Introduction

The presence of oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) in young people confers the risk for a wide range of future psychopathology in later adolescence and adulthood [1]. While links to later conduct disorder (CD) are well established, ODD also predicts anxiety and depression. These associations are particularly remarkable as they have been shown to exist independently from comorbid CD, with CD conferring no additional risk independently from ODD. Studies of ODD symptoms have provided evidence for distinct dimensions of irritability versus defiant behavior [1,2,3,4,5,6], as reflected in revisions to DSM 5 [7] and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) [8]. Irritability robustly predicts later depression and anxiety [1, 5, 6, 9, 10], but not later attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [11], CD [12], substance use [6], bipolar disorder symptoms [11], or borderline personality disorder [13, 14]. Furthermore, irritability can be validly measured early in childhood [15, 16], and intergenerational links have been observed between preschool irritability and parental history of depression, suicidality and anxiety [15, 16], but not parental antisocial behavior or substance use [15, 16]. The convergent and discriminant validity of irritability as a meaningfully distinct dimension from defiant behavior is thus supported both over individuals’ developmental lifespans and inter-generationally.

Several studies demonstrate moderate genetic influences on irritability, with heritability estimates ranging from 31 to 54% across studies in the UK [17], Sweden [18, 19], and USA [20]. Moderate heritability estimates for defiant behavior, ranging from 41 to 45%, also have been reported in these studies, with moderate shared environmental influences as well. In the UK sample, the genetic correlation of irritability was stronger with depressed mood (rA = .70) than with delinquency (rA = .57), whereas the genetic correlation of defiant behavior was stronger with delinquency (rA = .80) than with depressed mood (rA = .46). In the US sample, irritability at age 11 shared genetic influences with internalizing symptomatology at age 16, whereas defiant behavior at age 11 shared genetic influences with later substance use disorder symptoms. The longitudinal Swedish study [18, 19] found that the covariation of irritability with internalizing symptoms accounted for by genetic influences ranged from 56 to 74% across waves. In order to more fully describe the specificity of the genetic contribution to the phenotypic associations between irritability and internalizing disorders and between defiant behavior and externalizing behavioral disorders, it is necessary to account for common genetic factors shared between the specific ODD dimensions and other psychiatric disorders. Evidence for a genetic pathway linking childhood irritability with later depression or anxiety would have profound implications for the early identification of affective disorder risk, given the potential to identify irritability during preschool.

The present study

Our previous work demonstrated that a bifactor structure including both a general ODD factor and specific irritability and defiant behavior factors provided the best fit to ODD symptoms in five large datasets [1], including the Georgia Twin Study (GTS), the sample used herein. In the GTS we operationalized the general factor using all symptoms, irritability using the temper, touchy, and angry symptoms, and defiant behavior using the argues, defies, annoys, blames, and spiteful symptoms. In this paper we first explore the external validity of the general and specific (irritability and defiant) ODD factors by testing their differential phenotypic associations with contemporaneous symptoms of internalizing psychopathology (depression/dysthymia, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD)) and externalizing psychopathology (CD, inattentive and hyperactive−impulsive ADHD symptom dimensions). We hypothesize that the ODD general factor reflects predispositions to, and thus will have phenotypic associations with, both internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. In contrast, we hypothesize that the specific defiant behavior factor will be more strongly associated with externalizing disorders whereas the specific irritability factor will be more strongly associated with internalizing disorders.

We next capitalize on the genetically informative design of the GTS to estimate the genetic and environmental influences on the ODD factors and their overlap with the internalizing and externalizing symptom dimensions. Previous studies have shown both common and unique genetic influences on externalizing and internalizing symptom dimensions [21]. We hypothesize that there will be substantial common genetic influences on the general ODD factor and all the other forms of psychopathology. Further, we hypothesize that the genetic influences specific to irritability will be shared primarily with internalizing psychopathology, whereas the genetic influences specific to the defiant behavior factor will be shared primarily with externalizing psychopathology. If supported, this will mean that the ODD general factor is important to isolate as it will allow the specific phenotypic and etiological associations of irritability and defiant behaviors with other outcomes to be more clearly studied.

Materials and methods

Participants and measures

Georgia Twin Study (GTS)

The GTS comprises 846 twin pairs from the Georgia Twin Registry, a population-based registry of twins (Mean age = 10.60 years, SD= 3.20 years, age range = 6−18 years), with 49% males, 82% European Americans, 11% African Americans, 1% Hispanic Americans, and 6% mixed/other ethnicity. The sample includes 392 (46%) monozygotic (MZ) and 454 (54%) dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs. In 1992−1993, using state birth records, 5620 parents of twins born between 1980 and 1991 in Georgia were contacted via mail. Of these, 1567 twin families joined the registry, among which 846 families provided complete ODD symptom ratings.

Symptom ratings were obtained from a parent (typically mothers) using the Emory Diagnostic Rating Scale (EDRS) [22]. The EDRS assesses symptoms of the major DSM–IV childhood psychiatric disorders. Parents rated symptoms of ADHD, ODD, CD, GAD, and depression/dysthymia on a 0–4 scale. Symptom scales based on these items demonstrated high internal consistency in the current sample (α = .95, .89, .91, .82, .90, .87, respectively). The EDRS yields ADHD and ODD diagnostic rates similar to population prevalences [22].

Parents of participating children provided written informed consent, and children provided assent, after receiving a complete description of the study. The study was approved by the Emory University IRB.

Results

Multiple regression analyses

Models were estimated using Mplus version 7 [23] using the robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) given non-normal symptom dimension distributions. Goodness of fit was evaluated using multiple indices, including the chi-square value, the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), the Tucker−Lewis index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the root mean square residual (RMSR) [24]. The acceptability of model fit was based on collectively comparing these fit indices against published guidelines: TLI ≥ 0.95 for excellent fit [25] and between 0.90 and 0.95 for acceptable fit [26]; RMSEA ≤ 0.08 for adequate fit and ≤0.05 for close fit [27]; RMSR ≤ 1.00 for good model fit [28]. The minimum value of the AIC and BIC was used to indicate the best-fitting alternative model [24].

We first examined the relations of the three ODD factors with the CD, inattention, hyperactivity−impulsivity, depression/dysthymia, and GAD symptom dimensions. Table 1 shows the standardized regression coefficients (β’s) of each symptom dimension on all three ODD factors and the sex, age, age2, sex × age, and sex × age2 covariates simultaneously. We also estimated the percentage of variance (i.e., R2) explained by all three ODD factors considered together. To test for sex differences, we contrasted the fit of a model in which the standardized regression coefficients for the three ODD factors were equated for boys and girls versus a model in which these coefficients varied by sex.

Table 1 Summary of external validity analyses of ODD factor scores from best-fitting bifactor model

As predicted, the ODD general factor was associated with all five symptom dimensions with β’s suggesting that each standard deviation increase in the ODD general factor was associated with a .18−.28 standard deviation increase in the external validity symptom dimensions. In contrast, the defiant behavior factor was uniquely associated with only the CD and inattentive and hyperactive−impulsive ADHD symptom dimensions (β’s = .53, .27, and .48, respectively) but not depression/dysthymia or GAD (β’s = −.04 and −.13, respectively). The irritability factor was uniquely associated with only the depression/dysthymia and GAD symptom dimensions (β’s = .35 and .40, respectively) but not the CD, inattentive, or hyperactive−impulsive symptom dimensions (β’s = −.07, .08, and −.06, respectively). The variance explained by the three ODD factors was 36% in CD, 23% in inattention, 29% in hyperactivity−impulsivity, 17% in depression/dysthymia, and 14% in GAD.

For all dependent variables except GAD and hyperactivity−impulsivity all three ODD factor regression coefficients could be equated for boys and girls. For GAD, a model in which the regression coefficient for irritability was larger for girls than for boys fits better, as indicated in the second row of results for GAD in Table 1. For hyperactivity−impulsivity, models in which the regression coefficients for irritability and defiant behavior varied by sex fit better than models in which all coefficients were equated but these are not shown due to space and the possibility that improved fit is due to chance.

Univariate behavior genetic analyses

We next conducted a set of univariate behavior genetic analyses to estimate genetic and environmental influences on the three ODD factors and the five external validity symptom dimensions as a prelude to our multivariate behavior genetic modeling. As shown in Table 2, the best-fitting model for the ODD general factor was the ACE model with moderate additive genetic and nonshared environmental influences (.41 and .45, respectively) and modest but significant shared environmental influences (.13). Although a model without shared environmental influences (the AE model) had a lower BIC (3619 versus 3623), all other fit indices favored the ACE model and the estimate of shared environmental influences was significant, thus favoring the ACE over the AE model. In contrast, the best-fitting model for the irritability and defiant behavior factors was the AE model, with moderate additive genetic (.64 and .68, respectively) and nonshared environmental influences (.36 and .32, respectively).

Table 2 Summary of univariate model fitting analyses of ODD factor scores from best-fitting bifactor model

As shown in Table 3, the best-fitting model for inattention and hyperactivity−impulsivity was the AE model, with the addition of the sibling influence/rater contrast parameter s. For both inattention and hyperactivity−impulsivity there were substantial additive genetic (.76 and .87, respectively) and nonshared environmental influences (.29 and .19, respectively), as well as rater contrasts (−.15 and −.12). Results were similar for CD, as the best-fitting model was the AE model with the addition of the sibling influence/rater contrast parameter s, with moderate additive genetic (.88) and nonshared environmental influences (.17) as well as rater contrast (−.09). For depression/dysthymia and GAD the best-fitting model was again the AE model, with moderate additive genetic (.61 and .70, respectively) and nonshared environmental influences (.39 and .30, respectively).

Table 3 Summary of univariate model fitting analyses of external validity symptom dimensions

Multivariate behavior genetic analyses

Our multivariate behavior genetic analyses used a series of Cholesky decompositions to model the genetic and environmental influences on the overlap between the three ODD factors and each of the five external validity symptom dimensions (see Table 4 and Fig. 1a–e). In these models the first factor includes all genetic (or environmental) influences that are common to the ODD general and specific factors and the external validity symptom dimension. The second factor includes all genetic (or environmental) influences on defiant behavior that are not shared with the ODD general factor but which also influence the irritability factor and the external validity symptom dimension. The third factor includes genetic (or environmental) influences on irritability that are not shared by the ODD general or defiant behavior factors but which also influence the external validity symptom dimension. The final factor represents genetic (or environmental) influences that are unique to the external validity symptom dimension. Because the ordering of variables is crucial to the interpretation of Cholesky decompositions, we also conducted these analyses switching the order of the defiant behavior and irritability factors. This allows assessment of whether each of these ODD factors shared incremental genetic (or environmental) influences with the external validity symptom dimensions after accounting for the genetic (or environmental) influences that were shared with both of these ODD factors. The effects of shared and nonshared environment were similarly structured.

Table 4 Summary of multivariate model fitting analyses of external validity disorders
Fig. 1
figure 1

a Best fitting model for genetic and environmental influences on three ODD dimensions and inattention. A, additive genetic influences; C, shared environmental influences; E, nonshared environmental influences; IN, inattention; Genl, general factor; Irr, irritability factor; Def, defiant behavior factor. Path coefficients for the genetic and environmental influences on the ODD factors and on inattention are squared standardized regression coefficients (i.e., variance components) with their 95% confidence intervals shown underneath. Decimal points are omitted to save space. b Best fitting model for genetic and environmental influences on three ODD dimensions and hyperactivity−impulsivity. A, additive genetic influences; C, shared environmental influences; E, nonshared environmental influences; HI, hyperactivity−impulsivity; Genl, general factor; Irr, irritability factor; Def, defiant behavior factor. Path coefficients for the genetic and environmental influences on the ODD factors and on hyperactivity−impulsivity are squared standardized regression coefficients (i.e., variance components) with their 95% confidence intervals shown underneath. Decimal points are omitted to save space. c Best fitting model for genetic and environmental influences on 3 ODD dimensions and CD. A, additive genetic influences; C, shared environmental influences; E, nonshared environmental influences; CD, conduct disorder; Genl, general factor, Irr, irritability factor, Def, defiant behavior factor. Path coefficients for the genetic and environmental influences on the ODD factors and on CD are squared standardized regression coefficients (i.e., variance components) with their 95% confidence intervals shown underneath. Decimal points are omitted to save space. d Best fitting model for genetic and environmental influences on three ODD dimensions and depression. A, additive genetic influences; C, shared environmental influences; E, nonshared environmental influences; Dep, depression; Genl, general factor; Irr, irritability factor; Def, defiant behavior factor. Path coefficients for the genetic and environmental influences on the ODD factors and on depression are squared standardized regression coefficients (i.e., variance components) with their 95% confidence intervals shown underneath. Decimal points are omitted to save space. e Best fitting model for genetic and environmental influences on three ODD dimensions and GAD. A, additive genetic influences; C, shared environmental influences; E, nonshared environmental influences; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; Genl, general factor; Irr, irritability factor; Def, defiant behavior factor. Path coefficients for the genetic and environmental influences on the ODD factors and on GAD are squared standardized regression coefficients (i.e., variance components) with their 95% confidence intervals shown underneath. Decimal points are omitted to save space. Dashed lines indicate paths that are not significantly greater than 0

In the models for CD and depression, the only shared environmental influences were those on the ODD general factor, but these were nonsignificant in the models for inattention, hyperactivity−impulsivity, and GAD. All nonshared environmental influences were significant in the models for inattention, hyperactivity−impulsivity, and CD, but for depression and GAD all of the nonshared environmental influences were unique to those symptom dimensions and none were shared with the ODD factors. The fit statistics for alternative models are shown in Table 4 and the parameter estimates and their 95% confidence intervals from the best-fitting multivariate genetic models are shown in Fig. 1a–e.

As shown in Figs. 1a–e and 2, the pattern of additive genetic influences differed across the five symptom dimensions. For inattention and hyperactivity−impulsivity, all genetic influences were significant. This suggests that the genetic influences that these ADHD symptom dimensions share with ODD are common to the general, irritability and defiant behavior factors, with additional residual genetic influences shared with both defiant behavior and irritability, and a final set of genetic influences shared only with irritability. Results were similar for CD, except that there were no genetic influences shared uniquely with irritability. Indeed, neither defiant behavior nor irritability shared unique genetic influences with CD when they were the third factor entered into the analyses.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Genetic variance components for external validity symptom dimensions. Hyper-Impuls, hyperactivity−impulsivity; CD, conduct disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder

The majority of the genetic influences that depression/dysthymia and GAD shared with ODD were common to the general, irritability, and defiant behavior factors, although they also shared additional residual genetic influences that were common to both defiant behavior and irritability. In contrast, depression/dysthymia and GAD did not share genetic influences uniquely with defiant behavior, as these were nonsignificant and much lower in magnitude (i.e., <= 1% of the variance). Although each of the five symptom dimensions shared substantial genetic influences in common with the three ODD factors, ranging from 19% for GAD to 38% for CD, the majority of genetic influences were unique to each symptom dimension and were not shared in common with the ODD factors. The breakdown of common and unique genetic influences on each external validity symptom dimension is shown in Fig. 2.

Discussion

We explored the relations of irritability, defiant behavior, and general ODD factors with other dimensions of externalizing and internalizing psychopathology. This included analyses at the phenotypic and etiological levels, capitalizing on the genetically informative design of the GTS. Both analyses support the external validity of distinguishing ODD symptom dimensions.

Regarding the specific ODD factors, our phenotypic analyses supported our predictions that the irritability factor would uniquely associate with depression and anxiety, but not the externalizing disorders, whereas the disruptive behavior factor would uniquely associate with the other externalizing disorders but not anxiety or depression, replicating previous findings [6, 11, 12, 15, 16, 29,30,31]. This clear differential pattern of correlates emphasizes the utility of distinguishing these ODD symptom dimensions, even though they typically are highly correlated (e.g. r’s = .73 [16], .79 [2], and .91 [1]).

As anticipated, the ODD general factor demonstrated phenotypic associations with all internalizing and externalizing symptom dimensions. It is important to note that our bifactor model specifies orthogonal specific and general factors. Thus, the associations of the internalizing and externalizing symptom dimensions with the ODD general factor were independent of their associations with the specific irritability and defiant behavior factors. This finding suggests that the bifactor modeling approach isolates a meaningful general factor of ODD in the presence of distinct irritability and defiant behavior factors, which cannot be achieved with simpler dimensional models. A key implication of this is that it may be erroneous to separate irritability from defiant behavior as a separate diagnostic category, as has been done with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in the DSM 5 [7].

Using univariate behavior genetic analyses we tested alternative etiological models of the ODD dimensions. These results using psychiatric symptoms are similar to and extend heritability estimates from previous studies [17, 18]. As in other studies, our modeling indicated that additive genetic rather than shared environmental influences underlie the familial aggregation of irritability and defiant behavior. We estimated moderate heritability (64 and 68%, respectively) for irritability and defiant behavior, estimates that are somewhat higher than those reported elsewhere (e.g., 37 and 45% in Stringaris et al. [17]). This may reflect our modeling of the ODD dimensions using DSM symptoms and as factors within a bifactor framework, with a consequent reduction in measurement error due to using latent factors. Our general ODD factor also showed moderate heritability (41%) and modest shared environmental influences (13%).

We predicted that the differential phenotypic associations between the three ODD factors and the other psychopathology dimensions would be reflected at the etiological level. Multivariate genetic analyses showed substantial overlap between genetic influences on the ODD dimensions and the other dimensions of psychopathology, consistent with findings of common genetic influences on different forms of psychopathology [21, 32]. Consistent with the generalist genes model, we found that the majority of genetic influences that underlie comorbidity were related to the general, irritability, and defiant behavior ODD factors.

The genetic influences on the irritability and defiant behavior factors that were independent from the general ODD factor also contributed to the other forms of psychopathology. We hypothesized that genetic influences specific to the defiant behavior factor would also underlie the externalizing symptom dimensions. As shown in Fig. 2, our results supported this prediction, as CD, inattention, and hyperactivity−impulsivity all shared genetic influences (ranging from 10 to 15% of the variance) in common with defiant behavior, whereas genetic influences shared in common only with irritability were minimal (≤2%) and in the case of CD, nonsignificant. Indeed, CD did not share any genetic influences uniquely with either irritability or defiant behavior, but rather shared genetic influences that were common to both irritability and defiant behavior. This may reflect common genetic influences on all three symptom dimensions shared with negative emotionality [33].

Similarly, as shown in Fig. 2, our prediction that depression and GAD symptoms would share genetic influences uniquely with irritability (ranging from 8 to 9% of the variance) was supported, whereas they did not share genetic influences uniquely with defiant behavior (≤1%). This result is consistent with other behavior genetic analyses, including Stringaris et al.’s [17] finding that the genetic correlation of depression with irritability is higher than with defiant behavior. One explanation for this pattern of results is that genes that underlie the irritability but not the defiant component of ODD increase risk for depressed mood (and generalized anxiety in our study). Thus, the identification of genes underlying the unique association between irritability and depression or anxiety will be obscured when genetic variance shared by irritability, defiant behavior and the general ODD dimension is not distinguished.

A growing body of literature highlights the need to elucidate the hierarchical structure of, and transdiagnostic relations among, different forms of psychopathology. Our results contribute to this emerging literature, and embody these recent trends that emphasize transdiagnostic [34] and hierarchical structural approaches to psychopathology [35,36,37]. Specifically, our findings validate the distinction between the irritability and defiant behavior ODD symptom dimensions while also furthering the evidence that irritability is correlated with the defiant behavior dimension. Within the heterogeneous ODD construct, shared genetic influences at least partly explain differential phenotypic associations (i.e., irritability with depression and anxiety; defiant behavior with externalizing disorders). The typically early emergence of the ODD phenotype suggests its likely utility for identifying important developmental risk factors for later psychopathology.

Strengths and limitations

The GTS has a number of advantages for testing the validity and utility of a bifactor model of ODD. These include a large community sample and a genetically informative design. An advantage over extant genetically informative studies of ODD dimensions is our explicit assessment of DSM symptom dimensions. Nonetheless, its cross-sectional design prevents tests of the longitudinal predictive validity of these factors.

Another limitation is the reliance on a single informant for all measures, which leaves correlations vulnerable to inflation due to common method variance [38], such as various rater effects. In bifactor modeling this is most likely to be captured by the general factor, as rater effects would likely be common to all items. In a twin study, where ratings are provided by a single parent, common method variance would be shared by both twins. This is consistent with our findings of shared environmental influences only on the ODD general factor. Given that these effects were small and we did not find shared environmental influences on the other psychopathology dimensions, it seems unlikely that common method variance has influenced our results to an appreciable extent. Nonetheless, future studies using multiple informants will be valuable in further developing the evidence for the external validity of our bifactor modeling approach.