Introduction

It has been known for over a century that the stride interval of human gait is remarkably stable. Small fluctuations of approximately 3–4% were attributed to the complexity of the locomotor system and treated as an uncorrelated random process1. From this viewpoint, the discovery of long-time, fractal correlations in stride-interval time series was unexpected2,3. Those early papers not only spurred interest in the emerging concept of fractal physiology4, but also shifted the focus of quantitative gait analysis from average values of typical parameters (e.g. stride intervals) to their temporal variability. That profound change of perspective brought new insights into locomotor manifestations of Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases, aging, and the connection between gait dynamics and fall risk, see ref. 5 and references therein.

From a plethora of physiologically accessible gait patterns, humans employ only walking and running. Walking feels easiest at low speeds, and running feels easiest when moving faster. Optimization of energy cost underlies not only the choice of gait6,7,8, but also determines average values of gait parameters, such as step length and duration9. During walking, humans need to adapt their spatio-temporal gait parameters at every step to be able to respond to exogenous (e.g. irregularities of walking surface) and/or endogenic (neuromuscular noise) perturbations10. While some neurological mechanisms that trigger these responses are known11,12,13,14, the fundamental principles governing step-by-step adaptation remain elusive15.

Treadmill walking, especially at high speeds, presents challenges that can be met only through effective short-term control of spatio-temporal gait parameters. In order to stay on a treadmill, the subject’s step duration and length must yield a step speed which can fluctuate over a narrow range centered on the treadmill belt’s speed. The results of previous experiments with walking on a split-belt treadmill underscore the intricacies of such aggregation. In particular, spatial and temporal controls of locomotion are accessible through distinct neural circuits16 and neural control of intra- versus interlimb parameters (calculated using values from both legs, e.g., step length, double support) during walking is to a large extent independent17. Herein, we investigate the dynamics of time series of gait parameters (step duration, length and speed) following a sudden, large deviation from the mean value. In particular, we test the hypothesis that whenever the value of a gait parameter is markedly greater (smaller) than the mean value, it is either immediately followed by a smaller (greater) value of the contralateral leg (interleg control), or the deviation from the mean value decreases during the next movement of ipsilateral leg (intraleg control). Said differently, during treadmill walking errors are not gradually attenuated via long-term corrections, but are corrected immediately by the same or opposite leg. Taking into account differences in the relative contribution of lower limbs to control and propulsion – the effect known as functional gait asymmetry18, we further hypothesize that in subjects with right foot preference the short-term control of gait spatio-temporal parameters is stronger for the right leg.

Results

In Table 1 we recorded data which are crucial for testing the main hypothesis of the paper: that errors in gait parameters are corrected immediately by the same or opposite leg. Let us focus on the first row of this table which concerns step duration control at a treadmill speed of 1.1 m/s. There were 180 “errors” defined as abrupt changes in step duration (equations 2,3,4). For the left leg, in 42 cases (column L-L), the deviation of step duration from the mean value did not trigger a compensating change in the step duration of the right leg. The value of the control parameter DLR, defined by equation 5, greater than 1 indicates the absence of such adjustment. However, the deviation decreased during the next left step as indicated by the value of intraleg control parameter DLL, defined by equation 7, smaller than 1 (the statistics of both inter- and intraleg control parameters are presented in Table 2). In other words, for the left leg, we observed intraleg control of step duration in 42 cases. In 51 cases (column R-L), the change in the step duration of the left leg compensated the deviation of step duration of the previous right step (interleg control). By adding columns L-L and R-L, we obtain 93 control events performed by the left leg. This number expressed as the percentage of all 180 control events is given in the column of Table 1 labeled as Left. Please note that only in 4 (column NC) out of 180 cases (2.2%), the appearance of a step duration error did not evoke either of the control mechanisms.

Table 1 The statistics of the occurrence of intraleg (columns L-L and R-R) and interleg (columns R-L and L-R) control mechanisms that are evoked in response to errors in step duration, length and velocity.
Table 2 The values of intra- and interleg control parameters for treadmill walking.

For all three gait parameters, the number of errors decreases with speed. For example, there were 180 errors in step duration at v = 1.1 m/s but only 103 at v = 1.7 m/s (a 43% reduction). A comparable drop in the number of errors was observed for step length (51%) and velocity (52%). Less than half of these changes can be explained by the 22% reduction of the number of steps taken by all the subjects at v = 1.7 m/s in comparison with v = 1.1 m/s. Please note that the number of steps decreases with treadmill speed since at each speed, the subjects were asked to cover the same distance of 400 m. It is worth emphasizing that there were approximately twice as many errors in step length and speed than in step duration. For all three parameters: step duration, length, and speed, in at least 90% of cases, the deviations from the mean value decreased during the subsequent two steps via either intra- or interleg control.

Regardless of treadmill belt speed, the control of step length is predominately intraleg (Table 1). For example, for the left leg at the lowest speed, the probability of evoking intraleg control (pLL = 0.33 is 42% greater than that of interleg control pRL = 0.19). In the same condition, for the right leg such difference is equal to 59% (pRR = 0.32 vs pLR = 0.13). There is no such pattern for the other two gait parameters.

For step duration, length, and speed, the control parameter D was independent of speed (Table 2). For all three gait parameters, both for the right and left leg, the mean value of D for interleg control was greater than that of intraleg control. For example, for step duration at v = 1.1 m/s DRL = 0.55 and DLL = 0.34.

The difference between the values of intra- and interleg control parameters for a given leg was statistically significant for all three treadmill speeds for step length:

  • at 1.1 m/s: pleft < 1 × 10−4, pright = 6 × 10−3;

  • at 1.4 m/s: pleft < 1 × 10−4, pright = 3 × 10−2;

  • at 1.7 m/s: pleft = 1 × 10−4, pright = 2 × 10−3;

as well as the step speed:

  • at 1.1 m/s: pleft < 1 × 10−4, pright = 6 × 10−4;

  • at 1.4 m/s: pleft < 1 × 10−4, pright < 1 × 10−4;

  • at 1.7 m/s: pleft = 2 × 10−3, pright = 9 × 10−3.

For step duration such differences were not so strongly pronounced:

  • at 1.1 m/s: pleft = 5 × 10−4, pright = 5 × 10−2;

  • at 1.7 m/s: pleft = 2 × 10−3.

With the exception of step duration and step length at the lowest speed (v = 1.1 m/s), the asymmetry parameter ΔDBE was smaller than zero indicating a dominant role of the right leg in short-term control of gait parameters during treadmill walking.

Table 3 shows the probability of compensatory response to errors in gait spatio-temporal parameters for intra- (L-L, R-R) and interleg (L-R, R-L) control. Such response corresponds to negative values of variables Sinter (equation 6) and Sintra (equation 8). For all speeds and parameters, the probability of interleg compensation is close to 1, roughly two times higher than that of intraleg response.

Table 3 Probability of compensation of errors in gait spatio-temporal parameters for intra- (L-L, R-R) and interleg (L-R, R-L) control.

Discussion

In overground walking with self-selected speed, fluctuations of stride interval, length, and speed exhibit persistent fractal scaling characterized by a Hurst exponent α > 0.52,3,19. Auditory metronomic cueing changes fractal statistics of stride intervals from persistent to antipersistent (α < 0.5)20. The super central pattern generator model, introduced by West and Scafetta21, elucidates the dynamic origin of such transitions. In particular, the transitions result from the driving of a fractal clock, which retains its properties under perturbation. In treadmill walking, fluctuations of interstride interval and stride length are also persistent. However, the time series of stride speed is antipersistent, which is a manifestation of increased central control of this gait parameter15,22. Terrier has recently demonstrated that visual cueing (alignment of step lengths with marks on the floor) also induced anti-correlated pattern in gait parameters23.

To a large extent, fluctuations of spatio-temporal gait parameters result from the intrinsic fractal properties of pattern generators. Hidden in these fluctuations are sporadic control events, triggered to accomplish a locomotor task such as remaining on a moving treadmill belt. This is why we study the dynamics of time series of gait parameters that follow a sudden large deviation from a mean value. For lack of a better word, we dubbed such events errors, but emphasize that they may originate either from the failure of the motor control system, or from the necessary adjustment of the subject’s position on a treadmill. While the definition of such events is arbitrary (equations 2,3,4), it satisfies the research objective.

We found that when the value of the gait parameter (step duration, length or speed) was conspicuously greater (smaller) than the mean value, it was either followed immediately by a smaller (greater) value of the contralateral leg (interleg control), or the deviation from the mean value decreased during the next movement of ipsilateral leg (intraleg control). The existence of distinct short-term control of step frequency (the inverse of step duration) was demonstrated by Snaterse et al.24. The time evolution of step frequency triggered by sudden stepwise increments in treadmill speed was modeled by the sum of two exponentially decaying terms. The time constant of the first term was 1.44 ± 1.14 s and its amplitude was two times larger than that of the second term, whose time constant was 27.56 ± 16.18 s. For those values of time constants, step frequency adjustments were two-thirds complete in less than two seconds. Snaterse et al. argued that the first term represents a rapid pre-programmed response, while the slower one models fine-tuning of step frequency driven by energy expenditure optimization. Herein we extended this line of reasoning by demonstrating that short-term control of gait parameters may be realized using intra- and interleg adjustments. The better understanding of short-term control mechanisms does not bring us any closer to understanding how, during treadmill walking, persistent stochastic variables: step duration and step length are combined to yield antipersistent step speed. We believe that a different mechanism operating at a longer time scale underlies this effect.

There are fundamental differences between the control of step duration and step length. The probability of evoking intraleg control of step length at the highest treadmill speed (v = 1.7 m/s) is approximately three times greater than that of evoking interleg control. There is no such distinct pattern for step duration. Moreover, the number of errors in step duration is half that of step length, regardless of treadmill belt speed. This is a strong indication that spatial and temporal controls of locomotion are accessible through distinct neural circuits. This interpretation is corroborated by the earlier study of Malone and Bastian, who investigated adaptation of spatial and temporal aspects of walking to a sustained perturbation, generated by a split-belt treadmill17. They demonstrated that conscious correction facilitates adaptation, whereas distraction slows it. The unexpected finding of their study was that those manipulations affected the adaptation rate of the spatial elements of walking, but not of the temporal ones. In the follow-up study Malone et al.25 demonstrated that temporal and spatial controls of symmetric gait can be adapted independently. Please note that continuous, conscious assessment of distance to surrounding objects lies at the heart of the control problem of remaining stationary on a moving treadmill belt. Thus, the large number of errors in step length as compared to step duration may reflect both the dominant role of spatial control and its susceptibility to distraction. It is worth mentioning that in casual walking, the coefficient of variation of stride time is much smaller than that of stride length and of walking speed9.

Step speed may be interpreted as the output of the intricate neuromuscular control system, which integrates different sensory-motor processes. The ratio of average values of step length and frequency, or walk ratio, is constant over a broad range of walking speeds. In other words, there is a linear relation between these gait parameters (the stride length – cadence relationship), a pre-programmed pattern which presumably simplifies gait control in steady state walking26. Let us analyze the interplay of step duration and step length during transient changes following the occurrence of errors. We previously pointed out that these two parameters are controlled in distinct ways. In particular, the probability of evoking the interleg control of step length is at least two times smaller than that of evoking the intraleg control (Table 1). In sharp contrast, the probability of either inter- or intraleg control of step speeds is comparable. Thus, we may hypothesize that negative-feedback adjustment of step duration of the contralateral leg underlies the interleg control of step speed. It is worth emphasizing that the intraleg control of step speed is stronger than the interleg control.

The recent work of Dingwell et al.15 provides insight into the maintenance of speed during treadmill walking. A subject can in principle choose any combination of stride length and time that yields step speed equal to that of a treadmill belt. These pairs of values form in phase-space a diagonal line called a goal equivalent manifold (GEM)27. Dingwell et al. decomposed deviation from this manifold into tangent and transverse components. Only the latter component was tightly controlled. Moreover, the time series of transverse deviations exhibited statistical antipersistence characteristic of stride speed. This study underscores the significance of interleg control of gait parameters. We believe that the GEM decomposition should be applied to time series of step velocities to quantify the interleg control in a more sophisticated way.

In able-bodied gait, asymmetry in spatio-temporal and kinematic parameters (such as speed profiles, step and stride length, foot placement angle, maximum knee flexion) for the left and right leg has been frequently reported28. To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first observation of asymmetry in dynamics of human gait parameters. With the exception of step duration control at the lowest speed, for all three gait parameters ΔDBE < 0, indicating dominance of the right leg in short-term control. The origin of this asymmetry can be traced back to differences in the relative contribution of lower limbs to control and propulsion – the effect known as functional gait asymmetry18. More specifically, the leg with greater muscle power generation dominates propulsion, while the support and control functions are more conspicuous for the leg with greater power absorption. Humans are typically right-footed for mobilization and left-footed for postural stabilization.

Special consideration should be given to step duration and step length control at the lowest speed v = 1.1 m/s. Only in this case, the asymmetry parameter ΔDBE was greater than zero, indicating the dominance of left lower limb. Note that the lowest asymmetry, |ΔDBE|, was observed for all three parameters at v = 1.1 m/s. Differences in low-speed gait have been reported before. Terrier and Schutz29 demonstrated that during overground walking, at low speeds the majority of subjects adopted a higher walk ratio and had a higher variability of stride time. However, in this study the lowest treadmill speed coincides with the preferred walking speed (PWS) of young subjects22. There are two possible explanations for the positive value of ΔDBE. It is likely that in the vicinity of PWS priority is given to balance maintenance and consequently stride duration control is shifted to the left leg, which is used for postural stabilization. Please note that our cohort included only subjects with clearly pronounced right foot preference. Alternatively, reversed asymmetry for step duration and low values of |ΔDBE| for step length and step speed may indicate that there exists a different strategy for control of gait parameters in overground walking (treadmill walking at v = 1.1 m/s may not be challenging for young subjects and may resemble unconstrained overground walking). This argument is plausible because in motor coordination tasks, humans correct only those deviations that interfere with task goals and allow variability in redundant (task-irrelevant) dimensions30. Following the logic of this minimum intervention principle, in treadmill walking, step speed must be tightly regulated. However, in overground walking, higher priority may be given, for example, to balance control, which would affect the value of the asymmetry parameter ΔDBE. These two qualitatively different strategies may also reflect other fundamental differences between overground and treadmill walking. The rate at which the environment flows past the eyes seems to be an important mechanism for regulating walking speed31,32. More specifically, vision is used correctively to maintain walking speed at a value that is perceived to be optimal. For treadmill walking, a discrepancy between observed and expected visual flow leads to a significant reduction (about 20%) of PWS33, as well as the speeds of walk-run and run-walk transitions31. It is worth pointing out that as far as kinetic and kinematic parameters are concerned, treadmill and outdoor gaits are similar34.

The discovery of dependence of functional asymmetry in short-term control of gait spatio-temporal parameters on treadmill speed was an unexpected outcome of this research. The elucidation of the transition from left-leg to right-leg dominance in short-term control entails determination of the PWS for each subject. Further research is also needed to understand why the probability of compensatory response for interleg control is close to 1 and is almost two times greater than that of intraleg control (Table 3). Undoubtedly, such a strong difference indicates different roles these two mechanisms play in control of gait during treadmill walking. One may hypothesize that the primary goal of interleg control is maintenance of balance via negative feedback from either leg while achieving specific goals such as matching the speed of the treadmill belt requires intraleg adjustments.

During human locomotion, the legs act as two coupled oscillators35. However, most studies disregard bilateral coordination and synchronization dynamics36,37,38 and focus on single-leg variability (stride time, length, speed). Herein we demonstrated asymmetric short-term intra- and interleg control of spatio-temporal gait parameters. We believe that a better understanding of these effects will not only pave the way for more realistic models of gait variability and control, but also help to refine procedures used in rehabilitation of gait impairments.

Methods

We recruited 20 healthy students (10 M/10 F, mean(SD): age 22 yr (2), height 1.73 m (0.1), weight 71 (15) kg, BMI 23 (4)) of the Wroclaw University of Science and Technology, who all signed an informed consent. The study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Wroclaw Medical University. The subjects were screened to exclude those with a history of orthopedic problems, recent lower extremity injuries, any visible gait anomalies, or who were taking medications that might have influenced their gait. We only enrolled subjects who used the right leg to: kick a tennis ball, manipulate a tennis ball around a circle, make a first step, make a step after being pushed from behind. These purely bilateral tasks are frequently incorporated into foot-preference inventories39,40. The protocol began with a 5 min familiarization period of walking on a level motor-driven treadmill. Then each subject was asked to walk 400 m three times at 1.1 m/s, 1.4 m/s i 1.7 m/s (4 km/h, 5 km/h and 6 km/h). The objective was to investigate control of gait parameters at treadmill speeds equal to or greater than the PWS of young subjects. Therefore, the lowest speed was equal to the preferred walking speed reported by Terrier and Deriaz22 and slightly smaller than the values determined by Dal et al.33 (1.19 m/s) and Dingwell10 (1.22 m/s).

The gait parameters were extracted from the trajectories of the 30 mm optical markers attached to both shoes below the ankle. The movements of those markers were recorded using an in-house motion capture system with a frame rate of 240 Hz and 720 p resolution. The optical tracking was implemented in C++ (Visual Studio 2013) using OpenCV library. A heel strike was defined as the point where the marker of the forward foot was at its most forward point during each gait cycle. A step length was the distance between the ipsilateral and contralateral heel strikes. A step duration was equal to the elapsed time between the ipsilateral and contralateral heel strikes. A step speed was calculated as the quotient of step length and step duration. The group averaged number of steps taken per trial was equal to 593 (23) at 1.1 m/s, 508 (59) at 1.4 m/s, and 456 (56) at 1.7 m/s.

In Fig. 1 we present a time series of step duration for treadmill walking at 1.1 m/s. The circle in this figure indicates step duration that was longer than the mean value (represented in this figure by horizontal, thick, dotted line) by more than 3/2 of standard deviation (the upper, horizontal, thin dotted gridline represents this threshold). It is apparent that the duration of the step, which immediately follows the “error”, suddenly decreases (this shorter interval is marked by the filled rectangle). This example hints at the existence of an interleg control mechanism that stabilizes the stride interval.

Figure 1: Time series of step durations for treadmill walking at 1.1 m/s.
figure 1

The circle indicates the step duration of left leg which was longer than the mean value (represented in this figure by horizontal, thick, dotted line) by more than 3/2 of standard deviation (the upper, horizontal, thin dotted gridline represents this threshold). It is apparent that the duration of the step which immediately follows the “error” suddenly decreases.

Let N be the number of steps taken by each leg. Let us introduce a notation that facilitates the analysis of interleg control. We write the time series of length 2N of one of the gait parameters (step duration, length or speed) in the following form:

where subseries and correspond to the ipsilateral and the contralateral leg, respectively. σI and σC are standard deviations of these series. The simple moving averages (the unweighted mean of the previous m data) of {Ij} and {Cj} are denoted by and , respectively.

We define as errors these values Ii which satisfy all of the following criteria:

These undoubtedly heuristic criteria are used to detect abrupt changes (equation (3)) which lead to conspicuous deviations from the moving average value (equation (2)) and which are not brought about by a deviation in the preceding step of the contralateral leg (equation (4)). As previously mentioned, we dub such events errors, but bear in mind that they may originate either from the motor control system failure, or from the necessary adjustment of the subject’s position on a treadmill. The rationale for using the moving average in the above definition of an error stems from non-stationarity of gait time series. This modification ensures that during transient linear trends the large deviation from the global mean value does not invoke the detection algorithm. Please note that equation (3) by itself is another safeguard for false error detection caused by the transient drift of local mean value. Herein, we report the values for m = 10.

Let us use Δ to denote a deviation of a given gait parameter from its moving average value, e.g. . In interleg control the gait parameter of contralateral leg Ci changes in such a way as to decrease deviation of Ii + Ci. To quantify such stabilization, we introduce the following metric:

The stabilization occurs when . The numerator in the above equation may become smaller than the denominator in two cases. In the first case the ΔCi has the opposite sign to ΔIi:

in other words, the contralateral leg compensates for errors, as shown in Fig. 2a. The perfect compensation corresponds to . In the alternative scenario, only the magnitude of the deviation of the contralateral leg from the mean value decreases () as illustrated by Fig. 2b.

Figure 2: In interleg control the gait parameter of contralateral leg Ci changes in such a way as to decrease deviation of Ii + Ci from the mean value.
figure 2

(A) The error of ipsilateral leg Ii is immediately compensated for by the contralateral leg. (B) In the alternative scenario, only the magnitude of the deviation of the contralateral leg from the mean value decreases.

It is possible that an error does not bring about a sudden change of gait parameter of contralateral leg. In this case, . However, stabilization may occur during the next step of ipsilateral leg. The change of Ii+1 may reduce the deviation . We refer to such a scenario as an intraleg control and define a corresponding metric:

To be able to directly compare the properties of both types of control (inter- and intraleg) we distinguish whether the intraleg control was achieved via compensation:

as shown in Fig. 3a, or by the reduction of the magnitude of the displacement of gait parameter of the ispislateral leg from the moving average value (Fig. 3b).

Figure 3: The error in a gait spatio-temporal parameter may not evoke a sudden change of the parameter of contralateral leg.
figure 3

However, the deviation of the next ipsilateral gait parameter Ii+1 from the mean value may decrease as a result of: (A) compensation or (B) the reduction of the magnitude of the displacement of parameter of the ispislateral leg from the mean value.

The flowchart in Fig. 4 elucidates the analysis of the dynamics of gait parameter time series which follows the occurrence of errors. Using Dinter and Dintra, we detect the activation of inter- and intraleg control mechanism, respectively.

Figure 4
figure 4

Flowchart of analysis of dynamics of gait parameter which follows an error – gait-parameter’s value which satisfy the criteria described by equations (2,3,4).

In our analysis of the experimental data, we use a more specific notation for the interleg parameter Dinter. For example, to indicate that an error in a given gait parameter of the left leg was followed by an adjustment of this parameter by the right leg, we write DLR. In the same vein, we use DLL, DRR to denote intraleg control parameter for the left and right leg, respectively.

In most cases D values, for a given gait parameter, speed, and control type, were not normally distributed (the Shapiro-Wilk test). For a given speed and gait parameter, the Levene’s test showed equality of variances among the control types (with the exception of step duration at 1.1 m/s and step length at all speeds). For a given gait parameter and control type (L-L, R-R, L-R, R-L), we investigated the dependence of D on treadmill speed. In this case, the Levene’s test showed homogeneity of variance. Consequently, the Kruskal-Wallis test with Tukey’s post hoc comparisons was used to detect differences across speed and control type. The significance threshold was set to 0.05.

To quantify functional asymmetry in control of gait spatio-temporal parameters we need to take into account the stochastic aspect of motor control system. Let us employ an analogy of detailed balance equation of statistical physics41 and call it gait detailed balance equation (DBE). In its original formulation, detailed balancing relates the relative population of two states by the probability of a transition between them. The principle applies equally well to physical systems, mathematical probability densities, or statistical processes in a variety of forms.

The smaller D the better stabilization of stride gait parameters. Consequently, the influence of a control mechanism (inter or intra) on gait parameters is proportional to its probability of occurrence and the inverse of the corresponding mean value of control parameter . For example, for the right lower limb, we may write:

in the same vein, for the left lower limb:

The perfect symmetry corresponds to the following equality:

We quantify the asymmetry in control of gait spatio-temporal parameters with the relative difference expression:

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Kozlowska, K. et al. Asymmetry of short-term control of spatio-temporal gait parameters during treadmill walking. Sci. Rep. 7, 44349; doi: 10.1038/srep44349 (2017).

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.