Introduction

Our ability to interpret responses of neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) to movies of natural scenes is limited1,2,3. The neuronal responses to the attributes of images play crucial roles in constructing the representation of daily vivid scenes from retinal images4,5. The classic studies in this field are predominantly focused on contrast without reporting the coding of uniform luminance6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16. This has led to a poor understanding of the responses of V1 cells to uniform luminance in comparison with what is known of their responses to contrast.

The roles of the uniform luminance-sensitive cells in visual functions remain largely unclear17,18,19,20,21. Early studies showed that V1 cells respond to diffuse illumination with sustained, transient, or slowly adapting discharges22,23,24. V1 cells also respond to the luminance of large patches of static uniform stimuli19,25. V1 responses have been shown to represent the brightness of uniform surfaces17,19,26,27. Moreover, V1 cells respond to uniform luminance modulated sinusoidally in time20,21. The response magnitudes of V1 cells depend on the levels of the stimulus luminance19,22,25. Response profiles of most V1 cells to large uniform stimuli show a monotonic increase or decrease as a function of luminance, but a minority show a V-shaped profile. The V profiles are often time-dependent with monotonic responses more evident in the later period of the stimulation19. Some cells respond maximally to the intermediate luminances when the stimulus luminance is slowly modulated sinusoidally20,21.

A few studies18,21,25 have directly compared responses of V1 cells to uniform luminance and contrast stimuli. To large uniform spots with various luminances brighter than the background, orientation selective cells show saturated responses to luminances over a limited range, while cells without orientation selectivity respond differentially to luminance over a larger range25. When stimulus size increases, responses to uniform luminance either increase or decrease; the effects do not reliably correlate with those to contrast gratings18. In response to a large patch of stimulation, V1 cells form two main groups: luminance-contrast cells that respond to both uniform luminance and contrast and contrast cells that respond only to contrast but not to uniform luminance. Responses to uniform luminance are weak in comparison with those to contrast21. Luminance-contrast cells tend to have large receptive fields (RFs), low spatial frequencies and weaker orientation selectivity than contrast cells. The responses of contrast cells to contrast signals are linearly correlated to the spatial ON-OFF structure of the RF, but those of luminance-contrast cells to contrast are not21. Other studies have reported that luminance changes in the background delay and suppress responses to contrast28,29. Although these studies shed light on the response properties of uniform luminance-sensitive cells, we still largely lack understanding of the relevance of these cells in visual functions.

V1 cells are sensitive to transiently changing contrast that resembles natural stimulation30. Since responses to both diffuse light and uniform luminance contain transient components19,22, it is possible that V1 cells also respond to such briefly presented luminance stimuli that resemble the luminance variations occurring in natural vision. To address the roles of uniform luminance-sensitive cells in processing transiently changing luminance, we investigated responses of cat V1 cells to large patches of uniform stimuli or grating stimuli that changed rapidly and consecutively in luminance and compared the speed preferences of luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells to the conventional drifting sinusoidal gratings. We show that the cells preferring decrements in luminance of uniform stimuli tend to prefer high speeds of motion. The results argue for a functional role for the luminance-sensitive cells in the detection of motion especially under dim light conditions.

Results

We presented uniform stimuli (0% contrast) and spatial stimuli (static sinusoidal gratings of 100% contrast, Fig. 1a) of different luminances to cells in cat V1. When the stimulus size ≤ RF, the contrast generated at the stimulus border by the difference between stimulus luminance and background luminance strongly impacted the neuronal responses. The effects were particularly strong for responses to uniform stimuli. Therefore, we used large circular stimuli with diameters at least 3 times larger than the size of the classical receptive field (RF) of the cell17,19,26,27. The contrast effects on responses to the luminance of the interior of the stimulus patch were also removed by blurring the border with a smooth change in luminance from the stimulus level to the background (25 cd/m2; Fig. 1a). Uniform stimuli included eleven luminance values from 0 to 50 cd/m2 in steps of 5 cd/m2 (5 of them decreasing and 5 increasing from the mean of 25 cd/m2 and the remaining one equal to the mean luminance). Gratings had a mean luminance equal to those of the eleven uniform stimuli (individually corresponding to 0–50 cd/m2 in steps of 5 cd/m2). The stimulus luminances were mostly within 1–1.5 log unit ranges that are typical of natural images31. Uniform stimuli were presented randomly one after another, each for 40 ms without blank intervals (the subspace reverse correlation method)34,35,36,37 and gratings were presented in the same way in a separate block (Fig. 1a). Thus, the luminance changes having different magnitudes in both presentations were random. The stimulus presentation simulated luminance changes carried by a uniform stimulus or a spatial contrast stimulus that often occur in natural vision. Note that the sole difference between the two kinds of stimuli was that the gratings had 100% contrast and the uniform stimuli had 0% contrast. The stimulus contrast was constant in a block of gratings presentations and therefore differed from our previous study21 in which the stimulus contrast varied slowly and sinusoidally.

Figure 1
figure 1

Visual stimuli and responses of V1 cells to luminance and motion speed.

(a) Luminance stimuli. Each stimulus was presented for 40 ms and all stimuli were presented successively and randomly without interval in a block. Uniform and grating stimuli were in separate blocks. Upper panels: uniform luminance stimuli (0% contrast); Lower panels: luminance expressed by grating stimuli (100% contrast). Dark circles indicate the receptive field of a cell. (b,c) Luminance response functions (LRFs) of a luminance-contrast cell to uniform and grating stimuli, respectively. (e,f) LRFs of a contrast cell. (d,g) The speed tuning curves of the two cells used as examples in (b,c) and (e,f). The X-axis is a logarithmic scale of motion speeds. The Y-axis is the normalized response by the maximal response. In (b), (c), (e), (f), the dots are raw data. In (d), (g), the dots are the normalized data after normalization by the maximal response and the lines are the fitted curves. Arrow heads in (b), (c), (e), (f) indicate the background luminance (25 cd/m2).

Luminance response functions (LRFs) were extracted from responses to different stimulus luminances at the time of the largest response variance when a cell showed the best tuning. Figs. 1b and 1c depict LRFs of a V1 cell to uniform stimuli and to grating stimuli. This cell was significantly tuned (see Data Analyses) to luminances of both kinds of stimuli. In a population of cells, 41% (109/269) showed this response behavior. Since the gratings contained 100% contrast, this group of cells was denoted as luminance-contrast cells. Many other cells (56%, 151/269) did not respond to uniform stimuli but only responded significantly to the luminances of the contrast gratings (i.e., contrast cells). An example of the latter cells is shown in Figs. 1e and 1f. The remaining cells (3%, 9/269) responded only to uniform luminance but not to gratings (i.e., luminance cells; not shown). Note that by distinguishing luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells in the current study we do not mean to classify V1 cells in the same manner as simple and complex cells and that within the confines of presenting large uniform and grating stimuli, V1 cells appear to be luminance-contrast cells or contrast cells. In fact, in the current experiment, responses to gratings were modulated by the luminances, but not by the contrasts, of the static gratings, because all gratings had the same 100% contrast. The variations in responses were due to luminance changes of the gratings. Responses to the luminance changes, however, were affected predominantly by the contrast of grating stimuli because, in general, these responses were stronger in magnitude than those to the luminance changes of uniform stimuli (Figs. 1b,1c,1e,1f) and were monotonic increases (Figs. 1c,1f), which is the typical response profile to stimulus contrast8. In our previous study21, the responses of cells to contrast stimuli were modulated by the sinusoidally changing contrast at a single border and in the current study, the responses of cells to contrast gratings were evoked by luminance changes. Although these responses to the two types of contrast stimuli are somewhat different, the response properties of luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells in both studies are similar. Here we still use the terms of luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells to conveniently facilitate the succinct depiction of our current study and in relation to our previous work21.

These 269 cells were recorded from the region representing the central visual field across all cortical layers of V1. Their basic response properties, including RF position, size and structure, were similar to those reported in our previous study21. To discriminate simple cells and complex cells, we analyzed the modulated responses of all cells to drifting gratings at 100% contrast. Crowder et al. (2007) have shown that F1/F0 values of complex cells depend on the stimulus contrast, but those of simple cells do not and concluded that only the cells with a modulation index (F1/F0) > 1 in response to 100% contrast gratings can be regarded as real simple cells38. Based on this strict criterion, 15% of the cells (40/269) in our sample were simple cells and 85% (229/269) were complex cells (F1/F0 < 1). The percentage of simple cells was reported as approximately 11% by Crowder et al.'s38 and was 13% in our previous sample21.

We examined the relationships between contrast cells vs luminance-contrast cells and simple cells vs complex cells. Of the sampled cells, 17% (18/109) of luminance-contrast cells, 13% (20/151) of contrast cells and 22% (2/9) of luminance cells were simple cells. We also analyzed the RF spatiotemporal structure of luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells by calculating the spatial and temporal overlap indices of ON and OFF subregions21 measured with sparse stimuli presented by the reverse correlation method32,33. Overall, no clear differences were observed between luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells in relation to the classes of simple cells vs complex cells and to the spatiotemporal organization of their RF ON-OFF subregions. This result is consistent with our previous study21 in which luminance-contrast cells and contrast cell were identified by slowly and sinusoidally changing luminance and contrast. Possibly the distinctions between luminance-contrast cells vs contrast cells and simple cells vs complex cells and the related RF ON-OFF structure largely lie in the large size of the stimuli we applied. With a large stimulus, a cell must integrate inputs from a region far beyond the classical RF. Responses to luminance decrements and increments in a large field are quite different from ON and OFF responses evoked by the sparse, small stimuli used to map a RF. Responses evoked by the large stimulus are also distinct from the modulated responses evoked by the drifting sinusoidal gratings used to measure F1/F0 values. In the following sections, we therefore analyzed the response properties of luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells without regard to the simple and complex classes.

We compared the speed selectivity of the luminance-contrast cells with the contrast cells. The tuning curves of the V1 cells to motion speeds were measured for sinusoidal gratings drifting at different speeds along the preferred directions of motion for each cell. Note that the speed tunings we studied here were measured at the preferred spatial frequency of each V1 cell (see Methods). Fig. 1d plots the fitted speed tuning curve of a typical luminance-contrast cell with a preferred speed at 10.0 °/s, while Fig. 1g is for that of a typical contrast cell with a preferred speed at 4.25 °/s. This example of a luminance-contrast cell had a preferred motion speed higher than the example of the contrast cell. The averaged speed tuning curves from raw data of responses across 109 luminance-contrast cells and 151 contrast cells are shown in Fig. 2g. It is obvious that luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells differ in their ranges of preferred speeds. The speed tuning curves of individual cells were fitted by a log-Gaussian function [equation (2)] to obtain the preferred speed (defined as the peak of the fitted curves, e.g., Figs. 1d,1g) for the drifting gratings. The distributions of preferred speeds of the population of contrast cells and of the population of luminance-contrast cells are shown in Figs. 2h and 2i, respectively. The mean preferred speed of contrast cells was 4.6 ± 0.20 °/s (mean ± s.e.m; n = 151; Fig. 2h) and that of luminance-contrast cells was 8.6 ± 0.24 °/s (n = 109; Fig. 2i). The difference was statistically significant (One-way ANOVA, P < 0.0001), indicating that luminance-contrast cells prefer higher motion speeds than contrast cells at the population level.

Figure 2
figure 2

Luminance-contrast cells preferred lower SFs, higher TFs and higher motion speeds than contrast cells.

(a,d,g) The averaged SF, TF and speed response curves of raw data of 151 contrast cells and 109 luminance-contrast cells after normalization by the maximal response for each cell (e.g., Figs. 1d,1g). LC: luminance-contrast; C: contrast. The vertical lines show the s.e.m. (b,e,h) Distributions of preferred SFs, TFs and motion speeds of 151 contrast cells. (c,f,i) Distributions of those of 109 luminance-contrast cells. The X-axis is a logarithmic scale of SFs, TFs and motion speeds. Arrows indicate the mean values. In (i), the vertical dashed lines indicate ± SD of preferred speeds for the data analysis of Fig. 5a.

The difference in speed tuning could be attributed either to the differences in responses of neurons to the spatial frequencies (SFs) or to the temporal frequencies (TFs) of drifting sinusoidal gratings. We further examined the SF and TF tunings of the luminance-contrast cells and the contrast cells. Figs. 2a and 2d show the averaged SF and TF curves of raw data for responses across the 109 luminance-contrast cells and the 151 contrast cells, respectively. It is noteworthy that the luminance-contrast cells show large differences in the SF tuning and small differences in TF tuning compared to the contrast cells. The SF and TF tuning curves of individual cells were fitted by a difference of Gaussian (DOG) functions [equation (1)] and a log-Gaussian function [equation (2)], respectively. Figs. 2b,2e and 2c,2f display the distributions of preferred SFs and TFs of contrast cells and luminance-contrast cells, respectively. The mean preferred SF of contrast cells was 0.55 ± 0.024 cycles/° (n = 151; Fig. 2b) and that of luminance-contrast cells was 0.40 ± 0.030 cycles/° (n = 109; Fig. 2c). This difference was significant (One-way ANOVA, P < 0.001). These tuning curves to SFs were obtained using static sinusoidal gratings flashed consecutively, each for 40 ms (subspace reverse correlation method). The data confirm the preliminary result from a relatively small number of sample cells in our previous study in which SFs were tested using drifting gratings21. Thus, different tests show similar results, namely, that luminance-contrast cells prefer low SFs in comparison to contrast cells. The average of the preferred TFs of contrast cells was 4.1 ± 0.15 cycles/s (n = 151; Fig. 2e) and that of luminance-contrast cells was 5.4 ± 0.20 cycles/s (n = 109; Fig. 2f). Again this difference was significant (One-way ANOVA, P < 0.01). Thus, both differences in SFs and TFs tuning possibly contribute to the difference of speed tunings between luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells.

One can ask whether luminance-contrast cells have response latencies that are shorter than those of contrast cells because they prefer faster drifting gratings. Therefore, we analyzed response latencies using the responses to the drifting gratings in the preferred direction of each cell rather than using the responses to the flashed stimuli. At the times when a cell showed the onset of the significant responses, the half peak magnitude and the peak magnitude of responses were computed. The time of the onset significant response was defined as the first point of five consecutive data points (in a step of 1 ms) in which all five data points had to meet the criterion for the significant response, that is, the response magnitude beyond the mean ± 2 SDs of the baseline spikes. From Fig. 3, it can be seen that differences of response latencies between luminance-contrast cells and contrast cells gradually increased from the onset time (35 ± 6.2 ms, n = 109 vs 41.9 ± 5.3 ms, n = 151), half peak time (52.3 ± 3.9 ms, n = 109 vs 65.7 ± 4.4 ms, n = 151) to the peak time (62.3 ± 3.6 ms, n = 109 vs 79.0 ± 4.5 ms, n = 151). The difference was significant for the response latencies at the half peak magnitude and at the peak magnitude (One-way ANOVA, P < 0.05). These data suggest that responses of luminance-contrast cells to drifting gratings reach their peak magnitude more rapidly on average than do those of contrast cells.

Figure 3
figure 3

Response latencies of 109 luminance-contrast cells and 151 contrast cells to drifting sinusoidal gratings along the preferred directions of the cells.

The bars and vertical lines show the mean ± s.e.m; * and the horizontal lines show the one-way ANOVA, P < 0.05. Onset: latency of the onset significant response; 1/2 peak: latency of half peak response; Peak: latency of the peak response.

Most luminance-sensitive cells respond to decrements in luminance20,21. In the current experiment, a cell might respond to luminance decrements (monotonically decreased LRF, e.g., Fig. 1b) or luminance increments (monotonically increased LRF) at the peak of its response variance curve, or to both at the peak and the sub-peak of the curve (e.g., Figs. 4a–c). In the sample of luminance-contrast cells, 50% (54/109) of cells had two peaks in response to both luminance decrements and increments (dark and light gray bars in Fig. 4d). Here, in order to quantitatively study the preference of a cell for luminance increments (lightening) and luminance decrements (darkening), its relative response strength to the increments and decrements was evaluated by an increment-decrement response index (IDRI). The responses of a cell to luminance increments and/or decrements were assessed with a stricter statistical criterion (see Data Analyses). A value 1 of IDRI indicates that a cell only responds significantly to luminance increments, 0 indicates no bias in response to either increments or decrements and −1 indicates that a cell responds only to decrements. The example cell shown in Figs. 4a–c had −0.20 of IDRI, responding better to luminance decrements than to increments. The mean IDRI of all luminance-contrast cells was −0.40, indicating a strong response bias to luminance decrements at the population level. Among the cells, 42% (46/109) responded only to decrements (IDRI = −1, dark bar in Fig. 4d), 34% (37/109) had a larger response magnitude to decrements than to increments (−1 < IDRI < −0.1, dark gray bars in Fig. 4d), 16% (17/109) had approximately equal responses to decrements and increments (−0.1 ≤ IDRI ≤ 0.1, light gray bars in Fig. 4d) and the remaining 8% (9/109) responded only to increments (IDRI = 1, light bars in Fig. 4d). Therefore, there are many more luminance-contrast cells responding strongly to luminance decrements than to increments.

Figure 4
figure 4

Uniform luminance-sensitive cells strongly preferred luminance decrements.

(a) Strong responses of a cell to luminance decrements at the response latency labeled with a in the variance curve of panel (c). (b) Weak responses of this cell to luminance increments at the response latency labeled with b in the variance curve of panel (c). Dots: raw data. (c) Variance curve of responses of this cell to uniform luminance stimuli. Curves in panels (a) and (b) are responses of this cell to luminance decrements and luminance increments at the times of the response variance curve indicated by a and b in panel (c), respectively. The cell had the first peak at 36 ms (a) and the secondary peak at 88 ms (b). Among the luminance-contrast cells having two significant response peaks (dark and light gray bars in Fig. 4d), the average first peak time was 53.9 ± 2.8 ms (mean ± s.e.m., n = 54) and the second peak time was 84.7 ± 4.7 ms. The mean difference between the two peak times was 30.8 ± 1.9 ms. (d) Distribution of increment-decrement response indices (IDRI; X-axis) of 109 luminance-contrast cells. Dark bar: the cells responded only to luminance decrements, IDRI = −1; Dark gray bars: the cells responded to luminance decrements more strongly than to increments, −1 < IDRI < −0.1; Light gray bars, −0.1 ≤ IDRI ≤ 0.1: the cells responded equally to luminance decrements and increments; Light bar: the cells responded only to luminance increments, IDRI = 1. Arrow heads in (a), (b) indicate the background luminance (25 cd/m2).

Since the above results show that luminance-contrast cells prefer higher speeds and most of them respond better to luminance decrements, do the cells selective for high speeds, for example, those on the right side in Fig. 2i, really tend to prefer luminance decrements? To address this question, we analyzed the relationship between IDRIs and preferred motion speeds of the luminance-contrast cells. The scatter plot between the IDRIs and the preferred speeds for these cells, however, did not show a clear correlation because the distribution of IDRIs was widely separated, that is, as shown in Fig. 4d, forming three clusters. Therefore, we explored the relationship in the other ways by dividing these luminance-contrast cells into different groups based on either their preferred speeds or their IDRIs. Using their preferred speeds, 109 luminance-contrast cells were divided into three groups: one group of cells (n = 14) had a preferred speed < (mean − SD) shown by the left dashed line in Fig. 2i (slow speed); the other group (n = 15) had a preferred speed > (mean + SD) shown by the right dashed line in Fig. 2i (fast speed); and the third group (n = 80) had a preferred speed between (mean − SD) and (mean + SD) shown by the intermediate region between the two dashed lines in Fig. 2i (medium speed). Then, the mean IDRIs of the three groups of cells were calculated. Overall, all three groups had negative IDRIs, indicating that in each group most cells preferred luminance decrements. The mean IDRIs of the slow, medium and fast speed groups of cells were −0.21 ± 0.23 (n = 14), −0.41 ± 0.05 (n = 80) and −0.54 ± 0.19 (n = 15). From Fig. 5a, it can be seen that the mean IDRI tended to be more negative when the preferred speeds increased. This decrease in the IDRIs with the increase in the preferred speeds was statistically significant (One-way ANOVA, P < 0.05). On the other hand, the luminance-contrast cells were divided into three groups according to their IDRIs: one group of cells only responsive to luminance decrements (IDRI = −1; dark bars of Figs. 4d,5b) on average had a preferred speed of 11.3 ± 2.14 °/s (n = 46); the other group responsive to both decrements and increments (−0.5 ≤ IDRI ≤ 0.1; gray bars of Figs. 4d,5b) had a preferred speed of 8.3 ± 2.36 °/s (n = 54); and the third group responsive only to increments (IDRI = 1; light bars of Figs. 4d,5b) had a preferred speed of 5.9 ± 2.14 °/s (n = 9). The mean preferred speeds of the three groups of cells significantly decreased when their IDRIs increased (Fig. 5b; One-way ANOVA, P < 0.05). Thus, these results analyzed from the standpoints of both preferred speed and IDRI support the view that luminance-contrast cells preferring high speeds are more likely to respond to luminance decrements.

Figure 5
figure 5

Relationship between the preferences of luminance-contrast cells for motion speeds and for luminance decrements vs increments.

(a) Mean IDRIs of three groups of luminance-contrast cells having slow, medium and fast speeds. The 109 cells were divided by the (mean – SD) and (mean + SD) of the preferred speeds of all luminance-contrast cells shown in Fig. 2i into the groups with slow (n = 14), medium (n = 80) and fast speeds (n = 15). For details, see the main text. (b) Mean speeds of three groups of luminance-contrast cells responsive only to luminance decrements (IDRI = −1, dark bar; n = 46), to both luminance decrements and increments (−0.5 < IDRI ≤ 0.1, gray bar; n = 54) and only to luminance increments (IDRI = 1, light bar; n = 9). For IDRI distribution of these luminance-contrast cells, see Fig. 4d. For details, see the main text. In (a), (b), the bars and vertical lines: mean ± s.e.m.

Discussion

Our main finding is that in cat V1 the cells responsive to uniform luminance prefer more rapidly moving stimuli than the cells responsive only to the luminances expressed by contrast stimuli. Most uniform luminance-sensitive cells also strongly prefer luminance decrements over luminance increments. These cells also respond to contrast, orientation and SF of visual stimuli21. Based on these response properties, we speculate that luminance-sensitive cells in cat V1 play roles in motion processing under conditions of dim illumination.

It is well known that in the cat's retinogeniculate pathway, Y cells tend to prefer lower SFs and higher speeds much as magnocellular cells and X cells prefer higher SFs and lower speeds much as parvocellular cells in primates39,40,41,42,43,44. Here we show that luminance-contrast cells prefer low SFs, high TFs and high speeds, like Y cells, whereas contrast cells prefer high SFs, low TFs and low speeds, like X cells. The correlated relationships between the retinogeniculate cells and the cortical cells in these response properties suggest that the input of the retinogeniculate Y pathway contributes to the responses of luminance-contrast cells to low SFs, high TFs and high speeds and that of the X pathway contributes to the responses of contrast cells. The results of SFs, TFs and motion speeds further support the roles of Y pathway in the generation of responses of luminance-contrast cells to uniform luminance in V1 discussed in our previous study21.

Uniform stimuli are the extreme of low SFs. Thus, responses of luminance-contrast cells to uniform luminance can be attributed to their tuning preference for the lower SFs. Moreover, their preference for high speeds can be accounted for by their tuning preferences for low SFs and high TFs. The response tunings of most V1 cells to SFs and TFs are separable. The separability of SF and TF tunings leads to the view that the SF and TF tunings are independent, that is, the SF that evokes the best response is the same across TFs and vice versa. Thus, the preferred speed of V1 cells decreases as a function of SF and increases as a function of TF45. The model of speed tuning in relation with SFs and TFs predicts that the cells preferring lower SFs respond best to higher speeds and the cells preferring higher TFs also respond best to higher speeds. Therefore, the preferences of luminance-contrast cells for lower SFs and higher TFs determine their preference for higher speeds and the preferences of contrast cells for higher SF and lower TF determine their preference for lower speeds. It is possible that the tuning preference of luminance-contrast cells for high speeds is built upon the inputs of the subcortical Y pathway carrying the low SF and high TF information and is further strengthened by the intracortical circuits that emphasizes the processing of the motion and luminance information.

Luminance and contrast information are largely processed independently in the early visual pathway from retina to geniculate to V112,21,31,46,47. The coding strategy endows the visual system with the advantages of detecting contrast across variations in luminance and detecting luminance across variations in contrast. Thus, the finding that most luminance-sensitive cells prefer fast motion and luminance decrements is of special behavioral and ecological significance because these cells signal fast moving stimuli even when light illumination decreases. This would aid perception and help an animal navigating through a low visibility environment, such as dark night and fog. This would also be particularly useful to a nocturnal species such as a cat which hunts at night but is small and therefore vulnerable to predators. Each species adapts to its own natural environment. Cats have excellent night vision and are much more sensitive to the low light level than primates48. This is partly because cat eyes have a tapetum lucidum which increases the sensitivity to dim light49. Thus, it is not unexpected that we found cells in cat V1 signaling information about stimuli moving at a high speed in conditions that would be deemed poor visibility for primates.

Using artificial stimuli with carefully designed parameters is the most effective method for in advancing our understanding of visual functions1,2,3,50. The transient luminance changes (with a timescale of 40 ms) carried by uniform stimuli and spatial stimuli such as the ones we used are similar to what occurs in natural vision47,51,52. The transient stimuli were well parameterized in terms of luminance, which ranged from 0 to 50 cd/m2 in steps of 5 cd/m2 and of contrast, which was maintained constant at 0% or 100%. The stimuli are somewhat simpler than natural stimuli2,50,53,54,55, but also contain the richness of natural stimuli in the dimension of luminance change because the random presentation of stimulus luminances simulates natural luminance changes47,51,52. The dynamic luminance stimuli combined with the traditional drifting spatial gratings captures response behaviors of V1 luminance-contrast cells to transiently changing luminances, revealing the functional roles of these cells in visual motion.

Methods

Physiological preparations

All animal care and experimental procedures conformed to the guidelines of the National Institutes of Health (USA) and were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Usage Committee of The Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences. Twenty-four normal adult cats (2–3 kg) were prepared for single-unit recording using procedures described in a previous study21. Animal anesthesia was induced by ketamine (20–30 mg/kg, i.m.) after injection of dexamethasone and atropine (i.m.). During recording, anesthesia and paralysis were maintained with an infusion of sufentanil (0.15–0.22 μg/kg/hr, i.v.), propofol (1.8–2.2 mg/kg/hr, i.v.) and gallamine triethiodide (10 mg/kg/hr, i.v.) in a Ringers solution containing 5% glucose. The corneas were protected with contact lenses that had sufficient power with 3 mm artificial pupils to focus the eyes on a CRT monitor at a distance of 57 cm. A glass-coated tungsten electrode (1–3 MΩ) and a TDT amplifier and data acquisition system (TDT, Inc, Florida) at a sample rate of 12 kHz were used to record extracellular action potentials from neurons in V1 (centered at Horsley-Clarke coordinates P 2.5 mm and L 2.5 mm) where their RFs represented the central visual field. Individual units were further identified off-line with a TDT OpenSorter. The time delay for the recording system from stimulus generation to data acquisition was calibrated.

Visual stimuli

Stimuli were displayed on a CRT monitor (Iiyama HM204DT A, 800 × 600 pixel resolution, 100 Hz refresh rate) that subtended 40 × 30° in visual angle in a dark room (0.1 cd/m2 measured using ColorCAL colorimeter [CRS, Ltd]). The display was calibrated to remove luminance nonlinearities and obtain a precise match between the requested and actual luminances. The animal and the space between the animal and the monitor were covered or surrounded with black boards to allow the eyes of the animal to face only the display in order to avoid scatter effects from other light sources and reflections. The size and location of the RF for the dominant eye of cells being recording were explored with stimuli controlled by a computer mouse and the other eye was covered. After the preliminary tests, the preferred orientation (0°–165° in 15° steps) and spatial frequency (SF, 0.1–2.3 cycles/°) were measured quantitatively using the subspace reverse correlation technique with sinusoidal gratings34,35,36,37 and the classical RFs were measured quantitatively using the reverse correlation technique with static white and dark short bars (1.5° × 0.5°)32,33. Preference for motion direction and temporal frequency (TF) were measured with 100% sinusoidal gratings having the preferred SF and drifting along the preferred direction. For a cell, the exact sampled data points of TF (0.125–10 Hz) were dependent on the preferred TF of the cell.

Uniform and spatial stimuli were applied to study responses of a cell to luminance. Both stimuli had the same size. The width of blurred regions equaled to those of non-blurred regions of the stimuli (Fig. 1a). For the luminance expressed by a spatial contrast stimulus, all gratings had 100% contrast (defined by Michelson contrast for a spatially periodic pattern), the preferred orientation and preferred SF of the cell. Each grating stimulus contained 8 spatial phases (0–7 π/4 in a step of π/4). Each stimulus was presented for 40 ms and repeated at least 800 times. For other stimulus parameters, see the main text.

Data analyses

Responses to a given stimulus luminance were sorted using the reverse correlation method from the spike train recorded from a cell to a stimulation sequence in order to obtain the average firing rate34,35,36,37. In the reverse correlation algorithm, an array of counters corresponding to each of the stimuli was reset to zero. To relate a stimulus to the spikes evoked by it in a spike train recorded, τ denoted the time delay of the spikes after stimulus onset. For each spike, we went back τ ms in time to look for the stimulus corresponding to the spike at that moment in the stimulus sequence. The counter corresponding to that stimulus had one added to it. For grating stimuli, responses to gratings having the same luminance but at 8 spatial phases were sorted to the same counter. Responses to a stimulus at a delay time of τ were counted across the spike train. The time course of responses was generated by shifting τ from −150 ms to 300 ms in 1 ms steps. The variance curve of responses of a cell to different uniform stimuli or grating stimuli was calculated with a 1 ms resolution (e.g., Fig. 4c). At the true response (optimal) latency, a cell responded best to the preferred stimulus and poorly to the non-preferred stimulus, therefore it had the maximal variance of responses to different stimuli and had the best tuning curve. To take an objective criterion for data selection, we estimated the noise level of responses by calculating the variance of spikes to all stimuli, τ from −150 to 0 ms, before stimulus onset. The mean and SD of these variances were obtained. The mean + 4 SDs of variances of the noise spikes from −150 to 0 ms (noise level) were taken as the criterion36,37. We accepted the data in which the maximal value of response variances to different luminances after stimulus onset exceeded this criterion of noise level. Luminance response functions (LRFs) were extracted from responses to different luminance stimuli at the time of the peak or at the secondary peak variance (e.g., Figs. 4a–c).

The SF tuning curve was fitted by difference of Gaussian (DOG) functions:

and R0, Pe, Pi, μe, μi, σe and σi were optimized to provide the least squared error fit to the data. The preferred SF is the peak of SF curve56.

The motion speeds were calculated by V (°/s) = TF (cycles/s) / SF (cycles/°). The speed (s) tuning curve was fitted by a log-Gaussian function:

and R0, A, s0, sp and σ are free parameters. sp is the preferred speed and σ is the width of the curve (in log speed)57,58. The TF tuning curve was also fitted by the log-Gaussian function.

To compare responses of a cell to luminance increments and decrements, a increment-decrement response index (IDRI) is defined as:

where RIncrement is response magnitude to luminance increasing from 25 cd/m2 of the average luminance to 50 cd/m2 and RDecrement is response magnitude to luminance decreasing from 25 cd/m2 of the average luminance to 0 cd/m2, respectively. The responses of a cell to luminance decrements and increments were evaluated with a stricter statistical criterion. The evaluation was based only on responses to three decrement stimuli of 0, 5 and 10 cd/m2 and on those to three increment stimuli of 40, 45 and 50 cd/m2. Response variances to the two groups of stimuli were calculated from 0 ms to 300 ms in 1 ms steps, respectively. Only the responses that met the criterion for a significant response, that is, a variance of responses to the group of three stimuli was beyond the mean ± 4 SDs of the variances of the noise spikes from −150 to 0 ms, were included in the IDRI analysis. Then, we determined the times at which the largest response variance occurred when the cell showed the best tuning to luminance decrements of the three stimuli or to luminance increments of the three stimuli. Next, The maximal responses (RIncrement and/or Rdecrement) were extracted from the monotonic response curves to the three increment stimuli and/or to the three decrement stimuli at the times of the largest variance, respectively. If the response of a cell to increment or decrement stimuli did not meet the criterion for a significant response, its RIncrement or Rdecrement was zero.