Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Memory-enhancing properties of sleep depend on the oscillatory amplitude of norepinephrine

Matters Arising to this article was published on 20 April 2023

Abstract

Sleep has a complex micro-architecture, encompassing micro-arousals, sleep spindles and transitions between sleep stages. Fragmented sleep impairs memory consolidation, whereas spindle-rich and delta-rich non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep promote it. However, the relationship between micro-arousals and memory-promoting aspects of sleep remains unclear. In this study, we used fiber photometry in mice to examine how release of the arousal mediator norepinephrine (NE) shapes sleep micro-architecture. Here we show that micro-arousals are generated in a periodic pattern during NREM sleep, riding on the peak of locus-coeruleus-generated infraslow oscillations of extracellular NE, whereas descending phases of NE oscillations drive spindles. The amplitude of NE oscillations is crucial for shaping sleep micro-architecture related to memory performance: prolonged descent of NE promotes spindle-enriched intermediate state and REM sleep but also associates with awakenings, whereas shorter NE descents uphold NREM sleep and micro-arousals. Thus, the NE oscillatory amplitude may be a target for improving sleep in sleep disorders.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Increased NE descent amplitude primes awakenings over micro-arousals.
Fig. 2: Prolonged NE descent promotes spindle-rich IS and REM sleep transitions.
Fig. 3: Optogenetic suppression of LC induces IS-REM sleep sequences and improves memory.
Fig. 4: Optogenetic reduction of NE amplitude reduces sigma activity and disrupts memory.
Fig. 5: Pharmacological reduction of NE amplitude promotes micro-arousal and compromises memory.
Fig. 6: Model diagram: NE amplitude defines the memory restorative properties of sleep.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed in this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

The custom-made MATLAB code used in this study is available from GitHub: https://github.com/MieAndersen/NE-oscillations.

References

  1. Andrillon, T. et al. Sleep spindles in humans: insights from intracranial EEG and unit recordings. J. Neurosci. 31, 17821–17834 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Fernandez, L. M. J. & Lüthi, A. Sleep spindles: mechanisms and functions. Physiol. Rev. 100, 805–868 (2020).

  3. Bonnet, M. H. & Arand, D. L. EEG arousal norms by age. J. Clin. Sleep. Med. 3, 271–274 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Boselli, M., Parrino, L., Smerieri, A. & Terzano, M. G. Effect of age on EEG arousals in normal sleep. Sleep 21, 351–357 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bonnet, M. H. Effect of sleep disruption on sleep, performance, and mood. Sleep 8, 11–19 (1985).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Rauchs, G. et al. Is there a link between sleep changes and memory in Alzheimer’s disease? Neuroreport 19, 1159–1162 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Landolt, H. P., Dijk, D. J., Achermann, P. & Borbély, A. A. Effect of age on the sleep EEG: slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in young and middle-aged men. Brain Res. 738, 205–212 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Soltani, S. et al. Sleep–wake cycle in young and older mice. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 13, 51 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Saper, C. B., Fuller, P. M., Pedersen, N. P., Lu, J. & Scammell, T. E. Sleep state switching. Neuron 68, 1023–1042 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Constantinople, C. M. & Bruno, R. M. Effects and mechanisms of wakefulness on local cortical networks. Neuron 69, 1061–1068 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Aston-Jones, G. & Bloom, F. E. Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep–waking cycle. J. Neurosci. 1, 876–886 (1981).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Hobson, J. A., McCarley, R. W. & Wyzinski, P. W. Sleep cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two brainstem neuronal groups. Science 189, 55–58 (1975).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Eschenko, O. & Sara, S. J. Learning-dependent, transient increase of activity in noradrenergic neurons of locus coeruleus during slow wave sleep in the rat: brain stem–cortex interplay for memory consolidation? Cereb. Cortex 18, 2596–2603 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Eschenko, O., Magri, C., Panzeri, S. & Sara, S. J. Noradrenergic neurons of the locus coeruleus are phase locked to cortical up–down states during sleep. Cereb. Cortex 22, 426–435 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Carter, M. E. et al. Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1526–1533 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Rasmussen, K., Morilak, D. A. & Jacobs, B. L. Single unit activity of locus coeruleus neurons in the freely moving cat. I. During naturalistic behaviors and in response to simple and complex stimuli. Brain Res. 371, 324–334 (1986).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Foote, S. L., Aston-Jones, G. & Bloom, F. E. Impulse activity of locus coeruleus neurons in awake rats and monkeys is a function of sensory stimulation and arousal. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 77, 3033–3037 (1980).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Shouse, M. N., Staba, R. J., Saquib, S. F. & Farber, P. R. Monoamines and sleep: microdialysis findings in pons and amygdala. Brain Res. 860, 181–189 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Léna, I. et al. Variations in extracellular levels of dopamine, noradrenaline, glutamate, and aspartate across the sleep–wake cycle in the medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. J. Neurosci. Res. 81, 891–899 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Yamaguchi, H., Hopf, F. W., Li, S.-B. & Lecea, Lde In vivo cell type-specific CRISPR knockdown of dopamine beta hydroxylase reduces locus coeruleus evoked wakefulness. Nat. Commun. 9, 5211 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Rajkowski, J., Kubiak, P. & Aston-Jones, G. Locus coeruleus activity in monkey: phasic and tonic changes are associated with altered vigilance. Brain Res. Bull. 35, 607–616 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Swift, K. M. et al. Abnormal locus coeruleus sleep activity alters sleep signatures of memory consolidation and impairs place cell stability and spatial memory. Curr. Biol. 28, 3599–3609 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Lecci, S. et al. Coordinated infraslow neural and cardiac oscillations mark fragility and offline periods in mammalian sleep. Sci. Adv. 3, e1602026 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Osorio-Forero, A. et al. Noradrenergic circuit control of non-REM sleep substates. Curr. Biol. 31, 5009–5023 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Novitskaya, Y., Sara, S. J., Logothetis, N. K. & Eschenko, O. Ripple-triggered stimulation of the locus coeruleus during post-learning sleep disrupts ripple/spindle coupling and impairs memory consolidation. Learn. Mem. 23, 238–248 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Feng, J. et al. A genetically encoded fluorescent sensor for rapid and specific in vivo detection of norepinephrine. Neuron 102, 745–761 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Osorio-Forero, A. et al. Infraslow locus coeruleus activity coordinates spindle rhythms and heart rate to gate fluctuating non-REM sleep substates. Preprint at https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.03.08.434399v1 (2021).

  28. EEG arousals: scoring rules and examples: a preliminary report from the sleep disorders atlas task force of the american sleep disorders association. Sleep 15, 173–184 (1992).

  29. Gottesmann, C. The transition from slow-wave sleep to paradoxical sleep: evolving facts and concepts of the neurophysiological processes underlying the intermediate stage of sleep. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 20, 367–387 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Krueger, J. N. et al. Amnesia for context fear is caused by widespread disruption of hippocampal activity. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 175, 107295 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Takeuchi, T. et al. Locus coeruleus and dopaminergic consolidation of everyday memory. Nature 537, 357–362 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Mahn, M., Prigge, M., Ron, S., Levy, R. & Yizhar, O. Biophysical constraints of optogenetic inhibition at presynaptic terminals. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 554 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. de Medeiros, M. A., Reis, L. C. & Mello, L. E. Stress-induced c-Fos expression is differentially modulated by dexamethasone, diazepam and imipramine. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 1246–1256 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Mayers, A. G. & Baldwin, D. S. Antidepressants and their effect on sleep. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 20, 533–559 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mitchell, H. A. & Weinshenker, D. Good night and good luck: norepinephrine in sleep pharmacology. Biochem. Pharmacol. 79, 801–809 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Watts, A., Gritton, H. J., Sweigart, J. & Poe, G. R. Antidepressant suppression of non-REM sleep spindles and REM sleep impairs hippocampus-dependent learning while augmenting striatum-dependent learning. J. Neurosci. 32, 13411–13420 (2012).

  37. Porter-Stransky, K. A. et al. Noradrenergic transmission at alpha1-adrenergic receptors in the ventral periaqueductal gray modulates arousal. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 237–247 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Rudzik, F. et al. Sleep spindle characteristics and arousability from nighttime transportation noise exposure in healthy young and older individuals. Sleep 41, zsy077 (2018).

  39. Peter-Derex, L., Yammine, P., Bastuji, H. & Croisile, B. Sleep and Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep. Med. Rev. 19, 29–38 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Scammell, T. E., Arrigoni, E. & Lipton, J. O. Neural circuitry of wakefulness and sleep. Neuron 93, 747 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Luppi, P. H., Aston-Jones, G., Akaoka, H., Chouvet, G. & Jouvet, M. Afferent projections to the rat locus coeruleus demonstrated by retrograde and anterograde tracing with cholera-toxin B subunit and Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. Neuroscience 65, 119–160 (1995).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Aston-Jones, G., Ennis, M., Pieribone, V. A., Nickell, W. T. & Shipley, M. T. The brain nucleus locus coeruleus: restricted afferent control of a broad efferent network. Science 234, 734–737 (1986).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Picchioni, D. et al. Autonomic arousals contribute to brain fluid pulsations during sleep. Neuroimage 249, 118888 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Fultz, N. E. et al. Coupled electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and cerebrospinal fluid oscillations in human sleep. Science 366, 628–631 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Purcell, S. M. et al. Characterizing sleep spindles in 11,630 individuals from the National Sleep Research Resource. Nat. Commun. 8, 15930 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Dringenberg, H. C. & Olmstead, M. C. Integrated contributions of basal forebrain and thalamus to neocortical activation elicited by pedunculopontine tegmental stimulation in urethane-anesthetized rats. Neuroscience 119, 839–853 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Gervasoni, D. et al. Effect of chronic treatment with milnacipran on sleep architecture in rats compared with paroxetine and imipramine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 73, 557–563 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Jones, B. E. The role of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons and neighboring cholinergic neurons of the pontomesencephalic tegmentum in sleep–wake states. Prog. Brain Res. 88, 533–543 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Williams, J. A. & Reiner, P. B. Noradrenaline hyperpolarizes identified rat mesopontine cholinergic neurons in vitro. J. Neurosci. 13, 3878–3883 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Latchoumane, C.-F. V., Ngo, H.-V. V., Born, J. & Shin, H.-S. Thalamic spindles promote memory formation during sleep through triple phase-locking of cortical, thalamic, and hippocampal rhythms. Neuron 95, 424–435 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Li, W., Ma, L., Yang, G. & Gan, W.-B. REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 427–437 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Ribeiro, S. et al. Novel experience induces persistent sleep-dependent plasticity in the cortex but not in the hippocampus. Front. Neurosci. 1, 43–55 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Giuditta, A. et al. The sequential hypothesis of the function of sleep. Behav. Brain Res. 69, 157–166 (1995).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Gais, S., Rasch, B., Dahmen, J. C., Sara, S. & Born, J. The memory function of noradrenergic activity in non-REM sleep. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2582–2592 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Rasch, B., Pommer, J., Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. Pharmacological REM sleep suppression paradoxically improves rather than impairs skill memory. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 396–397 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Aghajanian, G. K. & VanderMaelen, C. P. α2-adrenoceptor-mediated hyperpolarization of locus coeruleus neurons: intracellular studies in vivo. Science 215, 1394–1396 (1982).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Washburn, M. & Moises, H. C. Electrophysiological correlates of presynaptic alpha 2-receptor-mediated inhibition of norepinephrine release at locus coeruleus synapses in dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 9, 2131–2140 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Hayat, H. et al. Locus coeruleus norepinephrine activity mediates sensory-evoked awakenings from sleep. Sci. Adv. 6, eaaz4232 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Oe, Y. et al. Distinct temporal integration of noradrenaline signaling by astrocytic second messengers during vigilance. Nat. Commun. 11, 471 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Funding: Lundbeck Foundation, R386–2021–165; Independent Research Council Denmark, 7016–00324A; Augustinus Foundation, 16–3735.; Novo Nordisk Foundation, NNF20OC0066419; US Department of Health & Human Services, National Institutes of Health (NIH), R01AT011439; US Department of Defense, Army Research Office, W911NF1910280; The Simons Foundation, 811237; Adelson Foundation; National Key R&D Program of China, 2021YFF0502904; National Natural Science Foundation of China, 31925017 and 31871087; NIH BRAIN Initiative, 1U01NS113358 and 1U01NS120824; and the FENG Foundation. We thank Myles Billard for technical support and Dan Xue for graphical assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization: C.K., M.A., H.H., M.N. and P.W. Fiber photometry and EEG methodology and investigation: C.K., M.A., N.H., C.D. and V.U. Viral constructs: Y.L., J.F. and F.D. HPLC methodology: P.W. Analysis: C.K., M.A. and B.S. Visualization: C.K. and M.A. Supervision: H.H. and M.N. Writing—original draft: C.K. and M.A. Writing—review and editing: C.K., M.A., H.H. and M.N.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Celia Kjaerby or Maiken Nedergaard.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Neuroscience thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 All ascending stages of NE oscillations are associated with EEG-defined micro-arousals or awakenings.

a, Mean power traces for delta, theta, sigma and beta frequency bands aligned to NE rise associated with EEG/EMG-based transitions from NREM sleep to continued NREM sleep (blue, MANE), micro-arousals (MAEEG/EMG, orange) and wake (grey, wakeEEG/EMG). b, Summary plot showing the reduction in band power across the different transitions. There was no difference between MANE and MAEEG/EMG for theta, sigma and beta band power, which are the frequencies used to assess micro-arousals. Significance was calculated by means of two-way repeated measures ANOVA with Šídák’s post hoc test (only MANE and MAEEG/EMG post hoc comparisons shown in graph for simplicity, P = 0.0003, delta; P = 0.13, theta; P = 0.11, sigma; P = 0.65, beta). c, Slope of linear regression on 5 initial seconds of NE rise was used as estimate for rise time. d, NE slope across the different type of transitions (repeated measures one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s multiple comparisons test, P = 0.0059, MANE vs wake; P = 0.041, MAEEG/EMG vs wake). e, Multi-taper power spectral analysis showed increased power for slower frequencies for NREM sleep compared to wake and REM sleep with no defined peak frequency likely due to the discrete nature of NE oscillations. n = 7. Data is shown as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 2 Decrease in sigma power marks tone-evoked arousal.

Based on the local decline in sigma power in response to tone (> 0.1 log(μV2)), the tone outcome was divided into arousal and otherwise maintained sleep. a, Mean sigma power traces in response to tone leading to either arousal or persistent sleep. b, The maximum value of sigma power maximum prior to tone (‘pre’) and the minimum sigma power after the tone (‘post’) for the defined arousal and sleep conditions. c, The difference in sigma power occurring in response to the tone. Significance was calculated by means of two-way repeated measures ANOVA with Šídák’s post hoc test (b, P = 0.035, pre; P = 0.023, post) or two-tailed paired t-test (c, P = 0.0034). n = 6. Data is shown as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 3 Relationship between norepinephrine, sigma power, spindles and delta power.

a, Representative trace showing sigma power of surface EEG recordings with corresponding detection of spindles based on S1 cortical LFP recordings. b, Correlation between spindle occurrences and mean sigma power across 10 s bins over a 5 min NREM sleep episode. c, Mean Pearson r values (0.63 ± 0.02). d, Mean sigma power traces normalized to baseline showing the amount of sigma power increase associated with NE descents preceeding microarousals (MANE or MAEEG/EMG) or awakenings (wakeEEG/EMG). Calculated area under the curve (AUC) for sigma power is largest during the NE descents associated with awakenings transitions (two-tailed paired t-test; P = 0.60, MANE vs MAEEG/EMG; P = 0.0057, MANE vs wake; P = 0.044, MAEEG/EMG vs wake). e, Mean correlation coefficient between delta power and NE level (5 min NREM sleep episodes). f, Three different example traces showing how optogenetic activation of locus coeruleus (LC, 2 s 20 Hz 10 ms pulses) during periods of NE descend leads to NE ascend followed by a delayed change in spindle occurrences and amplitude reduction of 7–15 bandpass filtered EEG that is not represented by sigma power (window = 5 s, overlap: 2.5 s). n = 4 (c); n = 7 (d, e). Data is shown as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 4 Optogenetic suppression of LC.

a, Arch expression in LC was verified by co-staining for TH and GFP. Arrowheads indicate the tip location of the optic fiber (scale bar = 800 µm). b, Close up of a (scale bar = 400 µm). c, Example traces showing norepinephrine level, sigma power, sleep spindles, bandpass LFP in sigma range, and EMG raw data. d, Mean sigma power aligned to onset of NE drop (NREM and IS-REM) or onset of laser stimulation (Arch). e, Time spent in sleep/wake stages during 2-h recording between memory encoding and recall (2-way repeated measures ANOVA with Šídák’s multiple comparison post hoc test). f, Number of laser stimulation (% of total number) in each sleep/wake stage (2-way repeated measures ANOVA with Šídák’s multiple comparison post hoc test). g, Mean NE trace at laser onset during wakefulness. h, NE descent amplitude induced by laser onset during NREM and wakefulness (2-way repeated measures ANOVA with Šídák’s multiple comparison post hoc test, P = 0.0046, NRREM). i, Percentage of laser stimulations during wakefulness resulting in transition to NREM sleep (unpaired t-test). j, Distance moved and ratio between approaches during the recall phase of the novel object recognition (NOR) (two-tailed unpaired t-test, P = 0.049, one-sample t-test P = 0.49, YFP; P = 0.026, Arch). k, Distance moved and object approaches during the acquisition phase of the NOR (unpaired t-test). l, Linear regression between object exploration ratio and time spent in REM sleep. m, Linear regression between object exploration ratio and number of REM bouts/h. n, Linear regression between object exploration ratio and mean theta power during REM sleep. o, Linear regression between object exploration ratio and time spent in NREM sleep. p, Linear regression between object exploration ratio and theta amplitude in response to laser. q, Linear regression between object exploration ratio and delta amplitude in response to laser. n = 9 Arch, 5 YFP. Data is shown as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 5 Optogenetic activation of LC.

a, Distance moved and ratio between novel and familiar object approaches during recall phase of novel object recognition (ratio: two-tailed unpaired t-test, P = 0.052; one-sample t-test, P = 0.035, YFP; P = 0.64, ChR2). b, Distance moved and object exploration during memory acquisition (right). n = 7 ChR2, 7 YFP. Data is shown as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 6 The effect of NE reuptake inhibition on EEG and arousability.

a, Representative traces of norepinephrine, raw EEG and EMG before and after administration of the NE reuptake inhibitor, desipramine (des, 10 mg/kg). b, Example EEG and EMG traces from NREM sleep prior to desipramine administration (top), during undefinable phases following desipramine administration (middle) and from defined NREM sleep following administration with desipramine (bottom). c, Sigma power traces aligned to the onset of awakenings (wakeEEG/EMG) following either treatment with saline or desipramine. d, Mean sigma power before and after NREM-wakeEEG/EMG transition (2-way repeated measures ANOVA with Šídák’s multiple comparison post hoc test, P = 0.0024, pre; P = 0.031, post). e, Reduction in sigma power amplitude (two-tailed paired t-test, P = 0.0007). f, Distance moved and ratio between novel and familiar object approaches during the recall phase of the novel object recognition (NOR) as well as distance moved and number of object approaches during the acquisition phase (unpaired t-test and one-sample t-test). n = 7 (ce), n = 7 des, 9 sal (f). Data is shown as mean ± SEM. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Source data

Supplementary information

Source data

Source Data Fig. 1

Statistical source data

Source Data Fig. 2

Statistical source data

Source Data Fig. 3

Statistical source data

Source Data Fig. 4

Statistical source data

Source Data Fig. 5

Statistical source data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 1

Statistical source data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 2

Statistical source data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 3

Statistical source data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 4

Statistical source data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 5

Statistical source data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 6

Statistical source data

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kjaerby, C., Andersen, M., Hauglund, N. et al. Memory-enhancing properties of sleep depend on the oscillatory amplitude of norepinephrine. Nat Neurosci 25, 1059–1070 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01102-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01102-9

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing