Introduction

The hypocretin (orexin) system consists of two excitatory neuropeptides (hypocretin-1/orexin A and hypocretin-2/orexin B) expressed by neurons of the hypothalamus that innervate a number of targets, including the ventral tegmental area (VTA), dorsal raphe nucleus, and locus coeruleus (de Lecea et al, 1998; Peyron et al, 1998; Sakurai et al, 1998). Accordingly, these brain structures show high levels of the peptides’ cognate G-protein-coupled receptors, OX1R and OX2R (de Lecea et al, 1998; Marcus et al, 2001; Sakurai et al, 1998; Zhu et al, 2003). Hypothalamic orexin neurons are in this way positioned to engender goal-directed behaviors (eg, feeding, mating, and drug-seeking) that are in turn regulated by monoamine transmitters (ie, dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine) native to these brainstem nuclei (Borgland et al, 2009; Harris et al, 2005; Muschamp et al, 2007; Sakurai, 2014). This is accomplished in part by modulating aminergic transmission in forebrain circuits that are critical for sustained attention and executive function, such as projections from prefrontal cortex to ventral striatum (Dalley et al, 2008; Ongur and Price, 2000). While this anatomical arrangement suggests that orexin transmission is plausibly involved in impulsive behavior, only a few reports have addressed this question, and a clear picture has not yet emerged (Boschen et al, 2009; Lambe et al, 2005; Muschamp et al, 2014).

Defined as a tendency to engage in behaviors without forethought (Evenden, 1999), impulsivity is apparent across psychiatric diagnostic categories and can lead to poorer outcomes in patients with attention deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD), bipolar disorder, and substance use disorders (de Wit, 2009; Kessler et al, 2014; Lopez-Torrecillas et al, 2014; Swann, 2010; Vall et al, 2015). The putative relationship between motivated behavior and orexin transmission support targeting orexin receptors for therapeutic intervention. The recent approval for clinical use of the OX1R/OX2R antagonist suvorexant in the treatment of insomnia raises the question of whether this compound may also be valuable in treating psychiatric disorders characterized by high levels of impulsivity (Khoo and Brown, 2014; Roecker et al, 2016; Scammell and Winrow, 2011). The present study sought to assess the potential therapeutic utility of suvorexant as well as subtype-selective OXR antagonists in two rodent models of impulsivity, the five-choice serial reaction time (5-CSRTT) and delay discounting tasks (Winstanley et al, 2006). Previous studies demonstrate reduced motor impulsivity following pre-treatment with a selective OX1R antagonist before 5-CSRTT (Muschamp et al, 2014). To gain further insight into the functional neuroanatomy that may underlie effects of OXR blockade on impulsive behaviors, we performed direct infusions of suvorexant within VTA, a DA-rich principal target of orexin afferents (Fadel and Deutch, 2002; Peyron et al, 1998). In addition, we assessed how orexin receptor blockade altered Fos immunoreactivity (ir) within DA-producing neurons of VTA and further how suvorexant influences orexin-and cocaine-evoked calcium transients within VTA neurons in vitro.

Materials and methods

Animals

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (Charles River) arrived at Temple University’s vivarium ~60 days old, were pair-housed and food-restricted to 85% of their free-fed bodyweight. Rats were acclimated for at least 1 wk. All experimental procedures were approved by Temple University’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Drugs

For all experiments, suvorexant (SelleckChem, Munich, Germany), SB334867, and TCS-OX2-29 (provided by Dr Yanan Zhang, Research Triangle Institute, NC) were dissolved in dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO) and administered at 0.3 ml/kg (i.p), or 200 nl when bilaterally injected into VTA (100 nl/hemisphere). Cocaine hydrochloride (Sigma Chemical Company, St. Louis, MO) was dissolved in 0.9% physiological saline and administered at 1.0 ml/kg (i.p.). Orexin A was purchased from American Peptide Inc. (Sunnyvale, CA).

Surgical Procedure

General anesthesia was induced and maintained with 2–5% isoflurane in oxygen (flow rate: 1.5 l/min). Rats were implanted with bilateral stainless steel guide cannulae (22 G, 8.5 mm in length) into VTA (from bregma in mm: AP -5.4; ML ±2.1 at 10o angle; DV −6.8). Stainless-steel obdurators (28 G, 8.5 mm in length) were kept in guide cannulae to maintain their patency. When ready for VTA-directed injections, internal cannulae (28 G, 9.5 mm in length) were used for injections (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA).

Experimental Procedures

Experiment 1: 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT)

Rats were trained in computer-controlled operant chambers housed inside ventilated, sound-attenuating cabinets (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT). Chambers were fitted with 5 internally illuminated apertures monitored by infrared detectors. The opposite wall contained an illuminated food hopper equipped with an infrared detector (Supplementary Figure 1). Rats were trained daily for 90 trials or for 30 min and received either a sucrose pellet (45 mg; Bio-Serv, Flemington, NJ) for a correct response (poking illuminated aperture) or a ‘time-out’ period in which the house light was turned off after an incorrect, premature, or omitted response. After rats achieved ≥70% correct, and⩽15% omitted responses across four consecutive daily sessions, drug treatment sessions began. Here, suvorexant or TCS-OX2-29 (0, 3, 10, or 30 mg/kg) was administered 30 min before testing and, in subsequent sessions, cocaine (0 or 3 mg/kg) was then administered 5 min before the start of testing. For direct site injections, suvorexant (3 μg per hemisphere) was administered via 28-G injector cannulae attached to Hamilton syringes (Model 7101, Hamilton) using a precision pump (PHD 2000, Harvard Apparatus) at a rate of 100 nl/min. Anatomical localization of injections to the VTA DA neuronal population was confirmed by immunohistochemical labeling for tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) in midbrain sections collected after behavioral testing was concluded (Figure 3b).

Experiment 2: delay discounting

A separate cohort of rats was trained in chambers as described above. Rats were trained for 60 trials and received either a single sucrose pellet for a response associated with the small-reward option or four sucrose pellets for a response associated with the large-reward option following a 0-, 15-, 30-, or 45-s delay of selecting the left or right internally illuminated apertures. The left or right apertures were randomly assigned for each rat to administer a large or small reward. Sessions were divided into 4 blocks of 15 trials each with two ‘forced-choice’ trials at the start of each block. In forced-choice trials, left and right apertures were illuminated individually in alternate trials and rewarded after the same delay period used in ten subsequent free-choice trials in which both left and right apertures are illuminated simultaneously. Thus, a single, enforced exposure to reward contingencies preceded every block of trials. Only rats that responded in both forced choice trials were used, demonstrating familiarity with the paradigm and capacity for consistent responding under time-contingent conditions. After rats were trained and responded with ≥70% correct and ≤15% omissions across four consecutive sessions, drug treatments were administered according to a Latin square design. Suvorexant, TCS-OX2-29, or SB334867 (suvorexant; TCS-OX2-29: 0, 3, 10, 30 mg/kg; SB334867: 0, 1, 3, 10 mg/kg) was administered 30 min before and cocaine (0 or 3 mg/kg) was administered 5 min before the delay discounting testing (Supplementary Figure 1D).

Experiment 3: TH and fos immunohistochemistry in VTA

To assess effects of OXR blockade on neuronal activation of DA-producing neurons in VTA, a behaviorally and pharmacologically naïve cohort of rats were administered suvorexant (0 or 30 mg/kg, i.p.) 30 min prior to cocaine (0 or 3 mg/kg) in their home cage. Ninety minutes following cocaine, rats were deeply anesthetized with a phenytoin-phenobarbital cocktail (120 mg/kg, i.p.) and perfused with ~100 ml cold phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; 0.1M, pH 7.4) followed by ~300 ml 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains were post-fixed for 24 h in 4 °C PFA followed by 72 h in 4 °C 30% sucrose and finally flash-frozen in 2-methyl butane chilled on dry ice. Brains were subsequently sectioned using a cryostat, and coronal sections (40 μm), containing the entire VTA (from bregma: AP −5.0 mm to −6.5 mm; Brain Maps III atlas available online: http://larrywswanson.com/) were collected.

For immunolabeling, tissue sections were washed in 0.1 M PBS, blocked in 5% donkey serum in PBS with 0.3% Triton X-100 (PBS+) and incubated in primary antibody solution for 72 h at 4 °C (rabbit anti-cFos (1 : 1,000; SC-52, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA) in 1.5% donkey serum in PBS+). Sections were then incubated in secondary antibody solution for 3 h (donkey anti-rabbit AlexaFluor 555 (1:400; ThermoFisher, Waltham, MA) in 1.5% donkey serum in PBS+). Sections underwent serial immunolabeling as above using rabbit anti-TH (1:1,000; AB-152, Millipore, Billerica, MA) primary antibody and donkey anti-rabbit AlexaFluor 488 (1:400; ThermoFisher)] secondary antibody.

All TH-ir neurons and Fos-ir nuclei that were visible in every third tissue section collected were counted by an experimenter blinded to treatment conditions using an Eclipse 80i upright fluorescent microscope (Nikon) under × 10 objective magnification (Supplementary Figure 3). Three sections were averaged together for counting. The percentage of Fos+–TH+ neurons was calculated by: [(TH+−Fos+)/(total TH+) × 100].

Experiment 4: intracellular calcium measurement

Neuronal cell culture

Neurons from the VTA were dissociated from neonatal (1–2-day old) Sprague Dawley rats (Ace Animal, Boyertown, PA) of both sexes as previously described (Barr et al, 2015). Newborn rats were decapitated, the brains removed surgically and immersed in ice-cold Hanks balanced salt solution (HBSS; Mediatech, Herndon, VA). The VTA was subjected to enzymatic digestion (papain, 37 °C) followed by mechanical trituration in presence of total medium – Neurobasal A (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) containing 1% GlutaMax (Invitrogen), 2% penicillin-streptomycin-amphotericin B solution (Mediatech) and 10% fetal bovine serum. Cells were cultured on round 25 mm glass coverslips coated with poly-L-lysine (Sigma) in six-well plates. Cultures were maintained at 37 °C in a humidified atmosphere with 5% CO2. The mitotic inhibitor cytosine β-arabinofuranoside (1 μM; Sigma) was added to the culture the third day to inhibit glial cell proliferation. Cells were used after 5 days in culture.

Calcium imaging

[Ca2+]i was measured as previously described (Arslan et al, 2000; Barr et al, 2015). Cells were incubated with 5 μM fura-2 AM (Invitrogen) in HBSS at room temperature for 45 min in the dark, washed three times with dye-free HBSS, and then incubated for another 45 min to allow for complete de-esterification of the dye. Coverslips (25 mm diameter) were subsequently mounted in an open bath chamber (RP-40LP, Warner Instruments; Hamden, CT) on the stage of an inverted microscope Nikon Eclipse TiE (Nikon; Melville, NY) equipped with a Perfect Focus System and a Photometrics CoolSnap HQ2 CCD camera (Photometrics, Tucson, AZ). During the experiments, the Perfect Focus System was activated. Fura-2 AM fluorescence (emission=510 nm), following alternate excitation at 340 and 380 nm, was acquired at a frequency of 0.25 Hz. Images were acquired and analyzed using NIS-Elements AR 3.1 software (Nikon). After appropriate calibration with ionomycin and CaCl2, and Ca2+ free and EGTA, respectively, the ratio of the fluorescence signals (340/380 nm) was converted to Ca2+ concentrations. In Ca2+-free experiments, CaCl2 was omitted. Calcium transients from VTA neurons were measured following application of orexin A (10 nM), orexin A with cocaine (10 μM), and orexin A with cocaine and suvorexant (1 μM). This method allows for detection of spontaneous Ca2+ transients under baseline conditions as well as from pharmacological compound application in vitro.

Statistical analyses

For 5-CSRTT, one-way repeated measures ANOVA with Bonferroni-corrected contrasts against vehicle-treated control groups were used to compare number of premature responses, omissions, latency to retrieve sucrose reward, and accuracy (# Correct/(# Correct+# Incorrect)) as measures of impulsivity. For delay discounting, a two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc tests were used to compare mean number of large rewards chosen at each delay period. For immunohistochemical analyses, one-way ANOVA was used to examine mean estimated proportion of TH+/Fos+ cells and total Fos+ cells between treatment groups. Calcium imaging data were analyzed using Bonferroni-corrected t-tests comparing (orexin only, cocaine+orexin+suvorexant) against the orexin+cocaine group.

Results

Experiment 1A: Suvorexant Attenuates Motor Impulsivity

Results from the present study revealed a significant main effect of suvorexant on premature responses in 5-CSRTT (F(2.8, 39.9)=2.90, p<0.05; Figure 1). Contrasts against the vehicle-treated control group found that the 30 mg/kg suvorexant was effective in reducing premature responses (p<0.05). In a separate cohort of rats, a significant effect of suvorexant (30 mg/kg, i.p.) on reducing premature responses relative to vehicle pre-treatment was also found (Supplementary Figure 2). No significant effects of TCS-OX2-29 on premature responses (F(2.0, 29.2)=2.38, NS), omissions (F(2.1, 28.8)=2.68, NS), latency to retrieve sucrose reward (F(1.7, 24.4)=1.42, NS) or accuracy (F(1.7, 22.8)=0.29, NS) were found. These results support the conclusion that suvorexant has a selective effect on motor impulsivity without interfering with the motivation to consume sucrose rewards.

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of suvorexant and TCS-OX2-29 on (a) number of premature responses in 5-CSRTT. Effects of suvorexant and TCS-OX2-29 on (b) number of omissions, % accuracy and latency to retrieve sucrose reward are also depicted. Data are mean±S.E.M. (n=15–16 rats per group). *p<0.05 compared to the respective 0 mg/kg group.

PowerPoint slide

Experiment 1B: Suvorexant Attenuates Cocaine-Induced Motor Impulsivity by Systemic and Intra-VTA Injection

A main effect of drug group (vehicle-saline, suvorexant-saline, vehicle-cocaine, and suvorexant-cocaine) on premature responses was found (F(1.67, 23.4)=10.88, p<0.001; Figures 2,3) Contrasts against vehicle-saline control group revealed that cocaine increased premature responding (p<0.01), but that suvorexant pretreatment normalized cocaine-elicited premature responses to control levels. Neither cocaine nor suvorexant, alone or in combination, had significant effect on omissions (F (3, 55)=1.52, NS), latency to retrieve sucrose reward (F(1.9, 26.6)=1.02, NS) or accuracy (F(1.7, 23.42)=0.67, NS).

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of suvorexant on cocaine-evoked premature responses, number of omissions, % accuracy and latency to retrieve sucrose reward in 5-CSRTT (n=15 rats per group). Data are mean±S.E.M. **p<0.01 compared to Veh-Sal group.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of site-directed suvorexant in the VTA on (a) cocaine-evoked premature responses, (b) number of omissions, % accuracy and latency to retrieve sucrose reward in 5-CSRTT (n=7 rats per group). (c) Atlas images of cannula placements. Cannula tracks of animals 1–7 are shown as circles within TH+ neurons of the ventral tegmental area. Atlas images taken from Brain Maps III, available online: http://larrywswanson.com/?page_id=164. Solid lines showing individual animal responses across sessions. Data are mean±S.E.M. * p<0.05 compared to vehicle-saline group.

PowerPoint slide

A main effect of drug group on premature responses was additionally observed when suvorexant (3 μg/hemisphere) was administered into bilateral VTA (F (1.8, 11.1)=9.99, p<0.01). Post-hoc tests revealed that cocaine increased premature responding relative to the vehicle-saline control group (p<0.05), while suvorexant pretreatment normalized cocaine-elicited premature responses to control levels. Neither cocaine nor suvorexant, alone or in combination, had any significant effect on omissions (F(1.9, 11.23)=0.58, NS), latency to retrieve sucrose reward (F(1.2, 6.95)=0.76, NS) or accuracy (F(1.5, 9.13)=1.61, NS).

Experiment 2: Orexin Receptor Antagonists Selectively Influence Impulsive Action

The present study revealed preference for the larger reward (4 sucrose pellets) decreased as delay time increased for all treatment cohorts: SB334867 doses (F(3, 64)=128.70, p<0.001) suvorexant doses (F(3, 188)=42.45, p<0.001) and TCS-OX2-29 doses (F(3, 72)=35.94, p<0.001)(Figure 4). Suvorexant did not alter impulsive choice across a dose-response (F(3, 188)=0.91, NS). TCS-OX2-29 dose-response revealed a significant main effect (F(3, 72)=4.72, p<0.05), but no significant pairwise comparisons were found. At the 10 mg/kg dose of SB334867 during the no-delay block, rats chose the small-reward option more frequently than the large-reward option (p<0.01).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of SB334867, TCS-OX2-29 and suvorexant on large reward options in delay discounting (n=5–14 rats per group). **p<0.01 compared to 0 mg/kg dose in respective delay block. ††p<0.01 indicates main effect of drug treatment across delay blocks.

PowerPoint slide

Experiment 3: Neither Cocaine nor Suvorexant, Alone or in Combination, Alter Fos-ir Within DA-Producing VTA Neurons

A one-way ANOVA examining TH+-Fos+ neurons in VTA revealed no effect of drug group (vehicle-saline, suvorexant-saline, vehicle-cocaine, suvorexant-cocaine) (F(3, 40)=0.76, p>0.05). Additionally, no differences in total Fos+ neurons in VTA were observed between drug groups (F(3, 40)=0.62, p>0.05; Figure 5a). Finally, no difference in Fos+ nuclei of non-TH-labeled cells found (Supplementary Figure 4).

Figure 5
figure 5

Effects of suvorexant on Fos-ir within VTA DA-producing neurons and cocaine- and orexin-evoked calcium transients within VTA neurons in vitro. (a) % of total TH+ neurons expressing cFos+-ir, average total TH−-cFos+ counts within VTA tissue slices and representative photomicrograph of TH+-cFos+ neurons (white arrows) TH+-Fos− (blue arrows) TH−-Fos+ (orange arrows) in vehicle-pretreated rats. Data in a are mean±S.E.M. At × 10 magnification (n=4–6 rats per group). (b) Tracings of VTA Ca2+ responses elicited by different cocaine, orexin A, and suvorexant treatments. (c) Comparison of amplitudes and AUCs of Ca2+ responses. Data are expressed as mean±S.E.M. (n=11–17 cells per group). ***p<0.0001 compared to cocaine+orexin calcium transients. A full color version of this figure is available at the Neuropsychopharmacology journal online.

PowerPoint slide

Experiment 4: In vitro Orexin A Potentiates Cocaine-Evoked Calcium Transients within VTA Neurons in a Suvorexant-Sensitive Manner

Cocaine (10 μM) did not elicit an increase in [Ca2+]i within VTA neurons in vitro (Δ[Ca2+]i=9±0.6 nM; area under curve (AUC) of Ca2+ response=8.53±0.7 nM) (Figure 5). Application of orexin A (10 nM) elevated [Ca2+]i of VTA neurons by 381±3.7 nM and AUC of 154.75±2.9 nM. Combined administration of cocaine (10 μM) 10 min prior to orexin A produced a potentiated increase in [Ca2+]i (Δ[Ca2+]i=544±4.6 nM; AUC=304.3±3.8 Nm, p<0.001). The effect of cocaine and orexin A was completely abrogated by pretreatment with suvorexant (1 μM, 10 min): Δ[Ca2+]i=12±0.8 nM and AUC=29.24±1.1 nM Amplitude: (F(3,20)=3914, p<0.001); AUC: (F(3, 20)=9655, p<0.001).

Discussion

We found that the dual orexin receptor antagonist suvorexant attenuates premature responses in the 5-CSRTT a rodent test of motor impulsivity (Bari et al, 2008; Winstanley et al, 2006). Future work will focus on further clarifying the role, if any, of OX2R in impulsivity (Hirose et al, 2003). The reductions in premature responding produced by suvorexant are similar to those we have demonstrated using the OX1R-selective antagonist SB334867 (Muschamp et al, 2014; Porter et al, 2001; Smart et al, 2001), suggesting the effect is mediated by this receptor. These findings are consistent with a dichotomous view of orexin receptor function where OX1R regulates motivated behavior and OX2R sleep homeostasis (Carter et al, 2013; España et al, 2011; Willie et al, 2003). Importantly and despite the clinical use of suvorexant to treat insomnia (Roecker et al, 2016), reductions in impulsive-like behavior were not due to somnolence or non-specific locomotor decrements. For instance, we observed no increase in the number of trials omitted, subjects’ latency to retrieve sucrose rewards, or accuracy even during high-dose drug trials. This is seemingly at odds with reports that reflect decreases in open-field ambulation and increased sleep following orexin receptor blockade (Bonaventure et al, 2015; Letavic et al, 2015; Rodgers et al, 2001). The effects we see may be due to higher level of engaging sensory stimulation present in the 5-CSRTT compared to the environment present during tests of open field ambulation. Moreover, in humans and rodents, dual orexin receptor antagonists act primarily to prevent awakening (ie reduced wake after sleep onset) rather than to induce sleep (ie modest effect on sleep latency; Brisbare-Roch et al, 2007). Together, these results suggest that basal orexin tone at OX1Rs facilitates prepotent locomotor responses and may mediate impulsivity which itself is characteristic of substance use disorders (reviewed in Alcaraz-Iborra and Cubero, 2015). The decrease in motor impulsivity in the high-dose suvorexant group (30 mg/kg, i.p.) is modest, and we suspected this to be a product of a ‘floor effect’ in which baseline (vehicle-pretreatment) premature responses were relatively low. To examine this possibility, we trained a separate cohort of rats, finding that their mean baseline premature responding was higher. Indeed, in this group we were able to resolve a relatively greater reduction in premature responses between vehicle- and suvorexant-pretreated groups (Supplementary Figure 2).

While OX1R antagonists alone can reduce spontaneous impulsive behavior (Muschamp et al, 2014), their possible clinical utility in mitigating psychostimulant-evoked impulsiveness is also of interest (see: de Wit, 2009). Accordingly, we then tested the extent to which suvorexant could attenuate premature responses evoked by a moderate dose of cocaine. As predicted, cocaine alone produced a two-fold increase in premature responses. This effect was mitigated by suvorexant pretreatment while measures of response capacity (ie omissions, latency, accuracy) again remained unchanged. Taken together, these results support potential clinical utility of orexin receptor antagonists in treating psychiatric disorders characterized by significant trait impulsivity or for transient impulsive behavior engendered by cocaine intoxication. Previous literature implicating the mesolimbic DA pathway in impulsivity coupled with observations of orexin-mediated excitation of VTA DA neurons (Korotkova et al, 2003; Moorman and Aston-Jones, 2010), lead us to hypothesize that it may be an important locus for some forms of impulsivity (Buckholtz et al, 2010; Jentsch and Taylor, 1999; Martin and Potts, 2004; Robbins, 2002). In general agreement with this, systemic cocaine again increased motor impulsivity, while suvorexant infused directly into the VTA attenuated this effect. We targeted a relatively rostral aspect of the VTA in these experiments. The extent to which suvorexant produces similar effects at substructures within the VTA, and potentially opposite effects by acting at adjacent modulatory structures (eg, rostromedial tegmentum, RMTg; Jhou et al, 2009a, 2009b) requires additional study.

Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct encompassing both deficits in response inhibition (ie motor impulsivity) and decision-making (eg, delay discounting; Evenden, 1999). To further characterize the possible ‘anti-impulsive’ profile of orexin receptor antagonists, we used a model of impulsive choice (Robinson et al, 2009). In this task, subjects are prompted to choose between a small sucrose reward available immediately or a larger sucrose reward delivered after a variable delay (15–45 s). Preference for the small reward models subjects’ impulsivity as they discount the value of the larger reward based on the delay to its receipt. In light of the largely OX1R-mediated effects of SB334867 and suvorexant on motor impulsivity in the 5-CSRTT, we predicted that these compounds would also increase the number of trials in which rats selected a larger, delayed reward. While subjects satisfied task requirements by completing virtually all trials, neither SB334867 nor suvorexant exerted any appreciable effect on selection of the larger, delayed reward. Interestingly, SB334867 did reduce preference for the larger reward when it was delivered immediately (ie, in the ‘0-delay’ block) and at a frequency narrowly above chance. Because OX1Rs are involved in reward processing, including that of palatable food rewards (Borgland et al, 2009; Zheng et al, 2007), this response pattern is interpretable as SB334867-mediated indifference or an ‘anhedonia-like’ state as has been observed during tasks probing brain reward function (Muschamp et al, 2014). That is, blockade of OX1Rs may devalue the incentive strength of the large, immediate reward such that even when available immediately, it is chosen no more often than the small reward. The mechanisms underlying this divergent effect are unclear but may derive from the differential expression of OX1R and OX2R at the gross anatomical level (Marcus et al, 2001). For instance, the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus involved in neuroendocrine function predominantly expresses OX2Rs, while the prefrontal cortex involved in planning executive function mainly expresses OX1R. Likewise, at the cellular level the synaptic location (ie pre- vs post-) of orexin receptor subtypes in principal or interneurons is incompletely characterized in structures known to play a role in the behaviors we tested.

In light of past literature showing direct excitatory effects of orexins on activity of VTA DA neurons (Korotkova et al, 2003), we sought to quantify differences in Fos-ir within TH+ neurons of the VTA after rats were treated with suvorexant alone or in combination with cocaine. Contrary to our expectations, we observed no change in Fos-ir in TH+ neurons of rats in any treatment group. We also saw no increase in Fos-ir nuclei (TH−) in VTA. Though many authors have reported effects of cocaine or other rewards on Fos-ir in VTA DA neurons, our results are more consistent with reports where robust manipulations (ie rewarding electrical stimulation of afferents to VTA) that evoke marked increases in Fos-ir, fail to do so in TH+ neurons of VTA (Hunt and McGregor, 1998; Ishida et al, 2001). It is then plausible that among the many identified immediate early genes expressed by neurons, Fos-ir may not be a reliable marker of neuronal activation in some experimental settings.

With this possibility in mind, we next used a related measure (ie changes in intracellular calcium) to explore the effect of orexin A, suvorexant, or cocaine on VTA neuronal activity. Applied together, we saw that cocaine appears to potentiate the effects of orexin A on intracellular calcium transients in a manner consistent with prior electrophysiology studies examining influence of orexin on VTA DA neurons (Borgland et al, 2006; Korotkova et al, 2003). Notably, we also found that increases in calcium transients produced by orexin A with or without cocaine were abolished by suvorexant. While we did not examine effects of suvorexant alone, little basal orexin tone in vitro and absence of reported inverse agonist activity by suvorexant, lead us to expect it would produce no effect on intracellular calcium. Behaviorally relevant doses of cocaine in vivo can produce low-μM concentrations of cocaine in the striatum that amplify IP3 signaling in medium spiny neurons of the NAc (Barr et al, 2015). Both OX1R and OX2R also use Gq signaling to generate IP3. Our results raise the possibility of similar signal transduction events in orexin-sensitive VTA neurons. Based on existing in vivo neurochemistry data, these events can be expected to increase the probability of VTA neuronal burst firing and can in turn elevate levels of synaptic DA in NAc (España et al, 2011; Gentile et al, 2017; Overton and Clark, 1997; Prince et al, 2015). Prior central infusion experiments show that pharmacologically -evoked DA release in the NAc results in greater premature responses in the 5-CSRTT (Robbins, 2002).

The effects of orexin receptor antagonists on premature responses in the 5-CSRTT but not on delay discounting suggest a preferential action on motor impulsivity. The reasons for this are not clear but are consistent with data that describe an important role for the orexin system in organizing goal-directed behavioral output. For instance, OX1R receptors in VTA can potentiate the excitability of DA neurons elicited during operant acquisition of a food reward (Borgland et al, 2009; Zheng et al, 2007). Electrophysiological recordings of identified orexin neurons in awake, freely moving rats also show that firing rates are highest in active waking, particularly during orienting responses or exploratory behavior (eg, rearing and whisking), and lowest during sleep (Mileykovskiy et al, 2005). The efficient performance of the 5-CSRTT observed here can be readily be described as ‘active waking’. We hypothesize that this behavior is typically accompanied by increased orexin transmission. Together, our findings suggest that this pattern of neuronal activity can contribute to increased emission of prepotent goal-directed behaviors. Accordingly, it provides a rationale for further study of suvorexant and similar drugs in treating psychopathologies characterized by impulsive-like behaviors including substance use disorders and ADHD.

Funding and disclosure

This work was supported by grants provided by USPHS grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and the Margaret Q. Landenberger Research Foundation (R21DA032837, R01DA040693, YZ and RTI; T32DA007237, P30DA013429, R00DA031767, Landenberger Found., JWM and TUSM/CSAR). The authors declare no conflict of interest.