Introduction

Working memory (WM) refers to the retention and manipulation of information over a brief period of time.1 A gradual increase in WM capacity is strongly related to the development of general intellectual ability2 and academic performance.3 Impaired WM capacity is a core feature of many psychiatric disorders, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder4 and schizophrenia.5 Furthermore, low WM capacity in children is a risk factor for psychopathology later in life, such as psychosis, depression and suicidal ideation.6, 7 WM capacity is also an important factor for emotional self-regulation,8 and restricted self-regulation has been associated with behavioral problems in children, in particular, but not restricted to, externalizing problems (for example, aggression, antisocial behavior).9 Increasing knowledge of the underlying neurobiological mechanisms of typical WM development is an essential step to understand atypical development and guide initiatives to remediate problem behavior.10

Genetic effects on psychiatric phenotypes are often subtle and hard to verify, possibly due to the inter-individual heterogeneity of the phenotype. An alternative approach is to focus on the functional deficits associated with psychiatric disorders, such as WM impairments. However, these cognitive endophenotypes11, 12 are the results of interactions between multiple brain regions. An interesting possibility is that biological measures, such as brain structure and function,13 which are biologically more closely linked to gene activity, may be more useful endophenotypes, with better potential of finding genetic associations.11

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have consistently shown WM-related blood-oxygen level dependent (BOLD) activations in a fronto-parietal network of brain regions, particularly in visuospatial WM (VSWM) paradigms.14 This fronto-parietal activity increases with age from childhood to young adulthood15, 16, 17, 18, 19 and correlates with accuracy on a trial-by-trial basis.17, 20, 21 Although some specific genetic polymorphisms have been associated with individual differences in capacity during this period,22, 23 the genetic basis for WM capacity is still largely unknown. Furthermore, the behavioral consequences of WM-related brain activity, and its association with psychopathological symptoms in particular, have not been reported previously. Therefore, in light of the onsets of certain psychopathologies during childhood and adolescence, elucidating the nature of gene–brain–behavior relationships during development seems critical.

On a molecular level, dopamine (DA) availability affects VSWM function, as demonstrated by experiments in both humans24 and primates.25 It has been suggested that DA regulation of VSWM is subject to developmental changes before adulthood, although for humans there is only indirect evidence from molecular genetic studies.26 Furthermore, performance-related differences in regional brain activity may be partially due to differences in DA signaling in those regions.27, 28 Genes involved in regulation of the DA system are therefore of particular interest, especially candidate genes linked to psychiatric disorders in which WM functioning is typically impaired.29, 30

In the present study, we aimed to investigate VSWM-related brain activity and VSWM capacity as intermediary phenotypes between genetic variation of the DA system and problem behavior. We genotyped single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in and in near vicinity of six genes implicated in DA transmission: COMT, SLC6A3/DAT1, DBH, DRD4, DRD5 and MAOA. Problem behavior was assessed using the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL).31 Higher CBCL scores have previously been associated with low WM performance in preadolescents32 and can predict future onset of psychopathology in children.33 It was hypothesized that BOLD activity in a fronto-parietal brain network would increase during development and be positively associated with VSWM capacity. Furthermore, we expected that polymorphisms in genes of the DA system would affect brain activity in this network and that these genetic effects may be age dependent.22 Finally, we hypothesized that VSWM capacity would predict CBCL scores in a large regional cohort of Swedish children and young adults and, in addition, tested whether genotypes of the DA system genes affecting VSWM brain activity could predict VSWM capacity and CBCL scores.

Methods

Participants

A total of 335 participants from nine different age groups (6, 8, 10, 12, 14, 16, 18, 20 and 25 years) were randomly selected out of a community sample from Nynäshamn in Sweden (‘Brainchild’ study).22, 23 Number of participants was distributed evenly among the different age groups (45 on average), except for the two oldest age groups, including 13 and 8 participants, respectively. Assessments included a baseline measurement (round 1) and a second round of testing 2 years later (round 2). A large majority (88.7%) of the participants and their parents were born in Sweden, 9.3% had at least one parent born outside Sweden, although still within Europe, and the remaining 2% had one or two parents that were born outside of Europe. All participants were enrolled in mainstream schools. Exclusion criteria were first language other than Swedish, diagnosis of neurological or psychiatric disorder (with exception of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or dyslexia). Informed consent was obtained from the participants and from the parents of children aged 18 years or younger. The local ethics committee of the Karolinska University Hospital, Stockholm, approved the study. Additional information on genotyping and participant exclusion can be found in the Supplementary Information.

Behavioral sample

Data on VSWM capacity was available for 329 participants; round 1: n=329 (49.2% male), 12.2 years±4.5 (mean age±s.d.), round 2: n=268 (50.4% male), 13.6 years±4.0. Data on problem behavior was available for participants of age 18 years and younger; round 1: n=260 (49.2% male), 11.0 years±3.3, round 2: n=233 (50.2% male), 12.7 years±3.3. All participants at round 2 had participated in round 1 as well and therefore represented repeated measures.

fMRI sample

Eighty nine participants were randomly selected from all age groups of the behavioral sample to participate in fMRI assessments. In total, 134 measurements were included; round 1: n=68 (51.5%), 13.8 y±5.3, round 2: n=66 (43.9%), 14.9 y±4.7 (including 45 repeated measures of round 1 participants).

Genetic analysis

Material for DNA extraction was collected in form of blood or saliva. Genotyping procedures have been previously published.22, 23 Details of the 18 SNPs used for this study are available in Table 1. For every SNP, two independent scorers confirmed all genotypes, and regenotyping of 5% of the study samples resulted in 100% concordance. All markers were found to be in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, taking autosomal or sex-chromosomal localization of the markers into account.

Table 1 Dopaminergic genes and genotyped SNPs

fMRI

Scan protocol

Subjects were positioned head first and supine in the MR scanner. Images were collected using a 1.5T Siemens Avanto scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany), using an 8-channel radio frequency head coil. For the structural scan, 19 slices were acquired using a T1-weighted 3D anatomical sequence (TR=308 ms, TE=7.8 ms, image matrix=256 × 256, FOV=230 mm, flip angle=90°, slice thickness=5.0 mm, voxel size=1.0 × 0.9 × 5.0 mm). Whole-brain functional images were collected using a T2*-weighted EPI sequences, sensitive to BOLD contrast (TR=3000 ms, TE=50 ms, image matrix=64 × 64, FOV=220 mm, flip angle=90°, slice thickness=4.5 mm, distance factor=11%, voxel size 3.4 × 3.4 × 4.5 mm, 30 axial slices). Each functional MRI run consisted of 96 images, preceded by two dummy scans that were excluded from further analysis.

VSWM paradigm

Participants performed two sessions of the task, each with a total of 16 WM and 16 control trials, in a pseudo-randomized order. Dots were presented sequentially in a four-by-four grid for 500 ms, with 500 ms interval between dots. The task included 50% load 2 and 50% load 4 trials. After 1500 ms delay, a number cue referring to a serial position in the previous stimulus sequence was presented in the grid for 3000 ms. Participants indicated with a yes/no (right index/middle finger) response whether the number and its position in the grid matched. In the control condition, the number cue was 8 and the response was always no, regardless of the position of the question mark. Control and WM trials were indicated by the color of the dots (red: WM trial, yellow: control trial). The response screen remained visible for 3 s, and was followed by a 2 s inter-trial interval. Total duration was 8 s for load 2 and 10 s for load 4 trials. The session started with a 6 s blank screen, resulting in a total duration of 4 min, 54 s for each session. Task, protocol and scanning parameters were exactly the same at both time points.

Data processing

Pre-processing and statistical analyses were carried out with SPM5 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm5) and have previously been published.22, 23 Runs were checked separately and therefore subjects could have one excluded run, and still be included with only one functional run. Functional images were first corrected for slice timing. Motion during scanning was then estimated by six parameters (three translations, three rotations) by minimizing the differences between the first volume and the subsequent volumes. These parameters were then used to realign the functional volumes to the first image of each run (second-degree B-spline interpolation). The mean image of the realigned functional images was co-registered to the anatomical scan, and the estimated co-registration parameters were then used to co-register all functional images. Next, the anatomical image was normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template, and the estimated normalization parameters were used to normalize all functional images. Finally, functional images were high-pass (140 s) filtered and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 12 mm full-width half maximum prior to statistical analysis.

Activations were modeled for each subject separately using a general linear model analysis with four predictors. The predictors were built by convolving the actual onset times of the experimental events with the standard hemodynamic response function. The four experimental events consisted of the correct trials only: working memory trials with load 2 and load 4, and control trials with load 2 and load 4. The duration of the events was set to include the time of maintenance and response, and was therefore equal to 8 s in load 2 trials and 10 s in load 4 trials. Time and dispersion derivatives, as well as the motion parameters and the global mean, were added in the design. With this model, one contrast image, ‘WM versus control’, was made for each subject by contrasting both working memory conditions with both control conditions.

These ‘WM versus control’ contrast images were used in flexible factorial design second-level analyses, which modeled whether the contrasts were from the same or different participants by including subject and testing round as factors. Regions of interest (ROI) based on local maxima were defined and extracted using MarsBaR.34 First, ROIs were created based on all activations from the contrast ‘WM versus control’, thresholded at FDR (false discovery rate) P<0.000001. These ROIs were then split into smaller regions, so that each ROI was centered around a single local maximum. ROIs were defined so that individual regions did not overlap, and together covered the majority of the working memory activation. Resulting ROIs were plotted on a surface-based human atlas (PALS)35, 36 using the Caret software37 (www.nitrc.org/projects/caret/). For each ROI, the average WM–control BOLD of all voxels within that ROI was calculated for each participant.

Behavioral assessment

VSWM capacity

Participants completed a large neuropsychological battery administered individually. VSWM capacity was assessed using a visuospatial grid task (dot matrix) from the AWMA battery38 similar to the VSWM fMRI paradigm (see refs 22, 23 for a detailed description).

Problem behavior

Parental reports were used to assess behavioral problems in 4–18-year-old children on the CBCL.31, 39 Three broadband scales (which assess a wide variety of behavioral symptoms) were used: total problems and the two subscales internalizing and externalizing problems. These last two scales largely correspond to mood/anxiety and disruptive behavior disorders, respectively.

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed with linear mixed models, using the PASW 18.0 statistical package.22, 40 This approach allows all data to be included within a single model, even when some participants have only attended one testing session. A compound symmetry covariance structure was used, one parameter corresponding to the variance within sessions, and the other to the covariance between sessions. In the fMRI sample, BOLD activity and VSWM capacity were entered as dependent variables in separate analyses. In the behavioral sample, VSWM capacity and problem behavior (CBCL T-scores) were used as dependent measures in separate analyses. Effects were considered significant at the level of P<0.05. Bonferroni-corrected P-values were applied when testing for genetic effects.

Results

fMRI

In-scanner performance

Overall accuracy was high, but participants showed lower accuracy in the WM than the control condition (WM: mean 90.0%±11.0; control: 98.3%±3.5) (t(134)=10.15, P<0.001), as well as slower reaction time (correct trials only, WM: 1272 ms±352; control: 737 ms±212) (t(134)=26.11, P<0.001). The difference between WM and control conditions decreased with age for both accuracy and reaction time (main effect of age: F(1,73.9)=14.95, P<0.001, and F(1,90.3)=35.0, P<0.001).

Main effect WM–control

Based on activity in the main effect of WM minus control (P<0.05, FDR), a total of 17 individual ROIs were identified and delineated: 8 frontal, 4 parietal, 3 occipital and 2 in the bilateral caudate nucleus (see Table 2).

Table 2 ROIs for the main effect of working memory–control

Principal component analysis (PCA)

ROI data were split per assessment (round 1 and 2) and transformed into a Z-score. All bilateral ROIs correlated higher with each other (all >0.7) than with other ROIs, and were collapsed to prevent multicollinearity in the data. In order to further reduce the number of comparisons, BOLD data for each ROI was entered into a PCA.

Factors were rotated using the direct oblimin method, assuming that underlying factors are not completely independent. An eigenvalue of 0.7 was used as cut-off for factor extraction and all communalities had to be 0.7 or higher after extraction.41 Initial PCA led to exclusion of the left cuneus ROI, due to a low communality (0.4). After removal of this ROI, the eight remaining ROIs (see Table 2) were used for PCA. Two-, three- and four-factor solutions were explored. The three-factor solution (F1, F2, F3; see Figure 1 and Table 2) accounted for 80.7% of the total variance and was considered most optimal. Running PCA separately for round 1 and 2 resulted in the same factor structures for both time points. Composite scores were created for each factor, based on the means of the ROIs with factor loadings >0.5 in the final pattern matrix (highlighted in bold values in Table 2).

Figure 1
figure 1

Render of delineated regions of interest (ROIs) of the (fMRI) main effect working memory (WM)–control (false discovery rate (FDR), P<0.05) on a surface-based human atlas (see Methods). Top row: Factor 1 ROIs with lateral and medial view of the left and right hemisphere. Bottom row: Factor 2 ROIs with lateral and medial view of the left and right hemisphere.

VSWM activity, age, sex and VSWM capacity

The mean BOLD value for each factor was regressed against three possible Z-score-transformed age variables: age (ageZ), the inverse of age (age−1Z), or the natural logarithm of age (ln(age)Z), using linear mixed models, to assess the best fit for developmental changes in VSWM-related BOLD activity. Sex was entered as a main effect and sex × age as an interaction effect. Age−1Z provided the best fit for all factors based on the Akaike's information criterion.42 There was a significant increase in BOLD activity for all three factors with age (F1: F(1, 84.6)=45.68, P<0.001; F2: F(1, 88.5)=5.58, P=0.020; F3: F(1, 84.9)=12.71, P=0.001), (Figure 2a). There was an additional main effect of sex for F2 (F(1, 72.4)=4.70, P=0.033) with males showing higher activity levels than females. No significant sex × age interaction was observed.

Figure 2
figure 2

(a): Scatterplot of the mean blood-oxygen level dependent (BOLD) working memory (WM)–control values plotted against age for all three factors. Black lines connect repeated measures for individuals who participated in both round 1 and 2. Red lines represent the mean predicted BOLD signal as a function of age, as obtained from the mixed-model analysis using the inverse of age as a fixed effect. (b) Scatterplot of the mean BOLD WM–control values of Factor 1 against Visuospatial WM (VSWM) capacity. Black lines connect repeated measures. The red line represents the mean-predicted BOLD signal for each age group, as obtained from the mixed-model analysis using the inverse of age as a factor.

All three BOLD factors were independently tested for an association with VSWM capacity. Age−1Z and sex were included as main effects in the model. Only F1 was significantly predicted by VSWM capacity (F1: F(1, 111.6)=8.72, P=0.003) and only when age−1Z was not included in the model (Figure 2b). No significant interactions between BOLD factors, age−1Z or sex were observed. This showed that, although activity increased with age in all factors, only activation within F1 regions was associated with VSWM capacity during development.

Genotypes and VSWM activity

Linear mixed models with age−1Z, sex, genotype and genotype × age−1Z entered as covariates were used to test for genetic effects on VSWM-BOLD activity. Additive models were used for testing the effect of each genotype (for example, 0=AA, 1=AG, 2=GG for the rs4680 located in the COMT gene). Similar additive models were used for X-chromosomal SNPs located in the MAOA (monoamine oxidase A) gene by grouping men with homozygous females, rendering the heterozygote group 100% female. Any significant main or interaction effects for these SNPs were followed up by sex-specific post hoc tests. Due to a minor allele frequency < 10% within the fMRI sample, three SNPs (DRD5: rs10939515; SLC6A3/DAT1: rs27072 and rs3863145) were re-coded to pool the rare homozygotes with the heterozygotes. To limit the number of tests, only F1 was analyzed, as this was the only BOLD factor found to be associated with VSWM capacity.

Two of eighteen SNPs, rs979606 and rs6609257 (both located within or near the MAOA gene, see Figure 3), showed a main effect of genotype on F1, with only the latter surviving Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (F(1, 75.7)=11.52, P=0.001). The same SNP (rs6609257, A>G) was the only one to show an interaction effect for genotype × age−1Z, but this effect did not survive Bonferroni correction (P=0.02). Mean-predicted values of F1 as a function of rs6609257 indicated that the A allele was associated with higher levels of VSWM-BOLD activity than the G allele (Figure 4). No main effect of sex was found, and post hoc testing did not show any effects when interactions with sex were added to the model (that is, age−1Z × sex, genotype × sex, age−1Z × sex × genotype). When the model was applied separately for males (n=64) and females (n=70), the main effect of genotype was significant for males, F(1, 35.4)=6.62, P=0.014, with A males showing higher levels of WM activation than G males. For females, there was a trend (P=0.08) in the same direction (AA and AG versus GG).

Figure 3
figure 3

Factor 1 BOLD (blood-oxygen level dependent) working memory (WM)–control contrast displayed for the MAOA single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs6609257. This chart is included for illustrative purposes and represents the means and s.e.m. (collapsing across longitudinal and cross-sectional data) of F1 mean working memory (WM)–control BOLD signal. Participants tested in both rounds were therefore included as two data points. The size of each group of genotypes is: 60 (hemizygotes A and homozygotes AA), 32 (heterozygotes AG) and 42 (hemizygotes G and homozygotes GG). The heterozygote (AG) group only represents female participants, as the MAOA gene is located on the X-chromosome. All hemizygote genotypes are from male participants who have either a single A or G allele.

Figure 4
figure 4

Chromosome X region (top) containing the genotyped single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with corresponding linkage disequilibrium (LD) values. The variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) previously shown to regulate MAOA activity is located approximately 1000 bases upstream of the MAOA transcription start site. Protein-coding exons are depicted as vertical lines and the 5′ and 3′ untranslated regions as filled black boxes. Rs6609257, significantly associated to visuospatial working memory (VSWM) after Bonferroni correction in this study, is highlighted in bold. The bottom panel shows the calculated LD between pairs of SNPs as percentage of the theoretical maximal LD with logarithm of likelihood odds ratio (D’/LOD) using Haploview 4.2. All SNPs are in high or complete LD with statistical significance. Values are based on our own ‘Brainchild’ sample set. Black squares without text depict complete (100%) LD.

MAOA, VSWM capacity and problem behavior

To test whether the identified MAOA SNP affecting VSWM-BOLD, rs6609257, also directly affected VSWM capacity in the behavioral sample, linear mixed models were used. Age−1Z, sex, genotype and genotype × age were entered in the model. There was a strong main effect of age−1Z (F(1, 507.3)=233.80, P<0.001), and no additional effect of sex, genotype or interaction effect of age−1Z × genotype on VSWM capacity. Further exploration of main and interaction effects after splitting the group for sex did not affect the outcome.

We next tested whether rs6609257 predicted problem behavior. Sex and age were excluded from the model because CBCL T-scores are corrected for these variables based on population norms. There was no main effect of genotype on total, internalizing or externalizing problems. Finally, we analyzed whether VSWM capacity was able to predict problem behavior when entered as a single fixed effect in the model. VSWM capacity significantly predicted level of externalizing problems (F(1, 492.3)=4.11, P=0.043), with low WM associated with more externalizing problems, and at trend-level for total problems (P=0.08).

The main results of this study are combined and illustrated by means of a heuristic model in Figure 5.

Figure 5
figure 5

Heuristic hierarchical model summarizing the main findings in our study. Solid black lines represent significant associations, the dashed black line indicates a trend-level association and blue lines marked with a cross refer to non-significant associations. Relevant P-values are provided adjacent to each line in the model with significant effects printed in bold lettertype.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the role of VSWM development, both in terms of behavior and brain activity, as an intermediate phenotype linking DA-related genes to problem behavior. VSWM-related brain activity in fronto-parieto-occipital regions (Factor 1) increased during development and was positively associated with VSWM capacity. Brain activity in this network was modulated by genotype for one SNP, rs6609257, located 6.6 kb downstream of the MAOA gene on Xp11.23 (see Figure 3). On a behavioral level, we found that lower levels of externalizing problems were directly related to higher WM capacity.

MAO genes (MAOA and MAOB, separated by 19.8 kb on human chromosome X, in a head-to-head orientation) encode enzymes that are expressed throughout the brain and affect multiple monoaminergic systems by degrading DA, norepinephrine and serotonin.43 Previous studies have highlighted potential modulating effects of an MAOA variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) in the MAOA promoter region (MAOA VNTR 30-bp repeat) with high- and low-activity variants.44, 45, 46, 47 This VNTR and a previously identified haplotype of three MAOA polymorphisms (rs12843268, rs3027400 and rs1137070) have also been linked specifically to VSWM cognition and brain activity.48, 49 Furthermore, on a behavioral level, the MAOA VNTR has been associated with traits such as depression50 and aggression,51 although ambiguously.52, 53, 54 Our study is consistent with the involvement of MAOA polymorphisms in regulating neurobiological mechanisms associated with VSWM capacity, which in turn can predict maladaptive behavior.

The main effect of rs6609257 in this study showed that carriers of the A allele had higher VSWM brain activity in fronto-parieto-occipital brain regions. There was no significant age × genotype interaction, although visual inspection of the data suggested a stronger age-related increase in brain activity individuals with an A allele, in particular for males. Because of its genomic localization, 6.6 kb downstream of the MAOA gene and 13 kb upstream of the neighboring MAOB gene, one cannot exclude that rs6609257 also has an effect on MAOB. Informatics analysis in the UCSC Genome Browser (hg18 assembly) showed that rs6609257 is located in a genomic region devoid of annotated transcripts. It is currently unknown whether there are any functional promoters or enhancers in this region affecting the brain. However, it is of modest evolutionary conservation and it does not overlap with predicted RNA secondary structures typical for non-coding RNA (EvoFold annotation). Therefore, it is unlikely that the region contains undiscovered coding elements.

To our knowledge, rs6609257 is a genetic marker that has not previously been associated with VSWM activity or capacity. However, it potentially tags the previously mentioned functional MAOA VNTR 30-bp repeat. The high-activity variant of the VNTR has been associated with higher enzyme expression and lower amine concentration,55 as well as increased prefrontal brain activity44, 45, 46, 48, 49 and lower levels of aggression.54 Our results suggest that the A allele of rs6609257 may act in a similar way for VSWM brain activation. Because information about VNTR repeats was not available for our participants, CEU population (Caucasians) data from the International Hapmap Project (http://hapmap.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/) were examined for linkage disequilibrium between a SNP 3 kb distant from the VNTR (rs4570308) and rs6609257. The SNP rs6609257 is on the far end of a major linkage disequilibrium block spanning the MAOA gene region also including the VNTR and rs4570308 markers. The two SNPs are in high but not complete linkage disequilibrium (D’=0.84). However, as the allele frequencies in our total study population and the HapMap CEU population are similar (HapMap: A allele=60%, G allele=40%; Brainchild: A allele=59%, G allele=41%), rs6609257 or any other informative SNP located in MAOA could potentially act as a surrogate for the tandem repeat.52 Our results require replication to answer this question and future studies on MAOA and VSWM could benefit from examining potential haplotypes including both VNTR and SNP polymorphisms.

Theoretically, X-linked genes have an increased likelihood of sex-specific functions. However, in vivo research indicates that MAOA and MAOB protein levels in the human brain are the same for both sexes.56, 57 Likewise, our results indicated a minimal influence of sex on outcome. However, when data were split into separate groups for males and females, the genetic effect on VSWM brain activity was significant only for males and at trend-level for females. Interestingly, we were unable to detect an effect of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism (rs4680) on ROI brain activity, contrary to earlier findings using a voxel-based approach in participants 18 years or younger,22 or in adult populations.58 However, the lack of COMT rs4680 findings here are likely to reflect differences in methodology, as our previous study22 revealed small parietal and frontal clusters showing an effect of genotype, whereas the current study focused on larger ROI analyses, based on the broad network of brain regions activated during VSWM.

The fMRI findings from this longitudinal study are in line with cross-sectional VSWM fMRI findings showing that increased activation in fronto-parieto-occipital regions is linked to VSWM performance.15, 16, 59, 60, 61 The observed non-linear increase in regional brain activation likely reflects functional maturation,15, 16, 62 and could, speculatively, be an indicator of functional specialization (that is, a relative increase in domain-specific neural activity) of relevant task-related brain regions. To address this, future studies could, for example, compare regional brain activation across multiple cognitive paradigms in a longitudinal developmental sample. Furthermore, the fronto-parieto-occipital regions highlighted in our study are part of the dorsal attention network, which is essential for goal-directed (top-down) attention.63 Other regions, including bilateral caudate, inferior frontal gyrus/sulcus, pre-SMA and middle frontal gyrus showed a moderate increase of VSWM brain activation with age, but were not related to VSWM capacity when age was removed as a factor from the model. It is possible, however, that using a higher WM load during scanning might have resulted in an association of BOLD values and capacity also in these regions.

In our study, genetic effects for rs6609257 were strongest on the level of brain activation and did not directly affect VSWM capacity or behavioral problems. This indicates a higher sensitivity of neuroimaging measures to genotypic variation, relative to behavioral measures. Polymorphisms could, however, contribute indirectly to behavioral problems by altering neuromolecular properties required for optimal VSWM function. Cognitive endophenotypes, such as VSWM, may partially mediate such effects, but this requires further investigation. The present study included a community sample with low problem behavior variability and did not reveal direct associations between genotypes of DA system genes and problem behavior. We believe future work in populations with (a high-risk for) psychiatric disorders may be more sensitive to such effects. In sum, the findings from our study provide evidence that VSWM brain activity measures could be used as a powerful phenotype for linking MAO genes to problem behavior in the general population.