Introduction

Individual personality traits such as emotional sensitivity and impulsivity could cause social conflicts, which can undesirably be manifested in criminal behavior or violence1. In addition, emotional dysregulation and impulsive behavior are deeply involved with various neuropsychiatric problems2. To verify the aversive issues related to emotional sensitivity and impulsivity, researchers have attempted to evaluate the associated traits.

Loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potentials (LDAEP) was calculated as the amplitude change of the evoked N1/P2 component in response to different auditory stimulus intensities3. LDAEP has been identified to be inversely associated with central nervous system serotonergic activity4, and has been proposed as a reliable indicator of central serotonin activity in humans5. In clinical studies, Fitzgerald et al.6 reported that MDD patients with melancholic features (i.e., lack of mood reactivity) had a significantly weaker LDAEP slope, whereas our previous study showed stronger LDAEP values in atypical depression7. While, previous studies reported that individuals who were sensitive to external stimuli have stronger emotional responses8, the specific relationship between emotional sensitivity and LDAEP has not been clarified yet.

Meanwhile, impulsivity has been defined as the lack of ability to refrain inappropriate behavior and clinically, has been regarded as the inability to inhibit behavioral response9. Recent meta-analysis reported that the components of event-related potential (ERP) of Go/Nogo tasks were associated with response inhibition10. For example, Nogo-N2 has been suggested to reflect a variety of cognitive control processes that underlie response inhibition, including response activation11, premotor inhibition12, and most importantly, conflict monitoring13; Nogo-P3 has been proposed to primarily reflect the inhibitory process itself11. In the Nogo trials, the P3 component, has been linked with the process of response inhibition14. Especially, N2 and P3 components, and accuracy rate from the Nogo task have mainly changed in patients with impulse control problems such as trichotillomania, antisocial personality disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)15,16,17. Both Nogo-N2 and P3 ERP component have been regarded to reflect impulsivity, however the interpretation of the changes in N2 and P3 ERP is inconclusive18. Furthermore, previous studies revealed that poor performance of Nogo could be related to low serotonin function and genetic mutation of serotonin system19,20, suggesting that serotonin function might be related with Nogo trials and that serotonin might play a core role among behavioral inhibition and LDAEP21. Previous studies directly pointed out that LDAEP was stronger in more impulsive individuals22. Despite the plausible relationship between LDAEP and impulsivity reflected by Go/Nogo paradigm, there have been no studies investigating the association between LDAEP and Nogo ERP in the general population.

Hence, we hypothesized that individuals with high LDAEP would demonstrate higher emotional sensitivity and impulsivity (Fig. 1). The first aim of the present study was to verify the relationship between LDAEP and emotional sensitivity measured by psychological scaling. The second aim of the study was to evaluate the relationship between LDAEP and impulsivity (impulsivity rating scales and Nogo-N2, P3). Finally, we explored the regional activity of the brain through source activity analysis of the Nogo ERP. It would support our hypothesis by verifying that brain regions known to be related with response inhibition activate in coherence with the change of the Nogo ERP.

Figure 1
figure 1

Study hypothesis.

Loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) could reflect sensitivity and Go/Nogo reflects impulsivity, and both have shared serotonin related regulation. We hypothesize that LDAEP might be correlated with emotional sensitivity such as depression, anxiety, and mood lability. Moreover, LDAEP would be correlated with impulsivity, which would reflect response error, and fast speed.

Results

Psychological and Behavioural measures

Table 1 displays comparisons of the demographic and psychological characteristics between participants in the low and high LDAEP groups. The women vs. men ratio of the high LDAEP group was higher, than that of the low LDAEP group (p = 0.01). Scores of the Barratt impulsivity scale (BIS) (attentional impulsivity & motor impulsivity), Conners’ Adult ADHD rating scale (CAARS) (impulsivity/emotional lability), and affective lability scale (ALS) (depression/elation & anger) were significantly greater in the high LDAEP group compared to the low LDAEP group. Beck depression inventory (BDI) and ALS (anxiety/depression) were marginally significantly greater in the high LDAEP group compared to the low LDAEP group.

Table 1 Comparison of baseline demographic, psychological, and behavioural characteristics in participants with low and high loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) (N = 157, low = 78, high = 79).

There was no significant difference in the average reaction time (376.55 vs. 375.65 ms, F = 0.910, df = 1, p = 0.34), and hit rate (0.94 vs. 0.93 ms, F = 0.075, df = 1, p = 0.78) in the Go condition between the low and high LDAEP groups. However, there was a significant difference in the false alarm rate of Nogo stimuli between the two groups (0.11 vs. 0.15, F = 6.184, df = 1, p = 0.01) (Table 1, Fig. 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

(A) The reaction time of Go between the low and high loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) groups. (B) The false alarm rate of Nogo between the two groups. The x-axis of each figure denotes the duration of Go/Nogo task. The mean and standard error of the mean are presented. * represents a statistically significant difference between the two groups (p < 0.05).

Electroencephalogram Data

Figure 3(A) presents the LDAEP waveforms at Cz in low and high LDAEP groups. Figure 3(B) presents the Go and Nogo ERP waveforms at FCz electrodes.

Figure 3
figure 3

(A) Grand averages of loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) event-related potentials (ERPs) at the Cz electrode for the low and high LDAEP groups. (B) Grand averages of Go ERPs and Nogo ERPs at the FCz electrode between the low and high LDAEP groups. (C) Scalp topographies of Go/Nogo N2 and P3 components between the two groups.

Loudness dependence auditory evoked potentials (LDAEP)

The subjects were divided into two subgroups based on the median LDAEP (=0.98) at the Cz electrode: the low group (n = 78, 0.56 ± 0.37) and the high group (n = 79, 1.53 ± 0.43). The peak-to-peak N1/P2 amplitudes for the five sound intensities of the low LDAEP group were 60 dB: 6.61 ± 2.24; 70 dB: 7.12 ± 2.55; 80 dB: 8.13 ± 2.60; 90 dB: 8.28 ± 2.52; 100 dB: 8.82 ± 2.59 μV, and the peak-to-peak N1/P2 amplitudes for the five sound intensities of the high LDAEP group were 60 dB: 6.89 ± 2.84; 70 dB: 8.10 ± 2.83; 80 dB: 9.84 ± 3.24; 90 dB: 11.14 ± 3.42; 100 dB: 13.00 ± 3.28 μV.

Go/Nogo condition

Because there were no group differences for latencies of N2 and P3 components across the Go and Nogo stimuli, the following analysis focused on the amplitude of each component.

In the N2 amplitude, there was a significant main effect of condition (F = 21.477, df = 1, p < 0.001). The main effect of electrode site was also significant (F = 12.681, df = 3, p < 0.001). Post-hoc analysis revealed that the N2 amplitudes of Fz and FCz were greater (more negative) than those of Cz and Pz. Importantly, the two-way interaction of group x condition was significant (F = 7.456, df = 1, p = 0.007). However, post-hoc analysis revealed that there was no significant difference between two groups in both Go and Nogo conditions.

In the P3 amplitude, there was a significant main effect of group (F = 10.838, df = 1, p = 0.001). The main effects of condition and electrode site were significant (F = 23.163, df = 1, p < 0.001; F = 9.866, df = 3, p < 0.001, respectively) as well. Post-hoc analysis revealed that the P3 amplitudes of FCz and Cz were greater than Fz and Pz. Although there was no significant interaction, all the three factors (group, condition, and electrode site) showed significant main effect and a simple main effect analysis was performed to check if there was a group difference in the Go and Nogo conditions. The simple main effect analysis indicated that both amplitudes of Go-P3 and Nogo-P3 were significantly lower in the low LDAEP group than in the high LDAEP group regardless of the electrode sites.

Correlation analysis

LDAEP was significantly correlated with psychological measures related to emotionality such as BDI (r = 0.235, p = 0.003), ALS total score (r = 0.229, p = 0.004) (Fig. 4A), the depression/elation subscale (r = 0.248, p = 0.002) and the anxiety/depression of the ALS (r = 0.175, p = 0.029), and anger (r = 0.165, p = 0.041). The previous studies revealed that the FCz electrode showed the greatest P3 amplitude and stronger N2 correlation compared to those of other electrodes23,24. In addition, the FCz showed robust findings in both N2 and P3 amplitudes in this study. Based on these findings, for the correlation between LDAEP and ERP, the FCz electrode was used for analysis to avoid the multiple comparisons. LDAEP was also significantly correlated with impulsivity measures such as impulsivity/emotional lability (r = 0.177, p = 0.027) of CAARS and Nogo-P3 amplitude at the FCz (r = 0.217, p = 0.007) (Fig. 4B). In addition, the false alarm rate of Nogo condition showed a significant positive correlation with the total score of BIS (r = 0.166, p = 0.038), and Nogo-N2 latency at the FCz electrode (r = 0.185, p = 0.021) (Fig. 4C).

Figure 4
figure 4

(A) The loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) showed a significant correlation with emotional scale scores such as Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), and Korean version of Affective Lability Scale (ALS) scores. (B) LDAEP showed significant correlation with impulsivity measures such as impulsivity/emotional lability subscale score of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) scale, and Nogo P3 amplitude at FCz. (C) The false alarm rate of Nogo showed a significant correlation with impulsivity measures such as Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS) score and Nogo-N2 latency at FCz.

Source P300 of Nogo condition

Source analysis of the Nogo-P3 revealed increased source densities of the cuneus (BA 17), lingual gyrus (BA 17, 18), and precentral gyrus (BA 6) in the high LDAEP group (p < 0.05; Fig. 5) compared to the low LDAEP group. Detailed information on the statistical values and voxel coordinates is provided in Table 2.

Table 2 Brain regions showing significant differences of Nogo-P3 source activity between low and high LDAEP groups.
Figure 5
figure 5

Differences in the source activity of Nogo P300 between the low and high loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) groups in three regions: (A) cuneus, (B) lingual gyrus, and (C) precentral gyrus.

Discussion

This study investigated whether nonclinical adults with higher LDAEP show higher emotional sensitivity and higher impulsivity. First, the high LDAEP group showed higher emotional sensitivity measured by psychological scaling. Second, the high LDAEP group showed increased Nogo false alarm rate, and increased BIS score compared to the low LDAEP group. The LDAEP values were significantly correlated with BIS score and Nogo-P3 amplitude. Additionally, the source activity of the Nogo P300 revealed significantly greater activation of the cuneus, lingual gyrus, and precentral gyrus in the high LDAEP group compared to those in the low LDAEP group.

As we hypothesized, the LDAEP values were correlated with BDI and ALS. The high LDAEP group also showed higher depressive symptom scores and affective lability than the low LDAEP group. Our results show that the level of LDAEP is closely related with emotional sensitivity, and reflect “mood reactivity or fluctuation tendency” in healthy participants. Similarly, a previous clinical study revealed that LDAEP is significantly related to the mood reactivity in patients with major depressive disorder25. These evidences suggest that LDAEP might be a marker for the emotional sensitivity not only in the clinical condition, but also in the general population.

The BIS scores of the high LDAEP group was higher than that of the lower group. The high LDAEP group showed significantly lower accuracy rate for Nogo trials as well. Additionally, the false alarm rate of Nogo was positively correlated with the BIS in this study. In previous studies, the false alarm (commision error) rate of Nogo has been known to be closely related to response inhibition26,27 and also associated with trait impulsiveness as measured by BIS18,28. These evidences suggest that high LDAEP would demonstrate higher impulsivity related to impulsivity scale and false alarm rate for Nogo trials.

Interestingly, post hoc revealed no significant difference between the two groups for both Go and Nogo conditions in the Go/Nogo-N2 amplitude, even though there was a significant two-way interaction of group x condition. However, we found that the degree of amplitude changes from Go condition to Nogo condition was larger in the high LDAEP group than in the low LDAEP group; the high LDAEP group showed lower Go-N2 amplitude and increased Nogo-N2 amplitude than the low LDAEP group. Smaller amplitude of Go-N2 may mirror decreased attention resources allocated to the competing stimuli17. Considering that Nogo-N2 is widely accepted to be relevant to conflict monitoring13, this increased Nogo-N2 amplitude in the high LDAEP group might reflect a compensatory mechanism that prevents less efficient conflict monitoring. Therefore, the increased degree of amplitude changes between Go and Nogo conditions in the high LDAEP group might indicate that less attention resource would be allocated to the competing stimuli and this would lead to weaker conflict monitor in the high LDAEP group than in the low LDAEP group.

As we expected, LDAEP was positively correlated with Nogo-P3 amplitude and Nogo-P3 amplitude was significantly higher in the high LDAEP group compared to the low LDAEP group. Previous studies suggest that the Nogo-P3 amplitude is related to impulsivity16,29. Hartman et al.26 insisted that diminished Nogo-P3 might be an indicator for poor response inhibition. Nogo-P3 components showed reduction of the amplitudes in juvenile delinquents with antisocial personality characteristics17, patients with ADHD16, and borderline personality disorder30. In contrast, people with internet addiction disorder exhibited higher Nogo-P3 amplitude than controls29. The increased Nogo-P3 in non-clinical individuals with high impulsivity reflect the need for enhanced inhibitory effort or the degree of cognitive endeavors in order to yield equal performance compared to that of low impulsive individuals14,29,31. Moreover, Benvenuti et al.18 commented that high impulsive individuals may require an greater effortful response inhibition in order to counteract the prepotent tendency to respond, which is elicited by the combination of high trait impulsiveness and high emotional arousal. This increased P3 amplitude might reflect a protective or compensatory mechanism that prevents the premature response or poor impulse control in high LDAEP group.

People with high sensory processing sensitivity are described to reflect an increased sensitivity of the central nervous system and a deeper cognitive processing of physical and emotional stimuli32. Considering that the higher LDAEP might be related to higher sensory sensitivity, these individuals could better respond to the positive and negative stimuli with higher reactivity. As a result, higher reactivity to negative stimuli could cause depressive mood or anxiety related to emotional sensitivity. On the other hand, the more sensitive individuals, with heightened positive emotions in response to rewarding stimuli, might be associated to the “openness” on the five basic personality dimension33.

Meanwhile, a previous study using functional magnetic resonance imaging reported that healthy subjects with higher impulsivity have a similar influence on the neuronal correlates of the coding of sound intensity and show more activation of auditory evoked potential than subjects with low impulsivity34. In the study, high impulsive subjects are presumed to show a greater serotonergic responsiveness, representing lower levels of serotonin, causing a higher auditory evoked activity in the primary auditory cortex, which is correlated with the loudness-dependent change of the extent of fMRI activation32. The underlying mechanism has not been clarified yet, LDAEP in the primary auditory cortex is positively correlated to “novelty seeking”35 and it might be related to impulsive choice related to the serotonergic responsiveness.

In the source activity of Nogo ERP, the precentral gyrus (BA 6), lingual gyrus and cuneus showed stronger activation in the high LDAEP group compared to the low LDAEP group. The precentral gyrus is related to motor control and plays a critical role in inhibiting inappropriate prepotent response tendencies in motor process36,37,38,39. The lingual gyrus is one of the activated regions in error processing and response inhibition in healthy controls37,40. The Left cuneus is one of the activated regions during response inhibition task in healthy control as well as patients with schizophrenia41,42. Activation of these regions during response inhibition task could be interpreted as an increased demand for more inhibitory efforts and resources. It suggests that the high LDAEP group need more effortful impulse control compared to the low LDAEP group.

There are some limitations in this study. First, the gender ratio was different between the high and low LDAEP groups. To overcome gender effects, we used partial correlation in LDAEP related analysis. Secondly, the present study lacked structured interview that screen healthy participants. Finally, our results may not be generalized to the clinical subjects. Further studies would be needed to evaluate pathophysiology of the clinical samples.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to show the relationship between LDAEP and Go/Nogo ERP reflecting impulsivity in non-clinical participants of large sample sizes. Consistent with our hypothesis, LDAEP was associated with emotional sensitivity and impulsivity. The source analysis of Nogo ERP supports our hypothesis. These evidences suggest that LDAEP is a useful tool to evaluate emotional regulation such as emotional sensitivity and impulsivity in healthy individuals.

Method

Participants

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board and Ethics Committee of Inje University Ilsan Paik Hospital and all experimental protocols were approved by the committee (2015-07-026-001). The study was performed in accordance with approved guidelines. Informed consent was obtained from all study participants. The study was performed on 157 non-smoking healthy volunteers (57 men and 100 women) with a mean age of 27.80 ± 6.37 (years). Participants were recruited from the local community through local newspapers and posters. Participants with any history of neurological or other mental diseases, and smoking history within 2 years were excluded from the study through the initial screening interviews. Each participant had normal or corrected to normal vision, as determined by checking visual acuity with the Snellen chart43.

Psychological measures

Psychological measures and scales were conducted to measure emotional sensitivity and impulsivity. To evaluate emotional sensitivity, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)44 State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)45 and Korean version of Affective Lability Scale (KALS)46,47 were applied. BDI is a validated scale composed of 21-items for measuring the severity of depressive symptoms44. Each BDI question was scored from 0–3, with higher scores indicating greater depressive symptom severity. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) is a commonly used tool that measures trait and state anxiety45. It includes the state anxiety inventory (SAI) and the trait anxiety inventory (TAI), which are comprised of 20-items each45. The 18-item ALS, which measures individual proneness to rapid shifts from the different emotional states of anxiety, depression, anger, and hypomania46, was also evaluated. The ALS is based on a three factor model of affective lability (depression/elation, anxiety/depression, and anger)48.

To assess impulsivity related traits, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS)49,50, Conners’ Adult ADHD rating scale (CAARS)51, Behavioral Activation System, and Behavioral Inhibition System52,53 were applied. The BIS consists of 11 questionnaires and is designed to assess the personality/behavioural construct of impulsiveness. The BIS has three second-order factors (attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsiveness)50. The CAARS is designed to assess the manifestations of ADHD in adults, and is composed of 42 items that are divided into four subscales including inattention/memory, hyperactivity/restlessness, impulsivity/emotional lability, and problems with self-concept51. Behavioral Activation System and Behavioral Inhibition System Questionnaires were used to measure self-reported dysregulation of behavioural activation and inhibition.

Electroencephalogram (EEG) Acquisition and Analysis

During the EEG task, each participant was tested in a sound-attenuated EEG room. EEG was acquired using a NeuroScan SynAmps amplifier (Compumedics USA, E1 Paso, TX, USA) with 64 Ag-AgCl electrodes mounted on a Quik Cap using an extended 10–20 placement scheme. The ground electrode was located on the forehead and the physically linked reference electrode was attached to both mastoids. The vertical electrooculogram (EOG) was positioned above and below the left eye and the horizontal EOG was recorded at the outer canthus of each eye. The impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. All data were processed with a 0.1–100 Hz band pass filter and sampled at 1000 Hz.

The recorded EEG data were preprocessed using CURRY 7. Gross artifacts, such as artifacts caused by movements, were rejected through visual inspection by a trained person with no prior information regarding the origin of the data. Artifacts related to eye movement or eye blinks were removed using the mathematical procedure implemented in the preprocessing software54. The data were filtered using a 0.1–30 Hz bandpass filter and epoched from 100 ms pre-stimulus to 900 ms post-stimulus. The epochs were subtracted from the average value of the pre-stimulus interval for baseline correction. If any remaining epochs contained significant physiological artifacts (amplitude exceeding ±75 μV) in any of the 62 electrode sites, they were excluded from further analysis. Only artifact-free epochs were averaged across trials and participants for ERP analysis. For analysis of Go/Nogo task, only correctly responded epochs were used. The number of epochs of LDAEP used for the analysis did not significantly differ between the low and high LDAEP groups (60 dB: 185.22 ± 15.20 vs. 183.15 ± 16.29, p = 0.413, 70 dB: 184.41 ± 15.91 vs. 182.29 ± 17.30, p = 0.426, 80 dB: 184.27 ± 16.49 vs. 183.18 ± 16.27, p = 0.677, 90 dB: 185.38 ± 15.19 vs. 182.19 ± 17.13, p = 0.218, 100 dB = 184.47 ± 15.14 vs. 182.54 ± 17.17, p = 0.456). The number of epochs of Go/Nogo used for the analysis did not significantly differ between the low and high LDAEP groups (Go condition: 209.05 ± 21.37 vs. 208.35 ± 23.09, p = 0.845, Nogo condition: 49.37 ± 5.97 vs. 47.38 ± 7.34, p = 0.064).

Loudness dependence auditory evoked potentials (LDAEP)

Auditory stimulation included 1000 stimuli with an inter stimulus interval that was randomized between 500 and 900 ms. Tones of 1000 Hz and 80-ms duration (10-ms rise and 10-ms fall) were presented through MDR-D777 headphones (Sony, Tokyo, Japan) at five intensities: 60, 70, 80, 90, and 100 dB SPL. These stimuli were generated by E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). For each subject, the N1 peak (most negative peak between 50 and 200 ms from the stimulus) and the P2 peak (most positive peak between 150 and 300 ms from the stimulus) were then determined at the Cz electrode55,56 for the five intensities. The peak-to-peak N1/P2 amplitudes were calculated for the five stimulus intensities and the LDAEP was calculated as the slope of the linear regression.

Go/Nogo experiment

Subjects were seated approximately 60 cm away from a computer screen (Mitsubishi, 22-inch CRT monitor). Stimuli for Go/Nogo task, which consisted of numbers 1–8, were randomly presented on the screen. The subjects were instructed to press the spacebar as accurately and quickly as possible when the Go stimuli (even numbers: 2, 4, 6, 8) appeared at the centre of the screen and not to respond when the Nogo stimuli (odd numbers: 1, 3, 5, 7) were displayed. There were 300 trials, which consisted of Go (80% probability) condition and Nogo (20% probability) condition. On each task trial, a fixation cross was presented for 100 ms. Following intervals of 700–1000 ms, Go or Nogo targets appeared for 500 ms, and then, there was a 500 ms interval before the next trial. These stimuli were generated by E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). In the Go condition, the N200 (the most negative peak between 150 and 350 ms after stimulus onset) and the P300 (the most positive peak between 250 and 500 ms after stimulus onset) were investigated at the Fz, FCz, Cz, and Pz electrodes. In the Nogo condition, the N200 (the most negative peak between 150 and 350 ms after stimulus onset) and the P300 (the most positive peak between 300 and 550 ms after stimulus onset) were investigated at the Fz, FCz, Cz, and Pz electrodes.

Source activity analysis

Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) is one of the representative source imaging methods for solving EEG inverse problem57. sLORETA assumes that the source activation of a voxel is similar to that of the surrounding voxels for calculating a particular solution, and applies an appropriate standardization of the current density. sLORETA was used to compute the cortical distribution of the standardized source current density of each ERP component. The lead field matrix was computed using a realistic head model segmented based on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 standard template, in which the three-dimensional solution space was restricted only to the cortical gray matter and hippocampus58. The solution space was composed of 6,239 voxels with 5 mm resolution. Anatomical labels such as Brodmann areas (BAs) are provided by using an appropriate transformation from MNI to Talairach space59.

The source images of N2 and P3 were analyzed in Nogo condition, and the time frames used to calculate the N2 and P3 source images were defined between 150 and 350 ms and between 300 and 550 ms after stimulus onset, respectively.

Statistical Analysis

Multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) was used to compare the scores of psychological and behavioural data between low and high LDAEP groups. A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed for Go/Nogo ERP amplitudes and latencies, with the condition (Go and Nogo) and electrode site (Fz, FCz, Cz, and Pz) as the within-group factors, and the two comparing groups (low LDAEP vs. high LDAEP) as the between-group factor. Because LDAEP could be significantly influenced by gender, age, and smoking60, gender and age were considered as covariates in both the MANOVA and repeated measures ANOVA.

The comparison of sLORETA images between the two groups for Nogo-N2 and P3 was done using a statistical non-parametric mapping method (SnPM) that was provided by the sLORETA software. This software provides voxel-by-voxel independent t-test for the 6239 voxels, followed by a randomization test (n = 5000) to correct for multiple comparisons.

In addition, the relationships among variables were analyzed by Spearman’s correlation. When LDAEP was included in the correlation analysis, partial Spearman’s correlation was used to control age and sex as covariates. The significant level was set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed). Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 21 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Kim, J. S. et al. Auditory evoked potential could reflect emotional sensitivity and impulsivity. Sci. Rep. 6, 37683; doi: 10.1038/srep37683 (2016).

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.