Introduction

Maintaining proper osmolarity is essential for life, and thus is under strong regulation. Internal water homeostasis can be monitored by tissue or cellular osmolarity1,2 or osmolarity/fluid volume within the circulatory system3,4. Mammals monitor internal water both through peripheral sensors in the kidney5 and central receptors in the brain6. Body osmolarity can then be regulated through excretion/retention or consumption of water. Water intake is regulated through actions in the hypothalamus7 and lamina terminalis6, and a well characterised endocrine system involving renin/angiotensin8, which promote thirst, and vasopressin9, which drives water retention. Dysregulation of internal water can result in dehydration and hypernatremia (high sodium in the blood), leading to low blood pressure, cognitive impairment, and in serious instances kidney injury, intracranial haemorrhage, and death10. As such water seeking and consumption is a robust innate behaviour worth systematic cellular and molecular interrogation.

Much progress has been made characterising the receptors and circuitry regulating water consumption in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Early work by the Benzer lab described a robust humidity-seeking behaviour (hygrosensation) in the fly, mediated through a humidity sensor in the arista (on the antenna) and dependent on TRP channels11 and ionotropic glutamate receptors12,13. When in closer proximity, insects can taste water14. In the fruit fly, water taste and consumption is mediated by gustatory15 and higher order interoceptive subesophageal zone16 neurons and through the water taste receptor ppk28 17,18. Water homeostasis is further regulated in the hindgut through the osmolyte transporter inebriated, which promotes water absorption when animals are fed salty food19. Dehydration also enhances water taste sensitivity20 and promotes water sensing and drinking behaviour, and in thirsty animals water can be used as a reward for conditioned learning tasks21.

Multiple assay systems have been developed to evaluate water sensation or consumption in the fruit fly. Hygrosensation is tested by providing flies with a choice between humid and dry air, and unchallenged animals prefer a low humidity environment22. Water taste can be assessed using Ca2+ imaging or electrophysiology15,17,18,23, by measuring time spent drinking17 or water responses using the proboscis extension response15. However, these assays are labour-intensive or require specific skills and equipment. Actual water consumption has been measured by spiking water with food coloring20,21, however this technique requires animals to be dehydrated before testing and cannot be used to monitor water consumption over long periods. To address these issues, here we present a simple, high-throughput method for assessing Drosophila water consumption over time.

Results

To facilitate the cellular and molecular dissection of thirst, we developed a novel behavioural assay to assess water intake in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster. In laboratory conditions, flies obtain their water from food and ambient humidity. To monitor water intake, we separated fly food from water by dehydrating the food and added the water back in a separate receptacle filled with 200 μl of water. Flies are then added to the vial, with a parallel vial left empty as an evaporation control (Fig. 1a). Flies exposed to standard hydrated fly food did not consume the additional water, whereas flies housed in dehydrated food consumed water in the tube at a constant rate over time (Fig. 1b). Water consumption was confirmed by spiking water with blue dye, which could then be observed in the fly abdomen (Fig. 1c). We found fly number per vial had no effect on cumulative water consumption per fly in the range from 5 to 20 flies per vial (Fig. 1d). We observed a temperature-dependence in water consumption, with flies exposed to 25 °C drinking significantly more water than flies housed at 18 °C (Fig. 1e). This response was also dependent on humidity (Fig. 1f), with high humidity conditions (70%) suppressing drinking behaviour whereas lower humidity conditions (55%) promoted drinking behaviour.

Figure 1
figure 1

Water consumption assay. (a) Schematic diagram of assay. (b) Water consumption using non-dehydrated and dehydrated food. (c) To facilitate visualisation of consumed water, blue dye has been added to the water and can be seen in the abdomen of the fly. (d) The number of flies per chamber does not influence individual water consumption. Five, ten, fifteen, or twenty files were tested per assay for 3 days. (e) Temperature regulates water consumption in flies. The water consumption assay was tested in either 18 °C or 25 °C conditions for 3 days. (f) Desiccation stimulates water consumption in flies. The water consumption assay was tested in either low humidity (55%) or high humidity (70%) conditions for 3 days. All data represented mean ± S.E.M (n = 3–6). Student’s t-test, **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant.

We observed a sex-specific difference in drinking behaviour, with females consuming significantly more water than males per fly (Fig. 2a). However, there was no significant difference between sexes once drinking behaviour was normalised for body mass (Fig. 2b). Since variations in fly mass influences intake, we use µl/mg H2O consumption for subsequent measurements. Overall, we found steady state water consumption was 65% of total body weight per day. Homeostatic water consumption was not maximal, since inclusion of sodium chloride (NaCl) in the desiccated food could promote increased water consumption in a dose-dependent manner (Fig. 2c). Further, the strong response to salt levels suggests this assay system may also be suitable for rapid functional genetic dissection of the fly renal system24. Thus, we report a robust high-throughput assay system for monitoring water intake that can be used to study neurological and renal regulation of water homeostasis in Drosophila.

Figure 2
figure 2

(a) Daily water consumption in male and female flies without body weight normalisation. (b) Daily water consumption in male and female flies with body weight normalisation. (c) Salt stimulates water consumption in flies. All data represented mean ± S.E.M (n = 3–6). Student’s t-test, **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant.

Flies can taste water specifically, and this occurs via the ppk28 17,18. ppk28 was also required for water consumption in our system, with neural specific elav-Gal4 > ppk28 RNAi showing a drastic reduction in water consumption compared to elav-Gal4 > w 1118 control (Fig. 3a). ppk28 mutant flies also showed a drastic reduction in water consumption (Fig. 3b) indicating that ppk28-dependent water taste is also required for drinking behaviour. The ppk28-dependent defect was water-specific, since control and ppk28 mutant animals showed comparable food intake (Fig. 3c). Thus the ppk28 water taste receptor is also required for water consumption in this system.

Figure 3
figure 3

The water taste receptor ppk28 regulates water consumption. (a) Water consumption of control (elav-Gal4 > w 1118) and elav-Gal4 > ppk28 RNAi flies. (b) Water consumption and (c) food intake of w 1118 control and ppk28 mutant flies (ppk28 Δ). All data represented mean ± S.E.M (n = 3–6). Student’s t-test, **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant.

To apply this system to further explore the neural regulation of thirst, we subjected flies to control fed and hydrated conditions, hydrated starvation, dehydration, and dehydrating starvation conditions for 6 hours. mRNA was then collected from fly heads and RNA sequencing was performed (Supplementary Table 1). We found 477 transcripts up-regulated specifically by dehydration. By GO terms from the molecular function ontology, 53 of these were either transmembrane transporter activity or signal transducer activity (Supplementary Table 2, Supplementary Figs 2 and 3). We selected 20 of these candidates to test for a role in regulating water consumption behaviour using a pan-neuronal (elav-Gal4) RNAi strategy25. We observed no major neuronal role in regulating drinking behaviour for ALiX, brp, Ca-α1 T, msn, Ptp10D, rdgB, Shab, SK, slik, slo, or Stat92E (Supplementary Fig. 1), however we did confirm multiple new negative regulators of water consumption (Supplementary Fig. 2). These include the voltage gated calcium channel Ca-α1D, the cell adhesion molecule Dscam1, the voltage-gated potassium channel eag, the transmembrane tyrosine phosphatase Lar, the voltage-gated sodium channel para, and the signalling molecule Plc21C. We also identified multiple positive regulators of drinking behaviour, including a GPCR of unknown function called Calcium-independent receptor for α-latrotoxin (Cirl), a Rho guanyl-nucleotide exchange factor sif, and the fly dopamine receptor DopR1 (Fig. 4a, Supplementary Fig. 2m–p). Pharmacological studies have implicated the dopamine system in regulating thirst from dehydration in rats26,27, and more recently dopamine was implicated in water reward-based learning in flies21, so we investigated a role for DopR1 in water consumption further.

Figure 4
figure 4

The dopamine receptor DopR1 regulates water consumption. (a) RNA Seq reads of DopR1 transcripts obtained from the heads of dehydrated or control flies. (b) DopR1 mRNA levels were analysed by RT-qPCR. (c) Water consumption of w 1118 control and DopR1 mutant flies (dumb 2). (d) Food consumption of w 1118 control and DopR1 mutant flies (dumb 2) highlights a specific defect in water consumption. (e) Proboscis extension response (PER) of w 1118 control and DopR1 mutant flies to H2O and 5% sucrose solution (f) L-dopa (3 mg/ml) pre-treatment increases the PER response to water in w 1118 control but not DopR1 mutant flies. Sucrose responses are similar between treatments and genotypes indicating a water specific defect. For PER response, each independent trial consisted of ≥ 10 animals. (g) Water consumption of control (UAS-DopR1 RNAi/  +), elav-Gal4 > DopR1 RNAi and MB247-Gal4 > DopR1 RNAi flies. (h) Water consumption of control (w 1118), DopR1 mutant (elav-Gal4/ + ; dumb 2/dumb 2) and rescue (elav-Gal4 > UAS-DopR1; dumb 2/dumb 2 and MB247-Gal4 > UAS-DopR1; dumb 2/dumb 2). All data represented mean ± S.E.M (n = 3–6). Student’s t-test or one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s post hoc test, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant.

RNA-seq data showed that DopR1 was upregulated specifically by dehydration (Fig. 4a) and this was confirmed by qPCR (Fig. 4b). We next tested a DopR1 mutant (dumb 2), which showed a strong decrease in water intake compared to control (Fig. 4c). This phenotype was specific for water consumption, since food intake was normal in these animals (Fig. 4d). Further, while dehydrated control animals exhibit a strong proboscis extension response (PER) to water, dehydrated DopR1 mutants are much less sensitive (Fig. 4e). Both genotypes responded equally to a 5% sucrose solution indicating that DopR1 is acting to specifically to regulate thirst. To directly test a role for dopamine in regulation of thirst, we fed flies either control food or food spiked with L-dopa. While L-dopa treatment enhanced thirst behaviour in wild type animals, DopR1 mutant flies were resistant to this effect and again all animals showed comparable sucrose responses (Fig. 4f). We tested sensory neuron Gal4 lines previously implicated in water perception (ppk28-Gal4, Nanchung-Gal4 and two PoxN Gal4 lines) however, none of these neuron populations were required for water consumption (Supplementary Fig. 4). Thus, with the available tools, we cannot provide data for or against DopR1 in directly regulating water responsive sensory neurons. Since DopR1 has been shown to regulate water reward through actions in the mushroom body21, we tested a role for DopR1 in indirectly regulating water consumption via the mushroom body. DopR1 knockdown either pan-neuronally (elav-Gal4) or in the mushroom body (MB247-Gal4) reduced water intake to a similar extent as the whole body dumb 2 mutation (Fig. 4g) and transgenic re-introduction of DopR1 either across the nervous system, or specifically within the mushroom body, was sufficient to rescue water intake (Fig. 4h).

Since DopR1 was required for both drinking behaviour and water responsiveness by PER, we reasoned that DopR1 may also play a role in regulating water taste perception. To test this, we performed electrophysiological recordings from water responsive taste sensilla on the Drosophila labellum. Control w 1118 males show a clear response to water and sucrose (Fig. 5a). In contrast DopR1 mutant males showed a loss of water taste, and this phenotype was comparable to the water taste defect observed in the ppk28 mutant flies. By temporal analysis we observe that the loss in water taste response in both ppk28 and DopR1 mutant animals is most prominent within the first second of stimulation (Supplementary Fig. 3), and this difference in spike frequency was significant (Fig. 5b) while again sucrose responses remain largely intact (Fig. 5c and Supplementary Fig. 3). Pan-neuronal (elav-Gal4) or mushroom body-specific (MB247-Gal4) re-introduction of DopR1 also rescued the water taste response (Fig. 5a and b, Supplemtary Fig. 3) and both groups still exhibited intact responses to a sucrose solution. Thus, from an unbiased functional genome scan, we identify a role for DopR1 in modulating water taste perception and water intake. Together, we describe a simple high throughput method of measuring water consumption, a process that is regulated by taste, food salinity, renal function, and other yet determined physiological parameters in the fly.

Figure 5
figure 5

DopR1 is required for water taste. Extracellular bristle recordings of w 1118 control, ppk28 mutant (ppk28 Δ), DopR1 mutant (dumb 2 and elav-Gal4/ + ;;dumb 2/dumb 2) and rescue (elav-Gal4 > UAS-DopR1; dumb 2/dumb 2 and MB247-Gal4 > UAS-DopR1; dumb 2/dumb 2) flies after stimulation with water or 40 mM sucrose. (a) Representative spikes showing response to water (1 mM KCl) or 40 mM sucrose. (b,c) Mean total spike number for the first second of response to (b) water or (c) sucrose is shown. 3 to 5 L-type labellar bristles were recorded per animal. All data represent mean ± S.E.M (n = 9–17 animals). One-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s post hoc test, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant.

Discussion

In this study, we describe a simple water consumption assay system that when combined with the power of fruit fly genetics will be useful to rapidly dissect multiple aspects of animal physiology through measuring water intake. This technique is highly reproducible, can be set up with minimal investment, and can be used to monitor water intake over several days. Importantly, known regulators of water taste and memory were also required in our system, meaning this assay can be used to identify factors that regulate water consumption at multiple levels. By coupling this system with transcriptomics and functional genomics, we demonstrate utility of this assay for screening and discovery of new genes that regulate water intake. Finally, we confirm a previously described role for dopamine in regulating water consumption and also implicate the dopamine system in regulating water taste, however the precise mechanisms for this regulation remain to be elucidated.

There are several aspects of our study that differ from previous work on this subject. First, we have developed a simple high throughput system that allows accurate measurement of water intake for a variety of purposes. This assay directly measures volume consumed and thus differs from previous systems evaluating time spent consuming water17 or assessment of water reward in memory21. In particular, we observed a strong increase in water consumption in animals that had consumed salty food, suggesting that this simple system could be applied to screen for molecular mechanisms governing hemolymph composition, renal function, or other whole animal physiological processes where water concentration is a factor. Through a synaptic silencing approach, we found ppk28, nan and multiple PoxN-Gal4 expressing neurons were not necessary for water consumption behaviour (Supplementary Fig. 4). Highlighting that either there are additional yet unidentified water consumption neurons that are critical for water intake, or conversely that the function of known water taste neurons are not dependent on tetanus-sensitive synaptic output to mediate water intake.

We found both ppk28 and DopR1 were required for water intake in this system, and dehydration pre-treatment was not essential to observe water consumption differences between genotypes. This data is in accordance with previous work describing a role for ppk28 in controlling time spent drinking, although this previous study required 18–24 hours of dehydration before drinking behaviour could be evaluated17. Similarly, in a water seeking assay, Lin et al. found that minimum 6 hours of dehydration was required for flies to begin seeking water21 and both water seeking and drinking behaviour was found to be independent of ppk28 17 or DopR1 21, while water learning required both. This result differs in some regard to the data we present here, where both DopR1 and ppk28 were found to be critical for naïve water consumption. This difference could be attributed to the complexity of our system, which should identify animals with defects in hygrosensation, water taste, thirst, learning and memory and animals that cannot regulate hemolymph osmolarity. Regardless, we present a new, simple and high throughput assay system to evaluate water consumption and this assay compliments the range of existing techniques.

DopR1 has been implicated in water reward after dehydration, through actions on the γ-lobe of the mushroom body21. Our results here are in line with the overall findings by Lin et al. that DopR1 acts in the mushroom body to regulate water intake. Additionally, we found that DopR1, through actions in the mushroom body, also indirectly regulates water perception or taste. We have also shown that L-dopa treatment can enhance wild type water response but not in DopR1 mutants, suggesting that dopamine can play an immediate role in promoting water responses. However, more work is required before we can conclusively rule out a developmental role for DopR1 in the appropriate development of water neurons, and the temporal requirement for DopR1 remains to be determined. Interestingly, in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD), which involves a gradual decline in the dopamine system, a 40% decrease in daily water consumption has been reported28,29,30,31,32,33, and water intake inversely correlated with disease severity. Importantly, in PD patients decreased water intake precedes constipation or motor symptoms in some cases by decades28. Thus, dopamine may also regulate thirst and water taste in humans.

Together, we describe a simple high throughput assay system to investigate water consumption, and through testing this system we uncovered an unanticipated link between dopamine and the regulation of water taste, providing a potential molecular mechanism for why water palatability increases when an animal is thirsty.

Methods

Fly Strains

Drosophila melanogaster were grown on standard fly food containing sugar, yeast, molasses cornmeal, nipagin, agar and 0.04% propionic acid. All flies tested were 7 to 10-days-old males. w 1118 was used as wild type control. UAS-DopR1.RNAi (201154) and w 1118 were obtained from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center. MB247-Gal4, 50742; UAS-TeTxLC.TNT (UAS-TNT, 28838) and UAS-TeTxLC.IMP TNT (UAS-iTNT, 28841); UAS-ALiX.RNAi, 50904; UAS-brp.RNAi, 25891; UAS-Ca-α1D, 25830; UAS-Ca-α1T.RNAi, 39029; UAS-Cirl.RNAi, 34821; UAS-Dscam1.RNAi, 38945; UAS-eag.RNAi, 31679; UAS-Lar.RNAi, 34965; UAS-msn.RNAi, 28791; UAS-para.RNAi, 33923; UAS-Plc21C.RNAi, 32438; UAS-Ptp10D.RNAi, 39001; UAS-rdgB.RNAi, 28796; UAS-sif.RNAi, 25789; UAS-SK.RNAi, 27238; UAS-slik.RNAi, 35179; UAS-slo.RNAi, 55405; UAS-Stat92E.RNAi, 35600; were obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center. The ppk28-null mutant line (ppk28 Δ) was obtained from Kristin Scott’s lab17, DopR1 mutant line (dumb 2) was obtained from Bruno Van Swinderen’s lab34 and UAS-DopR1 transgene was obtained from Barry J. Dickson’s lab35. elav-Gal4 with UAS-Dicer2 on X chromosome was previous reported25.

Water consumption assays

To measure water consumption, flies were tested in vials containing dehydrated food with 200 μl distilled water provided in a small PCR tube (Eppendorf). To make dehydrated food, 0.4 ml of food was placed in a vial and then desiccated for 2 weeks. Each experiment included an identical vial without flies to measure the evaporation rate. Water consumption was calculated by measuring the weight difference of PCR tube after correcting for the evaporation rate. For blue dye experiments, food colouring blue dye (1:2000; Queen Fine Foods Pty. Ltd., QLD Australia) solution was added in the PCR tube. For NaCl experiments, 1–5% NaCl (Sigma-Aldrich) was dissolved in the fly food and then desiccated for 2 weeks. Feeding assays are similar to water consumption assays except that flies were tested in an empty vial with wet kimwipes and 200 μl 5% sucrose solution was supplied to flies in the PCR tube.

Proboscis extension response assays

For standard proboscis extension response (PER) assays, male flies were fed in vials and tested as described previously36, with slight modifications.

In brief, 10 experimental flies were glued to a glass slide to avoid their escape. The flies were starved at 19 °C and 70% humidity for 2 hours. To measure water and sugar response, each fly was tested with pure water or 5% sucrose (Sigma-Aldrich) stimuli two times. The percentage of PER was calculated as the sum of responses divided by the number of total tested flies (x 100%). For each genotype, 3 batches of 10 flies were tested on different days as independent triplicates. For L-dopa experiments, 3 mg/ml L-dopa precursor (Sigma-Aldrich) was dissolved in the fly food, and flies were maintained on this fly food for 2 days before PER assays. To measure water and sugar response, fixed flies were first starved for 1.5 hours (19 °C and 70% humidity).

RNA Extraction and RNA Sequencing

Total RNA was extracted from fly heads by homogenization and cell lysis using TRIzol (LifeTechnologies) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Further sample processing, library preparation and TruSeq RNA sequencing and analysis of the sample were performed by BGI (Shenzhen, China). Briefly, the total RNA samples were treated with DNase I to degrade any possible DNA contamination. mRNA was then enriched using oligo(dT) magnetic beads (for eukaryotes) and mixed with the fragmentation buffer and fragmented into short fragments (about 200 bp). The first strand of cDNA is synthesized by using a random hexamer-primer. Buffer, dNTPs, RNase H and DNA polymerase I are added to synthesize the second strand. The double strand cDNA is purified with magnetic beads. End reparation and 3′-end single nucleotide A (adenine) addition is then performed. Finally, sequencing adaptors are ligated to the fragments. The fragments are enriched by PCR amplification. During the QC step, Agilent 2100 Bioanaylzer and ABI StepOnePlus Real-Time PCR System are used to qualify and quantify of the sample library. The library products are ready for sequencing via Illumina HiSeqTM 2000 or other sequencer when necessary.

Reverse transcription quantitative real-time PCR (RT-qPCR)

Total RNA was extracted from fly heads by homogenization and cell lysis using TRIzol (LifeTechnologies) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Single-stranded cDNA was synthesized from 2 µg total RNA according to the manufacturer’s procedure (LifeTechnologies). The primers used for SYBR Green RT-qPCR were as follows: for DopR1, sense, 5′-ACG ATG GCA CAA CGT TGA CA-3′ and antisense, 5′-GCA CCG ATA GGA AGA TGC CA-3′; for 18 S, sense, 5′-TCT AGC AAT ATG AGA TTG AGC AAT AAG-3′ and antisense, 5′-AAT ACA CGT TGA TAC TTT CAT TGT AGC-3′. RT-qPCR was performed using the Applied Biosystems 7900HT Real-Time PCR System. Relative quantification of mRNA levels was performed using the comparative Cq method (∆∆Cq method) with 18 S as the reference gene.

Screening of differentially expressed genes (DEGs)

The p value was used to detect the difference in gene expression in two different samples. p value corresponds to differential gene expression test. False Discovery Rate (FDR) is a method to determine the threshold of p-value in multiple tests37. p value < 0.0001 and FDR ≤ 0.001 was used as the threshold to judge the significance of gene expression difference.

Electrophysical recording

The electrophysiological responses of labellar taste neurons were recorded by using the tip recording method, similar to those described previously38,39,40. Briefly, all recordings were performed on L-type labellar bristles of 7–10 day-old male flies. 3–5 individual L-type bristles were recorded on each fly. The recording electrode (tip diameter, 10–12μm) was filled with 1 mM KCl for water sensing response or 40 mM sucrose in 30 mM tri-choline chloride (TCC; Sigma-Aldrich, as an electrolyte) for sucrose sensing response. Each bristle was first recorded for water response and then healthiness confirmed by checking response to sucrose. Signals were acquired using an AxonClamp 900 A amplifier and digitized with a 1400 A D-A converter (Molecular Devices) at a sampling rate of 10 kHz (filtered at 3 kHz). Electric signals were further amplified and filtered by a second amplifier (CyberAmp 320, Axon Instrument, Inc., USA, gain × 100, eighth order Bessel pass-band filter 1600Hz).

Data was analyzed using the Clampfit 10 software (Molecular Devices). Spikes between 0 and 2 s after initiation of stimuli were counted as firing frequency evoked by the tastant. The mean value of spikes was calculated on 3–5 bristles recorded on each fly as one statistical sample. The mean ± SEM in figures and text were based on number of flies.

Data analysis

All the experiments were performed at least three times. All values are expressed as mean ± SEM. Data was analyzed by Student’s t-test or one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s post hoc test using GraphPad Prism 6 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA). p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.