Introduction

In mammals, the dominant light-entrainable circadian pacemaker is contained in the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN). Individual SCN neurons function as autonomous circadian oscillators, expressing ~24 h oscillations in core clock genes/proteins, such as per1-2/PER1-21. Such intracellular molecular rhythms can be visualized over several cycles in vitro in individual cells of living SCN brain slices prepared from mice in which an enhanced destabilized green fluorescent protein (eGFP) reports the activity of the per1 promoter (per1::eGFP mice2). Rhythmic expression of clock genes does not itself define SCN timekeeping; these cell-autonomous oscillators must synchronize their activities to produce global SCN output of sufficient amplitude and coherence to organize circadian rhythms in whole animal physiology and behavior3. Intra-SCN signaling utilizes a range of neurotransmitters including arginine vasopressin (AVP), vasoactive intestinal polypeptide (VIP), gastrin-releasing peptide (GRP) glutamate and γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA). Both VIP and its receptor, VPAC2, are expressed by SCN neurons4,5 and intercellular communication via VIP-VPAC2 signaling plays a key role in the coupling of SCN cellular timekeepers6,7. For example, mice lacking VIP or VPAC2 expression (Vip−/− and Vipr2−/−, respectively) exhibit pronounced abnormalities in the timing of their behavior and physiology and SCN expression of clock genes/proteins is greatly diminished8,9,10,11. Indeed, in comparison to SCN from behaviorally rhythmic wild-type (WT) per1::eGFP mice, single cell oscillators in the Vipr2−/− × per1::eGFP SCN are fewer in number, oscillate at lower amplitude and are less well synchronized7. Therefore, circadian deficits observed in vivo can be accurately mapped to ex vivo assessments of SCN clock cell activity.

Animals with functional deficits in the intracellular molecular clockworks can express aberrant circadian behavior in constant darkness (DD)12, with SCN rhythms in clock genes typically blunted or disrupted13,14,15. Intriguingly, behavioral and SCN molecular rhythms can be restored in some such models by maintaining them in constant light (LL)16,17,18. Light information is relayed to the SCN via the retinohypothalamic tract (RHT), using glutamate and pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide (PACAP) as neurotransmitters19,20 and induces expression of clock genes including per121. It is possible that LL provides a tonic excitatory drive onto the SCN thereby circumventing intracellular molecular lesions16, though it is unclear if LL can also restore circadian activities in mice with intercellular signaling defects. Since Vipr2−/− mice express all the known clock genes, albeit at diminished levels8 and because the light input pathway in Vipr2−/− mice is intact22,23,24,25, we investigated whether LL could rescue behavioral and SCN cellular rhythms in this model of a circadian system weakened through impairment of the key intercellular synchronizing pathway. We report that extended exposure to LL promotes behavioral rhythms and SCN intercellular synchrony in Vipr2−/− mice. This indicates that manipulations based on non-invasive lighting strategies can be effective to improve circadian competence and highlights the plastic nature of SCN circadian function.

Results

Differential Effects of Constant Light on Wheel-Running Behavior in WT and Vipr2−/− Mice

Both WT and Vipr2−/− mice confined the majority of intense wheel-running activity to the dark phase of the LD cycle. On transfer to LL, WT mice behaved in a manner consistent with previous descriptions26,27,28, exhibiting a large phase delay in locomotor activity (~5 h) and suppression of wheel-running compared to LD (~110 revs/h in LL vs. ~400 rev/h in LD; p = 0.001; t-Test; n = 12; Fig. 1a,b). Initial disruption of rhythmicity, lasting ~2–3 days, was followed for the majority (11 of 12) of individuals by more robust rhythms that persisted throughout the 36 days of LL examined here. Unlike DD behavior, where arrhythmicity in WT mice is uncommon and unexpected24,29,30, one WT mouse failed to express identifiable circadian rhythms in behavior in LL demonstrating, as has previously been reported, that LL can be disruptive to WT circadian rhythms in locomotor activity. Over the time course of LL examined here, we observed no significant change in the percentage of rhythmic WT mice (p = 0.677; Fisher’s Exact Test with Freeman-Halton extension; n = 12; Fig. 1e; early vs. mid vs. late LL; though also see Fig. 4a) and no significant change in rhythm strength (p = 0.25; paired t-Test of FFT spectral power in early vs late LL; n = 12; Fig. 1h). Indeed, 50% of WT individuals exhibited increased spectral power in late LL vs early, while 50% exhibited a decrease (Fig. 1i). As is common in LL, rhythmic WT mice expressed a mean period that was substantially longer (25.03 ± 0.11 h; see Fig. 1a,b) than is observed for this strain in DD7,31.

Figure 1
figure 1

Exposure to LL improves the expression of circadian behavior in Vipr2−/− mice.

Actograms showing wheel-running behavior of WT and Vipr2−/− mice in LD and LL, with complimentary FFT spectrograms for the LL portion of activity data (ad). Summary histograms show rhythmicity of WT and Vipr2−/− mice in LL (e,f) and subsequent DD for a subset of mice (g, also see Fig. 2); rhythm strength (FFT spectral power) in early vs late LL (h) and the percentage of mice that showed increasing and decreasing rhythm strength over time in LL (i). After initial disruption, LL improves circadian behavior in Vipr2−/− mice. For baseline comparison, DD rhythmicity data are presented in panel (g) for the subset of 12 Vipr2−/− mice that were exposed to LL-DD-LL conditions (actograms shown in Fig. 2). Actograms show behavior of individual animals and are double-plotted with 2 consecutive days’ data on each line. Spectrograms are presented aligned horizontally with the corresponding behavioral data and vertical white dotted lines show the 24 h period mark. The color scale is normalized between 0 spectral power (purple) and mean spectral power plus 3 standard deviations (μ + 3σ; red). Color scale for spectrograms in (bd) is as shown in (a). Gray shading on actograms indicates darkness. Histogram legends for (e,f) are as shown in (g). Panel f shows a significant increase in the proportion of rhythmic Vipr2−/− mice over time in LL (p<0.001; Fisher’s Exact Test with Freeman-Halton extension), an effect which is absent for both Vipr2−/− mice in DD (g) and WT mice in LL (e). Panel h shows a significant increase in rhythm strength (FFT spectral power) of Vipr2−/− mice in late LL vs early LL (p<0.01; paired t-Test). Panel i provides a visual illustration that rhythm strength increases over time in LL in the majority of Vipr2−/− mice, but not WT mice. * p<0.05; *** p<0.001.

Figure 2
figure 2

LL-induced improvement in circadian behavior of Vipr2−/− mice depends on continued exposure to LL.

Actograms showing wheel-running behavior of Vipr2−/− mice housed under LL conditions for 36 days, then transferred into DD for 36 days, before a second 36 day exposure to LL (a). On transfer to DD, rhythmic Vipr2−/− mice do not sustain the ~24 h rhythms expressed in LL and instead become arrhythmic or show shorter period (~22.4 h) rhythms in wheel-running (also see 24). When subsequently re-exposed to LL, Vipr2−/− mice rapidly alter wheel-running to once again show overt rhythmicity and the characteristic longer period (~24 h) expressed in LL. Actogram plotting and shading as in Fig. 1. Panels (b,c) show a box and whisker plot of period and histogram of rhythmicity, respectively, for LL1, DD and LL2, assessed over the last 12 days of each epoch. Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum range of period data points. ***p < 0.001.

Figure 3
figure 3

Behavioral rhythmicity in WT and Vipr2−/− mice is associated with increased intercellular synchrony in the SCN. a-b (upper panels).

Composite rhythm plots show per1::eGFP profiles for 30 individual SCN cells in two brain slices from each genotype. Insets show a reduced selection of cells to aid visualization of synchrony and highlight differences in synchrony between cells within slices. (ab) (middle panels): Photomicrographs showing peak and trough expression of per1::eGFP for rhythmic slices and representative images from equivalent timepoints for arrhythmic slices. Insets show the regions indicated by white boxes at higher magnification (note synchronized peak and trough expression for SCNs from rhythmic mice and lack of coherent day-night differences for SCNs from arrhythmic mice). (a,b) (lower panels): Rayleigh plots showing time of peak per1::eGFP expression of rhythmic SCN cells analyzed within single slice cultures. Arrow heads indicate the time of peak fluorescence for individual cells, the length of the central line indicates the degree of synchrony between the times of peak expression of individual cells (quantified as Rayleigh R; longer line indicates greater synchrony) and the inner broken ring shows the threshold for statistical significance of synchrony. (ch): Histograms describing analyzed rhythm parameters for per1::eGFP expression in slices from rhythmic and arrhythmic WT and Vipr2−/− mice. Key for panels dh is as shown in panel c. Behavioral rhythmicity was assessed during LL for the last 10–14 days before cull and data are presented as a comparison between the SCN parameters of mice classified as behaviorally rhythmic and arrhythmic, irrespective of time in LL (which is considered in Fig. 4). Panel (h) shows period difference and phase difference between dorsal and ventral parts of the SCN in cultures from behaviorally rhythmic and arrhythmic WT and Vipr2−/− mice. Across both genotypes, most SCN cultures from behaviorally rhythmic mice were synchronized (d,e), while no SCN cultures from behaviorally arrhythmic animals expressed significantly synchronized cellular rhythms (e). SCN slices from behaviorally rhythmic WT mice contained significantly more rhythmic cells (c), whereas SCN slices from behaviorally rhythmic Vipr2−/− mice exhibited reduced dorsal-ventral regional heterogeneity in period and phase (h).

Figure 4
figure 4

Relationships between time in LL, behavioral rhythmicity and SCN cellular synchrony for WT and Vipr2−/− mice.

Vipr2−/− data describe a three-way positive correlation between increasing time in LL, greater behavioral rhythmicity and increasing SCN cellular synchrony (df). For WT mice, behavioral rhythmicity positively correlates with SCN cellular synchrony (c). Conversely, the relationships of both of these parameters with time in LL for WT mice are poorly fit by linear trends (broken lines on panels a,b) but ultimately result in a progressive decrease in both rhythmicity (a) and synchrony (b) after extended (>29 days) durations of LL (light unbroken lines added for visual reference; though see main text for statistical assessment of these subsets of data for >29 days in LL). Data in panels cf are shown fitted with linear trend lines (heavy unbroken lines) that provided high goodness of fit and illustrate a linear relationship of ranked data in each case (all data sets in panels (cf) show significant correlations; Spearman’s rho = 0.755 (p = 0.0025) (c), 0.522 (p = 0.038) (d), 0.6 (p = 0.007) (e) and 0.512 (p = 0.0215) (f)).

The majority of Vipr2−/− individuals exhibited overtly disrupted rhythms on release into LL, though unlike WT mice, this disruption persisted substantially longer than the first 2–3 days in LL (Fig. 1c,d). Indeed, while typically ~50% of Vipr2−/− mice generate robust rhythms in locomotor behavior in DD (Figs 1g and 2 and see24), only ~30% of Vipr2−/− mice (7 of 24) exhibited identifiable circadian rhythms in wheel-running during early LL (Fig. 1f). With increasing duration in LL, however, the percentage of rhythmic individuals significantly increased, reaching 83% (20 of 24 individuals) by late LL (Fig. 1f; p = 0.00032, Fisher’s Exact Test with Freeman-Halton extension; n = 24). Consistent with this, rhythm strength of Vipr2−/− mice significantly increased over time in LL (Fig. 1h; p = 0.040; paired t-Test of FFT spectral power in early vs late LL; n = 24), an increase observed in 75% of Vipr2−/− individuals (Fig. 1i). As such, the effect of LL on Vipr2−/− locomotor rhythms was time-dependent, with initial disruption of rhythmicity in the short term followed by a rhythm enhancing effect after extended exposure.

Rhythmic Vipr2−/− mice (n = 20) expressed a mean period of 24.29 ± 0.08 h in LL, significantly shorter than WT mice under these conditions (p = 0.000009; n = 11 WT and 20 Vipr2−/−). Once rhythmic, the period of Vipr2−/− mice remained consistent throughout the duration examined (Fig. 1c,d) and was longer than has been observed for this strain in DD24,32 (and see Fig. 2). Wheel-running activity (rev/h) of Vipr2−/− mice did not significantly reduce on transfer from LD to LL (173 ± 23 vs. 135 ± 20 rev/h, respectively; p = 0.35; n = 24; Fig. 1c,d) and this parameter of Vipr2−/− wheel activity under LL was not significantly different to that of WT animals under LL (p = 0.50; n = 12 WT and 24 Vipr2−/−).

Thus, exposure to LL has a disruptive effect on the expression of WT behavioral rhythms but induces a time-related enhancement of rhythmicity in Vipr2−/− mice after extended durations.

LL-induced Improvements in Behavioral Rhythmicity Are Not Sustained in the Absence of Constant Light

To test whether LL-induced improvements in behavioral rhythmicity persisted in the subsequent absence of light, following the initial 36 days of LL (LL1) a subgroup of 12 Vipr2−/− mice were transferred to DD for 36 days. The rhythm characteristics of this subset were representative of the whole Vipr2−/− group examined in LL1. On transfer into DD, Vipr2−/− mice rapidly reverted to behavioral phenotypes well-documented for this genotype under DD conditions24,32,33; a continuum of phenotypes was observed across individuals, spanning arrhythmicity through to rhythmic with a short period (Fig. 2a). Whilst 83% of this cohort had expressed a circadian rhythm in behavior at the end of LL1, only 50% were rhythmic during DD and the period of these rhythms, where present, was reduced by ~1.6 h from near 24 h to ~22.4 h (24.03 ± 0.04 h for this subset in LL1 (n = 10) to 22.41 ± 0.04 h in DD (n = 6); p < 0.001; Fig. 2). The rhythmicity of these mice did not overtly change across the duration DD examined here (Figs 1g and 2a). This subgroup of Vipr2−/− mice were subsequently returned to LL for a further 36 days (LL2) during which each individual expressed a locomotor phenotype consistent with its behavior during LL1 (83% (10 of 12) rhythmic in LL2, mean period 24.05 ± 0.05 h; Fig. 2). Vipr2−/− mice re- exposed to this second epoch of LL tended to regain the ‘LL-like’ behavioral phenotype more rapidly than during the original LL exposure (e.g. Fig. 2a).

Intercellular Synchrony in the SCN Correlates with Behavioral Rhythmicity

Given the time-dependent effect of LL on Vipr2−/− behavior, we next assessed the correlation of LL-induced behavioral changes with per1::eGFP expression in the SCN. A separate cohort of behaviorally phenotyped WT and Vipr2−/− mice were culled at randomly assigned timepoints after increasing durations of LL and, using confocal microscopy, we imaged per1::eGFP expression in live SCN-containing brain slice cultures from each individual (Fig. 3).

Mice used in this part of the study showed the same trends in behavioral rhythmicity as those in the initial behavioral examinations presented previously. Indeed, ranking all Vipr2−/− mice used for confocal imaging in order of behavioral rhythmicity at the time of cull revealed a significant positive correlation between time in LL and ranked behavioral rhythmicity (Spearman’s rho value 0.522; p = 0.038; R2 = 0.387; n = 16; Fig. 4d). A linear trend line poorly fitted the data for WT time in LL plotted against behavioral rhythmicity rank (R2 = 0.085; Fig. 4a) and these data were not significantly correlated (Spearman’s rho = −0.442; p = 0.15; n = 12). However, visual interpretation of the data revealed that with extended durations of LL (>29 days), rhythmicity consistently decreased in WT mice, an association that did reach statistical significance for this subset of animals (Spearman’s rho = −0.813; p = 0.005; n = 10; Fig. 4a).

To assess the relationship between behavioral rhythmicity and circadian function in the SCN at single cell level, we divided per1::eGFP expression data for each genotype into 2 groups; individuals behaviorally rhythmic at the time of cull (n = 9 WT; n = 10 Vipr2−/−) and individuals behaviorally arrhythmic at cull (n = 3 WT; n = 6 Vipr2−/−). The time of cull for each individual was assigned prior to the experiment commencing and therefore was not influenced by the degree of behavioral rhythmicity of the animals. SCN slices from behaviorally rhythmic WT mice contained a significantly higher percentage of rhythmic individual cells than slices from behaviorally arrhythmic conspecifics (99.6 ± 0.4% vs. 96.7 ± 1.9%; p = 0.016; Fig. 3a,c). Intriguingly, however, we found no significant difference in the percentage of rhythmic cells between slices from behaviorally rhythmic and arrhythmic Vipr2−/− mice (both ~80% of cells rhythmic; p = 0.380; Fig. 3b,c).

Crucially, this intergenotype difference in the relationship of behavioral and SCN cellular rhythmicity was not seen for synchrony between cellular rhythms in the SCN; we observed significantly greater intercellular synchrony within slices from behaviorally rhythmic WT and Vipr2−/− mice than in slices from arrhythmic conspecifics (0.36 ± 0.05 vs. 0.18 ± 0.01; p = 0.0039 [synchrony in slices from rhythmic WT mice vs. arrhythmic WT mice, respectively]; and 0.41 ± 0.07 vs. 0.22 ± 0.03; p = 0.0284 [slices from rhythmic vs. arrhythmic Vipr2−/− animals]; Rayleigh R values; Fig. 3a,b,d). Further, while 67% of slices from behaviorally rhythmic WT animals were significantly synchronized and 60% of slices from behaviorally rhythmic Vipr2−/− mice, no slices from behaviorally arrhythmic mice of either genotype exhibited significant intercellular synchrony (Fig. 3a,b,e). We found no significant differences in the mean period or amplitude of cells between slices from behaviorally rhythmic and arrhythmic individuals for either WT or Vipr2−/− mice (all p < 0.05) though both the period and amplitude of Vipr2−/− oscillations were consistently lower than those of WT oscillations, regardless of behavioral rhythmicity (all p < 0.001; Fig. 3f,g).

As regional heterogeneity in period and phase within the SCN have previously been associated with the maintenance of circadian synchrony34, we next assessed differences in these parameters between the dorsal and ventral regions of the SCN in behaviorally rhythmic and arrhythmic mice of both genotypes. In WT SCN we found no significant changes in dorsal-ventral period or phase heterogeneity between behaviorally rhythmic and arrhythmic animals (Fig. 3h). In Vipr2−/− SCN, however, the differences in both period and phase between dorsal and ventral regions were significantly smaller (as well as less variable; see smaller SEM) in slices from behaviorally rhythmic mice (Fig. 3h), a characteristic consistent with improved cellular synchrony in behaviorally rhythmic animals.

Finally, to better describe the genotype-dependent and -independent aspects of the relationships between behavioral rhythmicity, SCN synchrony and time in LL, we calculated Spearman’s Rank Correlation Coefficients for these parameters using behavioral data ranked in order of rhythmicity. For both WT (n = 12) and Vipr2−/− (n = 16) mice we found a significant correlation between ranked behavioral rhythmicity and intercellular synchrony (p = 0.0025 [Spearman rho = 0.755] for WT and p = 0.0215 [0.512] Vipr2−/−) and that a linear trend line fit the data well (R2 = 0.462 and 0.168, respectively; Fig. 4c,f). Consistent with this and with the positive correlation between time in LL and behavioral rhythmicity in Vipr2−/− mice (Fig. 4d), we also found a significant positive relationship between time in LL and SCN intercellular synchrony in Vipr2−/− mice (p = 0.007; rho = 0.6; Fig. 4e). In WT mice however, as we observed for the relationship between time in LL and behavioral rhythmicity (see above; Fig. 4a), time in LL was not significantly correlated with SCN synchrony (Spearman’s rho = −0.374) and a linear trend line poorly fitted the data (R2 = 0.010; Fig. 4b). Similarly to time in LL vs. behavioral rhythmicity for WT mice however, visual interpretation of the time in LL vs. cellular synchrony plot for WT mice suggested progressively decreasing synchrony later in LL at extended (>29 days) durations of LL; we found a significant correlation between these parameters in WT mice when we assessed this >29 day in LL subset of animals only (Spearman’s rho = −0.875; p = 0.001; n = 10; Fig. 4b).

Discussion

Here we show that longer term exposure to constant light, a non-invasive environmental manipulation, promotes ~24 h behavioral rhythmicity and stable, synchronized cellular rhythms in the master circadian pacemaker of mice with an intercellular signaling deficit. This is in marked contrast to the disruptive and potentially detrimental influence of the same stimulus, constant light exposure, on circadian rhythms in animals with an intact circadian timing system. These findings provide novel insight into the restoration of circadian rhythmicity at behavioral and single cell levels in mice deficient in signaling via the VPAC2 receptor; a major conduit for SCN intercellular communication.

LL differentially altered the expression of behavioral rhythmicity in individual WT and Vipr2−/− mice. To identify potential factors underpinning the circadian profile of behavior, we assessed synchrony among SCN clock cells of rhythmic and arrhythmic animals of both genotypes. Compared with behaviorally rhythmic individuals, synchrony among per1::eGFP SCN neurons of behaviorally arrhythmic mice was reduced, regardless of genotype. Indeed in both genotypes, statistically significant intercellular synchrony was absent in slices from all behaviorally arrhythmic individuals. Importantly, the degree of synchrony among SCN clock cells from behaviorally rhythmic animals did not differ between WT and Vipr2−/− mice, despite a lower proportion of rhythmic individual cells in the Vipr2−/− SCN. In Vipr2−/− mice we found that, for both period and phase, the regional differences between dorsal and ventral areas of the SCN were reduced in rhythmic individuals. As such, across both genotypes, greater behavioral rhythmicity was associated with increased intra-SCN synchrony, but different features of the circadian architecture of the SCN are permissive of this effect between mice with intact and disrupted circadian timing systems. Notably, both temporal and spatial organization of SCN circadian function have been implicated in the generation and maintenance of robust tissue level oscillations34,35,36,37,38. Indeed, oscillations at the single-cell level have been reported to cluster into dorsal and ventral SCN subregions, with a smaller period difference between these subregions associated with greater cellular synchrony38. These data are consistent with the reduced dorsal-ventral SCN period difference in behaviorally rhythmic Vipr2−/− mice under LL and the increased SCN cellular synchrony observed for these mice, as well as their longer free-running behavioral periods.

A previous report of restored behavioral rhythmicity in mice bearing intracellular molecular clock defects was accompanied by the demonstration that tissue-level rhythms in clock gene expression can also be recovered16. It remains unclear, however, as to whether SCN cellular synchrony is also elevated in this and other models. Similarly, while a previous report provided preliminary evidence that the disruptive effects of LL are somewhat reduced in Vip−/− mice39, nothing has been reported of SCN function in this model under these conditions. Our current data demonstrate that the effect of an intercellular signaling deficit is diminished by exposure to LL, resulting in both improved wheel-running rhythms and increased SCN cellular synchrony. Interestingly and consistent with our WT data in extended LL (>29 days), in animals with fully functional intra- and intercellular SCN processes and indeed in some clock gene mutants with a less severe behavioral phenotype, exposure to LL is commonly associated with disruption of SCN molecular and neuronal function as well as the perturbation of other behavioral and physiological rhythms26,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49. Thus, constant light is frequently detrimental to circadian rhythmicity in animals with either strong or fully functional circadian clocks, but can be beneficial to animals with a severely weakened circadian system. Moreover, in addition to the effects of LL on adult circadian systems, maintaining animals under LL during development can also influence subsequent circadian function; intact rats and mice housed under LL during development display reduced disruptions to rhythmicity when housed under LL as adults50,51 and mice deficient in expression of the core clock genes Cry1 and Cry2 (Cry1−/−Cry2−/−), when raised under LL conditions, express improved rhythmicity in constant darkness compared to LD-raised counterparts52. The developmental effects on behavioral and SCN rhythmicity and synchrony of raising neuropeptide deficient mice in LL is currently unknown, though Ono et al. (2013) report that, unlike Vipr2−/− mice, housing adult LD-raised Cry1−/−Cry2−/−mice in LL does not improve circadian oscillations in locomotor activity.

Long-term exposure to LL resulted in a greater proportion of Vipr2−/− mice expressing clear circadian rhythmicity than is seen in DD (current data as well as24,32,53) and notably, the rhythmicity of mice under DD conditions was not predicted by the responses of each individual to LL. Unlike scheduled voluntary exercise, whose rhythm promoting actions on wheel-running in neuropeptide signaling deficient mice are sustained for up to 4 weeks once the exercise regimen has terminated53, the rhythm-promoting actions of LL on behavior rapidly deteriorate on transfer to DD. This illustrates differences in the long-term reorganizing properties of different external stimuli on the circadian system of mice with intercellular signaling deficits9,54. These data indicate that continuous exposure to this signal is necessary to organize the Vipr2−/− SCN to drive improved behavioral rhythmicity. However, initial exposure to constant light disrupted behavioral rhythms in Vipr2−/− mice, which also indicates longer term adaptation and remodeling of the SCN to continual activation of light input pathways. Indeed, the stabilizing actions of extended LL on behavioral rhythms were robustly reproducible; re-exposure of Vipr2−/− mice to LL for a second block of 36 days evoked the same behavioral parameters as had been observed in the first 36 day exposure, though interestingly, the ‘LL-like’ phenotype appeared to be more rapidly acquired during this second exposure, showing that although this phenotype is lost in DD some underlying changes at least partially persist.

There are several possible mechanisms underpinning the enhanced SCN cellular synchrony and improved rhythmic wheel-running arising from exposure to constant light. Electrophysiological investigations reveal that adult Vipr2−/− SCN neurons tend to be hyperpolarized55 and are less spontaneously active than SCN neurons from adult C57BL/6 mice56. Vipr2−/− SCN neurons retain acute responsiveness to glutamatergic signals56, such as those involved in transmitting photic information from the retina to the SCN, via the RHT19. Moreover, elevated electrical activity is sustained in adult Vipr2−/− SCN in vitro in response to continuous glutamatergic stimulation over several hours57. As the RHT utilizes both glutamate and PACAP, it is likely that either elevated glutamatergic tone, or alterations in the glutamate-PACAP relationship, arising from exposure to constant light over several weeks, excites Vipr2−/− neurons and improves their synchrony through stimulation of factor(s) that are independent of VIP-VPAC2 signaling. Candidates for this include: GABA, GRP and Neuromedin S, neurochemicals intrinsic to SCN neurons that have been implicated in cell-cell communication and SCN synchrony6,32,57,58; AVP, an abundant SCN-intrinsic neuropeptide59,60 with altered expression under LL5; and signaling via adenosine receptors, which are implicated in modifying the sensitivity of the SCN to retinal input61. Further, optic synapses in the SCN show plasticity62 and it is possible that constant light causes remodeling of synaptic contacts and gap junctions63 to improve SCN intercellular communication.

Outwith the direct effects of RHT signaling to the SCN, altered LL behavior of mice lacking 5-HT1a receptors suggests the potential for mechanistic involvement of arousal pathways in mediating the actions of LL on the circadian system28. Indeed, the possibility of increased activity of extra-SCN neural oscillators under LL has yet to be investigated and cannot be excluded64. Intriguingly, an as yet unidentified dopamine-sensitive extra-SCN oscillator, with the potential to explain a variety of non-canonical circadian phenomena, has recently been reported65,66. Interactions between such an oscillator and a weakened, but intact and functional SCN, such as that of Vipr2−/− mice, provide a further possible avenue for investigation in the context of the current data.

In conclusion, our data illustrate that long-term exposure to LL promotes behavioral rhythmicity and SCN cellular synchrony in mice with deficient VIP-VPAC2 signaling. Further, we present evidence that, regardless of genotype, behavioral rhythmicity correlates closely with intercellular synchrony in the master circadian pacemaker but that, in WT SCN, this is associated with an increase in rhythmic cells, whilst in the Vipr2−/− SCN, this is associated with reduced regional heterogeneity.

Methods

Animals

This study used adult male and female mice that expressed the per1::eGFP reporter and either expressed the Vipr2 gene (WT) or lacked expression of the VPAC2 receptor (Vipr2−/−, see7). Prior to experimentation, all mice were group housed under a 12 h light:12 h dark (LD) cycle with ad libitum access to food (Beekay, B&K Universal, Hull, UK) and water. Environmental temperature was maintained at 23 °C and humidity at 40%. All procedures and experimental protocols were carried out in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and approved by The University of Manchester Review Ethics Panel.

Experimental Design and Behavioral Assessment

WT and Vipr2−/− mice were individually housed in running wheel-equipped cages under a 12:12 LD cycle for 7–14 days (~3 × 1014 photons/s/cm2 lights on [~110 μW/cm2 lights on, from a broad spectrum fluorescent light source]) then released into LL (~3 × 1014 photons/s/cm2). Wheel revolutions were recorded in Chronobiology Kit (Stanford Software Systems, Santa Cruz, California, USA) using 5 min bins for analysis. Animals of each genotype were divided into 2 cohorts for assessment of 1) the effects of LL on wheel-running behavior (n = 12 WT, n = 24 Vipr2−/−) and 2) correlation of the effects of LL on wheel-running behavior with SCN per1::eGFP expression (n = 12 WT, n = 16 Vipr2−/−).

For experiment 1, mice were free-run undisturbed for 36 days of LL (LL1) during which behavioral rhythms were assessed in early (1-12 days), mid (13-24) and late LL (25-36). After LL1, n = 12 Vipr2−/− mice were placed in DD for 36 days before a further 36 day LL epoch (LL2). The remaining Vipr2−/− mice (n = 12) and all WT mice (n = 12) were removed from the experiment after LL1. Rhythmicity was assessed by 4 experienced observers, blind to genotype and experimental conditions, using a combined evaluation of locomotor actograms, periodograms, average waveforms and the time-frequency spectrogram obtained from sliding window fast Fourier transform (FFT)34, performed with a 7-day window that slid at 1-hr increments through wheel-running time series data smoothed with the Hodrick-Prescott filter (processed using Mathematica, Wolfram Research). In the majority of cases assessments of rhythmicity were unanimous (>90%), but for the infrequent cases where a consensus decision could not be easily reached, a final decision on rhythmicity was based on objective analysis of average waveforms. We determined that for a behavioral epoch to be considered rhythmic, a window centred on the most active part of the average waveform, consisting of 50% of its duration, should contain at least 75% of total activity. This method was validated on our entire data set and generated results entirely consistent with our prior assessments of behavior where we had initially been able to classify rhythmicity. For behavioral epochs that were clearly arrhythmic and for which no period could be determined from either periodogram analysis or visual inspection of actograms, a nominal value of 24 h was used to generate average waveforms. Where behavioral epochs were considered rhythmic, period was assessed in Chronobiology Kit using eyefit regression lines through onsets of activity and confirmed with spectral power analysis and Chi2 periodograms.

For experiment 2, mice were initially free-run in LL, as in experiment 1, though culled after increasing durations of LL ranging from a minimum of 22 days, to a maximum of 39 days. As the aim of this experiment was to investigate a possible association between time in LL, behavioral rhythmicity and SCN circadian function, mice were randomly assigned a cull timepoint in LL prior to the experiment commencing. This avoided the possibility of unintentional bias associated with selecting cull timepoints after behavioral data had already been collected. Immediately following cull, SCNs were collected for confocal imaging of per1::eGFP expression. Behavioral rhythms were analyzed for the last 10–14 days before cull and the behavior of all mice ranked in order of rhythmicity for correlation with SCN per1::eGFP data and duration of time in LL. Ranking was performed blind by 4 experienced observers and correlated strongly (R2 = 0.675; p < 0.0001) with the percentage of activity contained during a 50% window of the average waveform using the methods described above. Experimenters were blind to both the identity of mice in behavioral experiments and the parameters of SCN per1::eGFP data at the time of assessment for ranking of behavior. Behavior was further categorized as either rhythmic or arrhythmic (using the same procedure as in experiment 1) to make within-genotype comparisons of SCN per1::eGFP data according to behavioral rhythmicity.

Confocal Imaging

Mice were culled by cervical dislocation following isoflurane anesthesia (Baxter Healthcare Ltd., Norfolk, UK), at circadian time (CT) 2–6 (to avoid different cull times influencing circadian parameters of SCN function67,68). Cultures were prepared as described previously7 using 250 μm thick SCN-containing coronal brain slices and 100 μg/ml and 25 μg/ml penicillin-streptomycin (Gibco Invitrogen Ltd, Paisley, UK) in collection medium and culture medium, respectively. Cultures were stored in darkness at 37 °C for ~24 h before imaging per1::eGFP fluorescence using a C1 confocal system running on a TE2000 inverted microscope (Nikon, Kingston, UK). Images were captured with a 10 x 0.3NA PlanFluotar objective (Nikon) and the system maintained at 37 °C. A 488 nm laser was used for excitation and emitted fluorescence detected using a 515/30 nm bandpass filter. 8 image ‘Z’ stacks were acquired every hour for ~120 h, using 4x Kalman averaging, 1.5x confocal zoom, an open pinhole and 0.2 Hz frame rate. Each stack covered a total Z depth of 32 μm and images were recorded at 512 × 512 pixels. Stacks were collapsed to an average projection using ImageJ and fluorescence profiles across time assessed for 30 individual cells selected at random7 using a region of interest tool. Raw fluorescence data were corrected for variations in background brightness by subtracting the brightness of a standardized, non-eGFP expressing, extra-SCN region from each data value before corrected data were smoothed using a 3 h running mean.

Cells were rated as rhythmic or arrhythmic by two experienced observers and amplitude, period and phase (time of peak at 12–36 hours) were assessed for each analyzed cell. The period of cellular rhythms was calculated using peak-peak and trough-trough durations for at least two cycles and phase data for individual cells were used to create Rayleigh plots to quantify the synchrony (phase-clustering) of rhythms between cells within each SCN69. Amplitude was calculated as the brightness differential from the peak used for phase analysis (12–36 hours into recording) to the following trough. Regional differences in period and phase within the SCN between behaviorally rhythmic and arrhythmic animals (period and phase heterogeneity) were assessed by classifying the location of each analyzed cell as either dorsal or ventral, based on anatomical characteristics of the SCN and comparing the mean difference in each parameter between cells located in these two subregions.

Statistics

As appropriate, statistically significant differences in continuous data were determined using either t-Tests (paired or unpaired) or one/two way ANOVA with a priori pairwise comparisons. Categorical data for the percentage of rhythmic and arrhythmic animals were analyzed using Fisher’s Exact Test with Freeman-Halton extension. Rayleigh Tests were used to determine statistical significance of cellular synchrony and the Rayleigh R statistic used to quantify the degree of synchrony. Statistical significances of correlations for ranked data were assessed using the non-parametric Spearman’s Rank Order Test. All statistical tests were run with α set at p < 0.05 required for significance, using Microsoft Excel, Graphpad Prism, the VasserStats online statistical resource (http://vassarstats.net/) and custom MATLAB scripts provided by Dr. Timothy Brown (University of Manchester).

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Hughes, A. T.L. et al. Constant light enhances synchrony among circadian clock cells and promotes behavioral rhythms in VPAC2-signaling deficient mice. Sci. Rep. 5, 14044; doi: 10.1038/srep14044 (2015).