Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Uncertainty about the impact of social decisions increases prosocial behaviour

Subjects

Abstract

Uncertainty about how our choices will affect others infuses social life. Past research suggests uncertainty has a negative effect on prosocial behaviour1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12 by enabling people to adopt self-serving narratives about their actions1,13. We show that uncertainty does not always promote selfishness. We introduce a distinction between two types of uncertainty that have opposite effects on prosocial behaviour. Previous work focused on outcome uncertainty (uncertainty about whether or not a decision will lead to a particular outcome). However, as soon as people’s decisions might have negative consequences for others, there is also impact uncertainty (uncertainty about how others’ well-being will be impacted by the negative outcome). Consistent with past research1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12, we found decreased prosocial behaviour under outcome uncertainty. In contrast, prosocial behaviour was increased under impact uncertainty in incentivized economic decisions and hypothetical decisions about infectious disease threats. Perceptions of social norms paralleled the behavioural effects. The effect of impact uncertainty on prosocial behaviour did not depend on the individuation of others or the mere mention of harm, and was stronger when impact uncertainty was made more salient. Our findings offer insights into communicating uncertainty, especially in contexts where prosocial behaviour is paramount, such as responding to infectious disease threats.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: A decision might cause a potentially negative outcome for another person, and the negative outcome may have a negligible or large impact on their well-being.
Fig. 2: Depiction of uncertainty manipulations across experimental conditions.
Fig. 3: Comparison of prosocial decision frequencies for the two game types and four receiver information conditions.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Dana, J., Weber, R. A. & Kuang, J. X. Exploiting moral wiggle room: experiments demonstrating an illusory preference for fairness. Econ. Theory 33, 67–80 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Exley, C. L. Excusing selfishness in charitable giving: the role of risk. Rev. Econ. Stud. 83, 587–628 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Larson, T. & Capra, C. M. Exploiting moral wiggle room: illusory preference for fairness? A comment. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 4, 467–474 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Hamman, J. R., Loewenstein, G. & Weber, R. A. Self-interest through delegation: an additional rationale for the principal-agent relationship. Am. Econ. Rev. 100, 1826–1846 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Brock, J. M., Lange, A. & Ozbay, E. Y. Dictating the risk: experimental evidence on giving in risky environments. Am. Econ. Rev. 103, 415–437 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Feiler, L. Testing models of information avoidance with binary choice dictator games. J. Econ. Psychol. 45, 253–267 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Güth, W., Levati, M. V. & Ploner, M. On the social dimension of time and risk preferences: an experimental study. Econ. Inq. 46, 261–272 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Brennan, G., González, L. G., Güth, W. & Levati, M. V. Attitudes toward private and collective risk in individual and strategic choice situations. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 67, 253–262 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Andreoni, J. & Bernheim, B. D. Social image and the 50–50 norm: a theoretical and experimental analysis of audience effects. Econometrica 77, 1607–1636 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Budescu, D. V., Rapoport, A. & Suleiman, R. Resource dilemmas with environmental uncertainty and asymmetric players. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 20, 475–487 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Gustafsson, M., Biel, A. & Gärling, T. Overharvesting of resources of unknown size. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 103, 47–64 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. De Kwaadsteniet, E. W., van Dijk, E., Wit, A., De Cremer, D. & de Rooij, M. Justifying decisions in social dilemmas: justification pressures and tacit coordination under environmental uncertainty. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 1648–1660 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Shalvi, S., Gino, F., Barkan, R. & Ayal, S. Self-serving justifications: doing wrong and feeling moral. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 125–130 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Nagel, T. What is it like to be a bat? Phil. Rev. 83, 435–450 (1974).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Harsanyi, J. C. Cardinal welfare, individualistic ethics, and interpersonal comparisons of utility. J. Polit. Econ. 63, 309–321 (1955).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hammond, P. J. in Interpersonal Comparisons of Well-Being (eds Elster, J. & Roemer, J. E.) 200–254 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1991).

  17. Falk, A. & Tirole, J. Narratives, Imperatives and Moral Reasoning Working Paper (mimeo Toulouse School of Economics, 2016); https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/8a0f/87ad25a8a51c7637b32b4e2fc5080f3963c4.pdf

  18. Mazar, N., Amir, O. & Ariely, D. The dishonesty of honest people: a theory of self-concept maintenance. J. Mark. Res. 45, 633–644 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Haisley, E. C. & Weber, R. A. Self-serving interpretations of ambiguity in other-regarding behavior. Games Econ. Behav. 68, 614–625 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Van Dijk, E., Wit, A., Wilke, H. & Budescu, D. V. in Contemporary Psychological Research on Social Dilemmas (eds Suleiman, R., Budescu, D. V., Fischer, I. & Messick, D. M.) 315–331 (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, NY, 2004).

  21. Krupka, E. L. & Weber, R. A. Identifying social norms using coordination games: why does dictator game sharing vary? J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 11, 495–524 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Platt, M. L. & Huettel, S. A. Risky business: the neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 398–403 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Buckholtz, J. W. et al. From blame to punishment: disrupting prefrontal cortex activity reveals norm enforcement mechanisms. Neuron 87, 1369–1380 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Savage, L. J. The Foundations of Statistics (Dover Publications, New York, NY, 1972).

  25. Davis, H. L., Hoch, S. J. & Ragsdale, E. K. E. An anchoring and adjustment model of spousal predictions. J. Consum. Res. 13, 25–37 (1986).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Greenstein, M. J. & Xu, X. My mug is valuable, but my partner’s is even more so: economic decisions for close others. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 174–187 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hsee, C. K. & Weber, E. U. A fundamental prediction error: self–others discrepancies in risk preference. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 126, 45–53 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Ziegler, F. V. & Tunney, R. J. Who’s been framed? Framing effects are reduced in financial gambles made for others. BMC Psychol. 3, 9 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Andersson, O., Holm, H. J., Tyran, J.-R. & Wengström, E. Risking Other People’s Money: Experimental Evidence on Bonus Schemes, Competition, and Altruism IFN Working Paper 989 (IFN, 2013).

  30. Beisswanger, A. H., Stone, E. R., Hupp, J. M. & Allgaier, L. Risk taking in relationships: differences in deciding for oneself versus for a friend. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25, 121–135 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., Sarr, B., Fagerlin, A. & Ubel, P. A. A matter of perspective: choosing for others differs from choosing for yourself in making treatment decisions. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 21, 618–622 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Green, J. D., Burnette, J. L. & Davis, J. L. Third-party forgiveness: (not) forgiving your close other’s betrayer. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 407–418 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Shalowitz, D. I., Garrett-Mayer, E. & Wendler, D. The accuracy of surrogate decision makers: a systematic review. Arch. Intern. Med. 166, 493–497 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kelly, B., Rid, A. & Wendler, D. Systematic review: individuals’ goals for surrogate decision-making. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 884–895 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Majesko, A., Hong, S. Y., Weissfeld, L. & White, D. B. Identifying family members who may struggle in the role of surrogate decision maker. Crit. Care Med. 40, 2281–2286 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Crockett, M. J., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Siegel, J. Z., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 17320–17325 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Engel, C. Dictator games: a meta study. Exp. Econ. 14, 583–610 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Bartling, B. & Fischbacher, U. Shifting the blame: on delegation and responsibility. Rev. Econ. Stud. 79, 67–87 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Aguiar, F., Brañas-Garza, P. & Miller, L. M. Moral distance in dictator games. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 3, 344–354 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  40. Brañas-Garza, P. Poverty in dictator games: awakening solidarity. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 60, 306–320 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Fong, C. & Oberholzer-Gee, F. Truth in giving: experimental evidence on the welfare effects of informed giving to the poor. J. Public Econ. 95, 436–444 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Reniers, R. L. E. P., Corcoran, R., Drake, R., Shryane, N. M. & Völlm, B. A. The QCAE: a Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy. J. Pers. Assess. 93, 84–95 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Grossmann, I., Brienza, J. P. & Bobocel, D. R. Wise deliberation sustains cooperation. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0061 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Kogut, T. & Ritov, I. The “identified victim” effect: an identified group, or just a single individual? J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 18, 157–167 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Small, D. A. & Loewenstein, G. Helping a victim or helping the victim: altruism and identifiability. J. Risk Uncertain. 26, 5–16 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G. & Slovic, P. Sympathy and callousness: the impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 102, 143–153 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Ecker, U. K. H., Hogan, J. L. & Lewandowsky, S. Reminders and repetition of misinformation: helping or hindering its retraction? J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 6, 185–192 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Putnam, A. L., Wahlheim, C. N. & Jacoby, L. L. Memory for flip-flopping: detection and recollection of political contradictions. Mem. Cogn. 42, 1198–1210 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Brummernhenrich, B. & Bromme, R. Dealing with uncertainty: readers’ memory for and use of conflicting information from science texts as function of presentation format and source expertise. Cogn. Instr. 31, 130–150 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Carlyle, R. C. What are the chances of a devastating pandemic occurring in the next 50 years? Huffington Post https://www.huffingtonpost.com/quora/what-are-the-chances-of-a_b_3839785.html?guccounter=1 (2013).

  51. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. & Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T. & Gosling, S. D. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 3–5 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Paolacci, G., Chandler, J. & Ipeirotis, P. G. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 5, 411–419 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  54. Phillips, J. & Cushman, F. Morality constrains the default representation of what is possible. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 4649–4654 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a Wellcome Trust Institutional Strategic Support Fund grant (204826/Z/16/Z) awarded to J.S., M.J.C., N.S.F. and G.K., a John Fell Fund award to M.J.C., and by the Oxford Martin Programme for Collective Responsibility for Infectious Disease. The work of J.S. for this paper was funded by a Wellcome Trust grant (104848/Z/14/Z). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The authors thank W. Sinnott-Armstrong, D. Batson, and members of the Crockett and MAD laboratories for helpful feedback.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

A.K. and M.J.C. developed the research concept. A.K., M.J.C. and A.-M.N. designed the studies. Testing and data collection were performed by A.K. and A.-M.N. A.K. performed the data analysis and interpretation in collaboration with A.-M.N. and M.J.C. A.K., A.-M.N. and M.J.C. drafted the manuscript and all other authors provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Andreas Kappes or Molly J. Crockett.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information

Supplementary Methods, Supplementary Note, Supplementary Figure 1, Supplementary References 1−9

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kappes, A., Nussberger, AM., Faber, N.S. et al. Uncertainty about the impact of social decisions increases prosocial behaviour. Nat Hum Behav 2, 573–580 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0372-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0372-x

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing