learning confers incentive salience to previously neutral cues by pairing them with a reward. The interaction between Pavlovian cues and instrumental behavior can be specific, that is, when a Pavlovian cue promotes instrumental behavior specifically associated with the same reward. In contrast, general PIT denotes that, depending on the motivational valence, Pavlovian cues can moderate instrumental approach and avoidance responses in general independent of reward types.5, 6 Thereby, positively valued Pavlovian cues encourage approach tendencies, while negative cues promote inhibition and avoidance actions.7 PIT is considered to be an essential mechanism in behaviors that are subject to cue-control, such as eating, alcohol use and drug-taking.

PIT paradigms have been extensively used in animal studies.4 Regarding substance dependence, stronger specific PIT effects were reported in rats after repeated cocaine or amphetamine administration, compared to drug-naive animals.8, 9 Notably, pretreatment with cocaine also enhanced general PIT, where neither Pavlovian cues nor instrumental responses were drug-related.10

Related to that, studies in humans demonstrated attentional biases towards non-drug related (for example, monetary) rewards leading to impaired goal-directed inhibition.11 Interestingly, those attentional biases were found to be greater in opioid-dependent patients compared to controls.12 Adapting animal PIT paradigms in humans confirmed that Pavlovian cues can influence instrumental responding in healthy human subjects.7, 13, 14, 15 In the context of alcohol dependence, our group recently reported that PIT was more pronounced in alcohol-dependent patients compared to controls.16 In a larger sample, we also observed that the PIT effect of non-drug-related cues was positively related to stronger signals in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) only in relapsers, but not in abstainers.17 These findings demonstrate that the degree to which Pavlovian cues affect behavior varies markedly across subjects, which might be further influenced by personality traits.

One important personality trait that is known to be a risk factor for initiating drug and alcohol use as well as addiction is impulsivity.18 Impulsivity is a multifaceted construct that has been operationalized in many ways.19, 20, 21 One of its components is the sensitivity of choices to delay, as assessed by delay discounting tasks, where impulsive choice behavior is defined as the tendency to prefer immediate over delayed rewards. There is a conceptual similarity between models of addiction and the tendency to prefer immediate over delayed rewards, since addictive behaviors are characterized by choosing to consume a substance in the present, often at the expense of concern about future outcomes.22 There are numerous studies reporting higher delay discounting in alcohol-dependent patients compared to controls (for a meta-analysis see MacKillop et al.23), and within patients, higher discounting rates seem to be sensitive for the prediction of relapse.24 On the neural level, impulsive decisions are linked to stronger activation in limbic regions including amygdala and NAcc,25, 26 which are also reported to play a key role during PIT (for example,27, 28).

The current study pursued three aims. First, we wanted to replicate our previous finding that severely alcohol-dependent patients compared to controls show a stronger general PIT effect16, 17 by now analyzing both, a clean replication sample (n=130) where we excluded subjects of previously reported subsamples and the full sample containing all assessed subjects (n=207). Second, due to the bigger sample size, we can provide a more exhaustive analysis to explore whether positive and negative Pavlovian cues differ in their potency to separately influence approach and avoidance behavior, as noted by Geurts et al.29 Third, based on the described literature linking impulsive choice behavior to PIT, we tested whether more impulsive individuals exhibit stronger PIT effects compared to less impulsive subjects.

Materials and methods

All procedures complied with the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the ethical committees of the Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin and of the the Technische Universität Dresden.

Participants

Data were collected in Berlin and Dresden as part of the LeAD study (Learning and Alcohol Dependence; www.leadstudie.de; clinical trial number: NCT01679145). We included 18–65-year-old male and female alcohol-dependent patients who were abstinent from alcohol by a mean of 17 days (s.d.=11) as well as age- and gender-matched healthy controls. Exclusion criteria for all subjects were left-handedness, MR-contraindications (for example, metal-containing foreign body, pacemaker, and claustrophobia), a history of dependence or current substance use except for nicotine; other current DSM-IV axis one psychiatric or neurologic disorders and borderline personality disorder. Included patients were severely alcohol-dependent with a history of alcohol dependence for a minimum of 3 years and had been recently detoxified. Patients were off any medication known to interact with the central nervous system (more than four half-lives post last intake) and displayed no relevant alcohol withdrawal symptoms (CIWA-Ar score3).30 We assessed 212 participants fulfilling all criteria. Due to technical problems in either the delay discounting or the PIT task, data of 5 participants were missing, resulting in a final sample of 116 patients and 91 age- and gender-matched healthy controls. For sample characteristics, see Table 1.

Table 1 Sample characteristics

General experimental procedures

Participants underwent two experimental days. On the first day, after giving written informed consent, participants were interviewed using the computerized Composite International Diagnostic Instrument37, 38 to check for DSM-IV axis I disorders. Following the interview, participants filled out several self-rating questionnaires (Table 1) before they completed a delay discounting task. On the second day, participants completed a PIT paradigm. We excluded current alcohol use by breath alcohol readings at the beginning of both experimental days. At the end of each day, participants received task winnings and a monetary compensation for study participation, resulting in an average payout of 10 €/hour.

Delay discounting task

Participants completed an adaptive delay discounting task which is part of a value-based decision-making task-battery.39 On each of 30 trials, subjects chose between a small immediate and a larger delayed monetary reward. Delays were set to 3, 7, 14, 31, 61, 180, or 365 days. Rewards varied between 0.30 €–10 €. Participants were informed that one of their choices was credited to their compensation at the end of the experiment. On the basis of the choices, we estimated individual delay discounting rates k assuming a hyperbolic discounting function as proposed by Mazur40 and a softmax decision function. Because the distribution of k was skewed, statistical analyses were performed on the natural logarithmic scale.

PIT paradigm

On the second experimental day, participants completed a PIT paradigm (Figure 1).7, 29 The task was programmed using Matlab 2011 (MATLAB version 7.12.0, 2011; MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) with the Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3 (PTB-3) extension.41, 42 The PIT task consisted of four sequential parts: (1) instrumental training, (2) Pavlovian training, (3) PIT and (4) a forced choice task. The instrumental training was conducted before and the forced choice task after the functional-MRI (fMRI), while the Pavlovian training and PIT part were assessed during fMRI scanning.

Figure 1
figure 1

Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (PIT) task consisted of four parts: (a) Instrumental training: To collect a ‘good’ shell, subjects had to move a red dot onto the shell by repeatetly pressing a button. To leave a ‘bad’ shell, subjects were instructed to do nothing. Correct responses were rewarded with 20 Cents in 80% of the trials and punished by loosing 20 Cents in 20% of trials, and vice versa for incorrect responses. Participants performed between 60 and 120 trials. (b) Pavlovian Training: Audio-visual compound cues (CS) were deterministically associated with one of five outcomes (−2€, −1€, 0€, 1€, 2€). Participants performed 80 trials. (c) PIT: Subjects performed the instrumental task again, this time without outcome feedback. The background was tiled with one of the money CS presented during Pavlovian training, or one of 4 beverage stimuli. Participants performed 162 trials. (d) Forced-choice trials: (i) subjects were faced with a choice between two money CS (30 trials); (ii) or a money CS and a beverage stimulus (alcohol or water, 60 trials), (iii) between two beverage stimuli (18 trials).

Instrumental training

Subjects were instructed to collect ‘good’ shells and leave ‘bad’ shells by repeatedly pressing a button while receiving probabilistic feedback. Participants had to press the button 5 or more times in order to collect a good shell (Collect-trials) and do nothing (perform 0–4 button presses) to leave a bad shell (Leave-trials). Correct responses were rewarded with 20 Cents in 80% of the trials and punished with a loss of 20 Cents in 20% of trials, and for wrong responses it was vice versa (Figure 1a). We presented six different shells while each trial lasted 2 s. Participants performed between 60 and 120 trials, depending on their performance: in an effort to ensure that all subjects were at comparable performance levels, a learning criterion stopped instrumental training if participants performed 80% correct choices over 16 consecutive trials.

Pavlovian training

For Pavlovian conditioning, 80 trials of pairing monetary rewards or punishments with compound visual and auditory CS were presented to the subjects. At the beginning of each trial, a compound CS consisting of abstract pictures and pure tones was presented for 3 s. The compound CS was followed by a delay of 3 s with two fixation crosses at the two potential CS locations (left and right), then a US (monetary reward or punishment) was presented for a further 3 s (Figure 1b). CS and US presentation was separated with a 3-second interstimulus interval. Subjects were instructed to passively observe the CS and US and to memorize the pairings. The set of stimulus pairings consisted of two positive CS paired with images of +2€ and +1€ coins, respectively; one neutral CS paired with 0€ and two negative CS paired with −1€ and −2€, respectively (coins with a superimposed red cross).

Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer

Subjects performed the instrumental task again, this time without outcome feedback. The background was tiled with one of the money CS presented during Pavlovian training, or one of 4 beverage stimuli (2 pictures of the favorite alcoholic drink and 2 pictures of water). Participants performed 162 trials while each trial lasted 3 s (Figure 1c).

Forced-choice task

A forded choice task was performed in order to verify acquisition of Pavlovian expectations. For each trial, subjects had to choose one of two sequentially presented compound CS (Figure 1d). All possible compound CS pairings were presented three times in an interleaved, randomized order and stimuli were presented one at a time for 2 s.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using R 3.2.0 (R Development Core Team, Vienna, Austria, 2014).43 Demographic group characteristics and group comparisons were examined using chi-square and t-tests (Table 1). While all further described analyses refer to the transfer part of our experiment (Figure 1c), analyses of the instrumental training and the forced choice trails can be found in the supplement (Supplementary Figures 2 and 3).

In a first step, we replicated our previous finding that patients compared to controls show a stronger general PIT effect16, 17 by creating a clean replication sample, excluding all subjects of those previously reported studies. To analyze differences in accuracy between patients and controls, we used binomial mixed-effects models (glmer, R-package: lme4). We report Bonferroni-corrected estimates, standard errors, z-values and P-values from the glmer output. We coded participants’ response for each trial as correct (=1) if a good shell was collected or a bad shell was left, and as false (=0) if a bad shell was collected or a good shell was left, respectively. In a second step, we used the same models to analyze the transfer part within our full sample. Finally, to check for the influence of impulsivity on the PIT effect in the full sample, we extracted the parameter k from the delay discounting task. Since the range of k was approximately equal in patients (Min=−13.3; Max=4.3) and in controls (Min=−12.6; Max=5.1), we performed a median split to divide the whole sample in high k vs low k subjects. After median split, the high k group included 68 patients and 35 controls, and the low k group included 48 patients and 56 controls.

Results

Replication sample

Effect of money CS in the background (‘money PIT’)

To predict accuracy for trials with money CS backgrounds within the replication sample, we calculated a model including the fixed factors money CS (−2€, −1€, 0€, 1€, 2€, linear), type of instrumental behavior (0.5=collect good shell vs −0.5=leave bad shell) and group (0.5=patients vs −0.5=controls), as well as the random intercept. We found a stronger PIT effect in patients compared to controls for leaving bad shells (Estimate=0.16, z=3.66, P<0.001), suggesting that with an increasing value of background CS, patients compared to controls were less able to accurately refrain from collecting bad shells (Supplementary Figure 1A).

Effect of beverage stimuli in the background (‘beverage PIT’)

To analyze trials with beverage backgrounds, we replaced the fixed factor money CS with the factor beverage (0.5=alcohol vs 0.5=water). Again, we found a stronger PIT effect in patients compared to controls, both for collecting good (Estimate=−0.44, z=−2.93, P=0.003) and leaving bad shells (Estimate=0.51, z=3.65, P<0.001), suggesting that patients compared to controls were less able to correctly collect good shells when alcohol pictures were presented in the background as well as they were less able to correctly leave a bad shell when water pictures were presented (Supplementary Figure 1B).

Full sample

Effect of money CS in the background (‘money PIT’)

To predict accuracy for trials with money CS backgrounds within the full sample, we calculated a model including the fixed factors money CS (−2€, −1€, 0€, 1€, 2€, linear), type of instrumental behavior (0.5=collect good shell vs −0.5=leave bad shell) and group (0.5=patients vs −0.5=controls), as well as the random intercept. We found a significant interaction of money CS × type of instrumental behavior (Estimate=0.91, z=36.62, P<0.001), indicating a PIT effect in both groups: when collecting good shells, subjects responded more often correctly with increasing value of money CS in the background, whereas for leaving bad shells they responded more often correctly with decreasing value of money CS. The significant interaction money CS × type of instrumental behavior × group (Estimate=−0.18, z=−3.69, P<0.001) revealed that patients and controls differed in the strength of PIT. When calculating separate models for collecting good shells and leaving bad shells, we found a significant interaction of money CS × group only in the model for leaving bad shells (Estimate=0.12, z=3.43, P<0.001), suggesting that with an increasing value of background CS, patients compared to controls were less able to accurately leave bad shells (see Figure 2a, left panel).

Figure 2
figure 2

PIT effects are shown as changes of correct responses as a function of Pavlovian background value or drinking backgrounds, respectively. High and low k groups were obtained by median split. (a) Left panel: Patients and controls showed PIT effects: when collecting good shells, participants accuracy increased with increasing value of background CS, while for leaving bad shells, accuracy increased with decreasing CS values. Patients had stronger PIT effects than controls when leaving bad shells. (a) Right panel: Participants collected good shells more correctely when water backgrounds were presented and left bad shells more correctly when alcohol backgrounds were presented. Patients showed a stronger PIT effect than controls when leaving bad shells. (b) Left panel: When collecting good shells, low k controls responded more correctly with increasing value of background CS compared to low k patients. (b) Right panel: We found no significant group differences when beverage backgrounds were presented. (c) Left panel: When confronted with bad shells, high k patients responded less correctly than high k controls with increasing value of background CS. (c) Right panel: When confronted with beverage backgrounds, high k patients showed stronger PIT effects than high k controls when leaving bad shells. CS, conditioned stimuli; PIT, Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer.

Modulation of money PIT by choice impulsivity

Including k (high k vs low k, indicating high vs low impulsive choice behavior) in the above described model revealed a significant three-way interaction between money CS × type of instrumental behavior × k (Estimate=0.31, z=6.19, P<0.001), indicating that impulsive choice behavior modulated the PIT effect. Further, this modulation differed between patients and controls, as suggested by the significant four-way interaction between money CS × type of instrumental behavior × k × group (Estimate=−0.59, z=−5.82, P<0.001). To disentangle this interaction, we computed separate models for collecting good and leaving bad shells for low k and high k participants, on one hand, and for patients and controls on the other. For low k participants collecting good shells, a significant interaction of money CS × group revealed that patients compared to controls were less affected by money CS (Estimate=−0.14, z=2.63, P<0.009; see Figure 2b, left panel). For low k participants leaving bad shells, we found no differences between patients and controls. We found no significant effects for high k subjects collecting good shells, but when high k subjects were confronted with bad shells, patients compared to controls were more affected by money CS (Estimate=0.26, z=4.80, P<0.001; see Figure 2c, left panel). In the model including only patients, we found significant interactions of money CS × k indicating that high k patients were more affected by money CS than low k patients which was true for collecting good (Estimate=0.33, z=6.74, P<0.001) and leaving bad shells (Estimate=−0.31, z=−6.53, P<0.001). In the model including only controls, high k and low k controls did not differ.

Effect of beverage stimuli in the background (‘beverage PIT’)

To analyze trials with beverage backgrounds within the full sample, we calculated a model including the fixed factors beverage (0.5=alcohol vs 0.5=water), type of instrumental behavior (0.5=collect good shell vs −0.5=leave bad shell) and group (0.5=patients vs −0.5=controls) as well as the random intercept. We found a significant beverage × type of instrumental behavior interaction (Estimate=1.63, z=16.88, P<0.001), indicating that a beverage PIT effect was present in both groups: when collecting good shells, subjects responded more often correctly with water pictures in the background, whereas for leaving bad shells they responded more often correctly with alcohol pictures in the background. The significant beverages × type of instrumental behavior × group interaction (Estimate=−0.58, z=−3.87, P<0.001) revealed that patients and controls differed in the PIT strength. Calculating separate models for collecting good and leaving bad shells revealed a significant interaction between beverages × group only in the model for leaving bad shells (Estimate=0.39, z=3.43, P<0.001), suggesting that patients compared to controls were less able to correctly leave a bad shell when water pictures were presented in the background (Figure 2a, right panel).

Modulation of beverage PIT by choice impulsivity

Including k in the model revealed a significant three-way interaction between beverages × type of instrumental behavior × k (Estimate=−1.54, z=6.19, P<0.001), indicating that impulsive choice behavior modulated the beverage PIT effect in patients and in controls. Again, this modulation differed between groups as indicated by the significant four-way interaction between beverages × type of instrumental behavior × k × group (Estimate=0.67, z=2.16, P=0.03). We computed separate models to disentangle this interaction. We found no significant main effects or interactions for low k participants (Figure 2b, right panel). For high k subjects collecting good shells, we found no significant effects while for leaving bad shells, a significant interaction of beverage × group revealed that compared to controls, patients were more affected by beverage backgrounds (estimate=0.48, z=2.83, P=0.005; see Figure 2c, right panel). When computing models including only patients, we found significant interactions between beverage × k, indicating that high k patients were more affected by beverage backgrounds than low k patients when collecting good shells (Estimate=−0.80, z=−5.29, P<0.001) and when leaving bad shells (Estimate=0.89, z=5.96, P<0.001). For controls, we found the same interactions, indicating stronger effects of beverage backgrounds in high- compared to low k controls for collecting good shells (Estimate=−0.54, z=−2.79, P=0.005) and leaving bad shells (Estimate=0.47, z=2.64, P=0.008).

Discussion

We observed stronger PIT effects related to both money CS and beverage stimuli in alcohol-dependent patients compared to controls. In general, positively valenced background stimuli (associated to monetary gains) substantially enhanced accuracy of approach behavior (that is, when collecting good shells) and impaired accuracy when inhibiting approach behavior (that is, when leaving bad shells). Complementary, negatively valenced background stimuli (associated to monetary losses) had the opposite effect on accuracy. Particularly, when patients were confronted with a combination of a negatively valued instrumental cue (for example, a bad shell) and a positively valenced background Pavlovian cue (for example, +1€, +2€, or water), their behavior was strongly driven by those background cues, rendering them less able than controls to appropriately perform the instrumentally learned inhibition. When accounting for impulsive choice behavior as measured by the delay discounting parameter k, we found stronger PIT effects in high impulsive compared to low impulsive participants. Further, the stronger PIT effect in patients compared to controls was only present in the high-impulsive group, whereas low impulsive patients and controls did not differ.

We could successfully replicate our previous finding of stronger PIT effects in alcohol-dependent patients compared to controls in the full sample as well as in a replication sample, for which subjects of previously reported subsamples were excluded. In these previous presented studies, patients were behaviorally more affected by money and by beverage cues,16 while the influence of money cues was positively related to stronger activity in the NAcc only in relapsers, but not in abstainers.17 The current results extend these data by separately analyzing Pavlovian influences on instrumental approach and avoidance behavior. We showed that patients compared to controls were more susceptible to Pavlovian background cues in general, and particularly to cues associated to monetary gains while they should perform an instrumentally learned inhibition. Therefore, Pavlovian effects may automatically and implicitly bias patients to make decisions that are contrary to their previously formed behavioral intention. Translated to everyday risk situations for relapse, this concept may manifest itself as a strong susceptibility to seduction, which can be a crucial aspect in high-risk situations for relapse.

Surprisingly, we found the same effect of failed behavioral inhibition when pictures of water were presented in the background, while pictures of alcohol decreased instrumentally learned approach. Both effects were more pronounced in patients, particularly in high-impulsive patients. Thus, water pictures exerted appetitive effects similarly to CS paired with winning money, while alcohol stimuli resembled CS paired with losing money. We could further confirm this assumption with data from the forced choice trials: participants preferred water pictures over negative (−1€, −2€) and neutral (0€) money CS as well as water and all money CS over alcohol pictures. When asking subjects to choose between alcohol and water pictures, both patients and controls preferred the water picture, while the aversion against alcohol pictures was even stronger in patients.

The unexpected aversion against alcohol pictures in patients might be related to the long stimulus presentation in our task (for example, 3 s in each trial during the PIT part). All studies investigating attentional biases towards alcohol-related cues used shorter presentation times. For instance, data from a visual probe paradigm shows that alcohol-dependent patients exhibit an attentional bias towards alcohol cues (for example, faster detection of a probe that replaces alcohol cues compared to neutral cues) only when the duration of stimulus presentation is short (50 ms). When cues were presented for 500 ms, social drinkers showed an increased attentional bias compared to alcohol-dependent patients, and when stimuli were presented for 1250 ms no attentional bias was found, neither in patients nor in social drinkers.44 Vollstädt-Klein et al.45 further showed that patients, who were abstinent for longer than 2 weeks, exhibited the same patterns in attentional orienting towards alcohol cues as healthy controls. These results suggest a vigilance-avoidance pattern towards alcohol-related stimuli that could also explain the stronger dislike of alcohol pictures in our data. Since our stimuli presentation times were relatively long, patients might have successfully applied attentional avoidance- and cognitive evaluation strategies to process those alcohol-related cues, especially in their current status after recent detoxification and within continuous abstinence-oriented treatment. Another possible explanation is that ‘general’ and ‘specific’ PIT effects exerted by alcohol pictures might differ in their direction: while we observed a general inhibitory effect of alcohol pictures on instrumental responses to money cues, alcohol pictures might still enhance the approach towards alcohol cues, which we did not test (that is, ‘specific’ PIT).

In our study, PIT effects were more pronounced in high-impulsive subjects, and above all in high-impulsive patients. Those patients seem to be particularly sensitive to the impact of Pavlovian cues on ongoing behavior. A possible mechanism underlying this overlap between choice impulsivity and susceptibility to Pavlovian cues might be a generally increased sensitivity to rewards. For instance, Hariri et al.46 reported the data of an fMRI study showing that higher discounting rates were positively correlated with increased ventral striatal activity in response to both, positive and negative feedback in a Striatal reactivity paradigm. Further, in the context of problem drinking and AUD risk, the combination of steep monetary delay discounting and activity patterns of high ventral striatal and low amygdala reactivity where identified as a possible pathway to increased stress-induced drinking behavior.47 In summary, these results imply that impulsive patients with a strong responsiveness to rewards might also be less able to inhibit a behavioral response towards a reward-associated background cue. Accordingly, this higher reward-sensitivity could explain the incentive salience those subjects attribute to Pavlovian cues. Another complementary theoretical approach to explain the susceptibility to Pavlovian cues can be found in the goal- and sign-tracking literature, which describes individual variance in the degree to which Pavlovian cues acquire motivational properties.48, 49 While goal-tracking individuals tend to approach the position of anticipated reward delivery, sign-trackers excessively approach the position where the stimulus predicting the reward is presented. Related to that construct, Tomie et al.50 showed that sign-tracking behavior was associated with higher choice impulsivity: high impulsive compared to low impulsive rats showed remarkably more approach behavior towards the CS. Moreover, sign-tracking as well as impulsive choice behavior further increased after acute administration of ethanol, but only in high impulsive, not in low impulsive animals. In a more recent human study, Garofalo and di Pellegrino51 used eye-tracking to identify goal-and sign-tracking behavior during a PIT paradigm. Correspondingly, they found that sign-trackers showed a stronger PIT effect and higher levels of impulsivity compared to goal-trackers. We assume that the strong PIT effects we observed in high-impulsive patients could rely on related mechanisms. Similar to sign-tracking individuals, high-impulsive patients appear to attribute incentive salience to Pavlovian CS, which motivate reward-related actions and thus interfere with instrumentally learned behavior.

We want to address two limitations to be considered when interpreting our data: First, there are a number of sample characteristics that differ between patients and controls, which possibly could have biased our results. For instance, the socioeconomic status of patients was lower compared to controls. Differences in the socioeconomic status might be important for decision-making in the delay discounting task as well as in the PIT paradigm, because it can affect the subjective value of presented money stimuli.52 Further, we allowed subjects to smoke before the beginning of the experiment. Since nicotine is known to independently affect reward learning53, 54 and impulsivity23 the current smoking state of subjects might have influenced our results. However, due to the complexity of our statistical models we could not control for differences in socioeconomic status or smoking state. Second, the stronger PIT effects in high-impulsive patients might have simply been explained by their poorer instrumental learning. Indeed, patients and particularly high-impulsive patients achieved lower accuracies than controls during instrumental training (Supplementary Figure 2) and therefore might have responded according to values of the money CS by neglecting instrumental learned stimuli during the PIT phase. To test this assumption, we included participants’ achieved accuracy during instrumental training in our model analyzing the PIT phase. Results revealed that instrumental training performance did not affect the stronger PIT effect in patients compared to controls, while this effect remained unchanged when we included impulsivity in the model. Taken together, we conclude that the strong effect of Pavlovian cues on highly impulsive patients’ behavior cannot be explained by poorer instrumental training performance.

In conclusion, our findings indicate that during a PIT paradigm, monetary Pavlovian cues as well as pictures of beverages can strikingly influence instrumentally learned behavior. Particularly, high-impulsive alcohol-dependent patients showed inappropriate approach behavior elicited by contextual cues and therefore failed to correctly inhibit previously learned actions. In real life, this trait might render them highly susceptible to seduction by environmental cues that are incongruent with their behavioral intention to stay abstinent. Such cues can be alcohol-related or alcohol-unrelated, and both occur in what therapists like to call ‘chains of seemingly harmless decisions’.

For example, listening to pleasant music while finding the picture of an old friend may prompt a patient to call that friend, the good food that friend suggests to have at a restaurant might make him accept the friend’s invitation, talking about the fun they had with old drinking buddies might make him go see them in a bar next street, and there ultimately be seduced to accept a drink.

Our results bear two implications to help highly impulsive alcoholics abstain despite their strong PIT effect. In the cognitive domain, they could be informed about their susceptibility to context cues and how these can influence everyday behavior in a way that increases relapse risk. On a more implicit, habitual level of behavioral control, these patients might particularly profit from cue-reactivity training, which aims to relearn automatic responses to cues and replaces approach by withdrawal reactions (for example, Eberl et al.,55 Wiers et al.56).