Introduction

In a recent US survey of high school seniors, 84.4% of all cocaine users reported that they began alcohol drinking or cigarette smoking before they began to use cocaine (Johnston et al, 2014). These results are in agreement with those from other epidemiological studies showing that initial exposure to legal drugs is associated with subsequent use of illicit drugs (Kandel, 1975; Kandel et al, 1992; Kirby and Barry, 2012; Wagner and Anthony, 2002). These findings led to the formulation of the ‘gateway’ hypothesis that posits that initial exposure to legal drugs causes subsequent addiction to illicit drugs (Kandel et al, 1992). However, epidemiological studies are correlational and cannot rule out the alternative hypothesis of time-independent shared addiction vulnerability to legal and illegal drugs (Vanyukov et al, 2012). In addition, human studies cannot test experimentally the gateway hypothesis, because a definitive study will require random assignment of subjects to groups exposed or not exposed to the legal drug. The gateway hypothesis can be tested in animal models where pre-exposure to the legal drug is under experimenter control. In our study, we empirically tested whether there is a biological basis for the gateway hypothesis with established rat models using alcohol and cocaine as prototypical legal and illegal drugs, respectively. Although the legal status of some drugs (eg, marijuana) may change over time, it is unlikely that cocaine will become a legal drug in the foreseeable future in the United States and other countries.

Several previous studies examined whether prior alcohol exposure increases cocaine conditioned place preference (CPP). In four studies, adolescent mice (Mateos-Garcia et al, 2015; Molet et al, 2013) or rats (Hutchison and Riley, 2012), or adult rats (Hutchison and Riley, 2012) that were pre-exposed to alcohol showed increased cocaine CPP. In contrast, Le Pen et al (1998) reported that alcohol pre-exposure in adult rats decreased subsequent cocaine CPP in low but not high alcohol drinkers. However, the relevance of these studies to the gateway hypothesis is limited, because the drug CPP procedure is not a good model of human addiction, because drug exposure is very low and independent of the subject’s behavior (Lu et al, 2003).

Two previous studies examined whether prior alcohol exposure increases cocaine self-administration in rats. Mierzejewski et al (2003) trained rats to self-administer alcohol in the sucrose-fading procedure and then divided them into low vs high alcohol self-administration. They reported that the high-intake group self-administered more cocaine during the initiation phase. However, the results of this study are difficult to interpret in the context of the gateway hypothesis, because the authors did not include a control group of alcohol-naive rats that only self-administered sucrose during training. In addition, the authors used a single cocaine dose. Thus, it is unknown whether increased drug intake reflects increased or decreased cocaine reward (Yokel, 1987).

More recently, Mateos-Garcia et al (2015) intermittently exposed adolescent male and female mice to alcohol (twice-daily, 2.5 g/kg, i.p) during adolescence and reported that this exposure modestly increased acquisition of cocaine self-administration but not drug self-administration under a progressive ratio reinforcement schedule. A limitation of this study, in which the authors also reported that prior alcohol exposure increased cocaine CPP, is that group differences in acquisition of cocaine self-administration may be due to the non-specific effects of alcohol as a pharmacological stressor (Sarnyai et al, 2001). Indeed, exposure to alcohol doses lower than the one used by Mateos-Garcia et al are aversive to adolescent rats (Philpot et al, 2003). Thus, as stress exposure early in life can increase cocaine self-administration (Lu et al, 2003; Marinelli and Piazza, 2002; Piazza and Le Moal, 1998), an alternative interpretation of Mateos-Garcia et al results is that alcohol increased cocaine self-administration not because of its rewarding effects, but because it induced a chronic stress-like state during adolescence. In conclusion, several studies have tested the gateway hypothesis using CPP and cocaine self-administration procedures in rodents. However, because of methodological and conceptual considerations described above, we conclude that the data in these studies neither support nor refute this hypothesis.

Here, we tested the gateway hypothesis in three experiments in which we used outbred Wistar rats and alcohol-preferring (P) rats and exposed them to alcohol using established alcohol pre-exposure procedures (intermittent access for 20% alcohol or alcohol vapor) for an extended period time (7 weeks) (Roberts et al, 2000; Simms et al, 2008; Wise, 1973); we exposed the rats to alcohol during the late adolescence/early adulthood phase (Figure 1b) (McCutcheon and Marinelli, 2009; Spear, 2015). We then tested the rats for differences in acquisition of cocaine self-administration using an ascending dose–response procedure (Goeders and Guerin, 1994; Stewart et al, 1996) and cocaine dose–response curve after acquisition. Next, we tested whether alcohol pre-exposure increases resistance to extinction, an established measure of drug seeking (Shalev et al, 2002; Venniro et al, 2016), and reinstatement of cocaine seeking induced by discrete cocaine cues (Meil and See, 1996) and cocaine priming injections (de Wit and Stewart, 1981).

Figure 1
figure 1

Experimental timeline, and age of onset of alcohol exposure, and alcohol pre-exposure. (a) Outline of the experimental procedures. (b) Age of onset of alcohol exposure in Exp. 1–3; the chronological timeline is based on McCutcheon and Marinelli (2009). (c) Left panel: alcohol intake (g/kg) in Wistar rats (Exp.1; n=12 per group) during intermittent home-cage access to 20% alcohol. Middle panel: blood alcohol levels (mg/dl) in Wistar rats (Exp. 2; n=12) during alcohol vapor exposure. Right panel: Alcohol intake (g/kg) in P-rats (Exp. 3; n=15) during intermittent home-cage access to 20% alcohol. Data are presented as mean±SEM.

PowerPoint slide

Materials and methods

Overview of Experimental Procedures

We gave Wistar (Exp. 1) or P rats (Exp. 3) intermittent access to water or 20% alcohol in their home cage for 7 weeks. We also exposed another group of Wistar rats (Exp. 2) to air or intermittent (14 h/day) intoxicating levels of alcohol for 7 weeks. We then tested the rats in the different experiments in the following identical sequence of test phases (Figure 1): Acquisition of cocaine self-administration, dose–response curve following the acquisition phase, extinction responding without the discrete cue, cue-induced reinstatement, extinction responding with the discrete cue, and cocaine priming-induced reinstatement (Table 1). For a detailed description of the experimental methods and the specific experiments see Supplementary online Materials and Methods Section.

Table 1 Inactive Lever Presses During the Extinction and Reinstatement Tests (mean±SEM per 1–2 h)

Results

Exp. 1. Wistar Rats: Prior Exposure to Intermittent Access to 20% Alcohol

The timeline of Exp. 1 is provided in Figure 1a. Before testing the rats for acquisition of cocaine self-administration, extinction, and reinstatement of cocaine seeking, we gave the rats intermittent access to water (n=12) or 20% alcohol (n=24) in their home cage for 7 weeks. We then divided the alcohol-exposed rats to two groups based on median split for the last week of drinking: low alcohol intake (mean±SEM of 3.0±0.3 g/kg/24 h, n=12) and high alcohol intake (mean±SEM of 5.8±0.5 g/kg/24 h, n=12) (Figure 1c, Exp. 1).

We also correlated the data for the preference for alcohol over water with the data for alcohol intake based on g/kg during the last week of drinking and these measures were positively correlated (low alcohol intake: r=0.84 p=0.001; high alcohol intake: r=0.55 p=0.07). Despite the lack of a significant correlation between alcohol intake and preference score in the high alcohol intake group, the lowest preference score in the high alcohol intake group was higher than the highest preference score in the low alcohol intake group. Overall, alcohol pre-exposure had no effect on the different measures of cocaine self-administration, extinction responding, and reinstatement of cocaine seeking. In addition, there were no correlations between mean alcohol intake (g/kg) and the different cocaine taking and seeking measures, except for a negative correlation in the high alcohol intake group for the 0.25 mg/kg/infusion dose during acquisition of cocaine self-administration (Supplementary Table S1, Supplementary Information). Below we provide the statistical results for the different experimental phases.

Acquisition of cocaine self-administration and cocaine dose–response after acquisition

Acquisition (left panel). (Figure 2, Exp. 1) The analysis of the number of cocaine infusions included the between-subjects factor of Pre-exposure condition (water, low alcohol intake, high alcohol intake) and the within-subjects factors of Cocaine dose (0.125, 0.25, 0.5, 1.0 mg/kg) and Training session (session 1, 2, and 3 for each dose). This analysis showed significant effects of Cocaine dose (F3,90=8.8, p<0.01), Training session (F2,60=36.4, p<0.01), and interaction between the two factors (F6,180=4.5, p<0.01). There were no significant effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between this factor and the other factors (p-values>0.05).

Figure 2
figure 2

Acquisition of cocaine self-administration and dose–response after acquisition. Left panel: acquisition of cocaine self-administration with ascending doses of cocaine (3 d per dose) in Exp. 1–3. Right panel: within-session dose–response curve after acquisition of cocaine self-administration in Exp. 1–3. Data are mean±SEM of cocaine infusions per 2 h. Exp. 1: n=10–12 per group; Exp. 2: n=8–11 per group; Exp. 3: n=12–14 per group.

PowerPoint slide

Dose–response after acquisition (right panel). The analysis of number of cocaine infusions showed a significant effect of Cocaine dose (F3,87=81.3, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Extinction without cue and cue-induced reinstatement

Extinction without cue (left panel). (Figure 3, Exp. 1) The analysis of number of active lever presses, which included the between-subjects factor of Pre-exposure condition and the within-subjects factor of Extinction session (session 1–9), showed significant effects of Extinction session (F8,216=22.1, p<0.01) and an interaction between Pre-exposure condition and Extinction session (F16,216=2.5, p=0.01), but no significant effect of Pre-exposure condition (p>0.05). The reason for the significant interaction is the somewhat lower responding of the low alcohol intake group in the two first extinction sessions.

Figure 3
figure 3

Extinction responding without cue and cue-induced reinstatement. Left panel: extinction responding without cue over nine extinction sessions in Exp. 1–3. Right panel: cue-induced reinstatement in Exp. 1–3. During testing, lever presses led to contingent presentation of the light cue but not cocaine. The No cue data are the mean of the last three extinction sessions without the cue. Data are mean±SEM of active lever presses per 2 h. Exp. 1: n=10–11 per group; Exp. 2: n=8–11 per group; Exp. 3: n=12–14 per group. # a significant difference between water and low alcohol intake; * a significant difference between water and high alcohol intake (Exp. 1), p<0. 05.

PowerPoint slide

Cue-induced reinstatement (right panel). The analysis of number of active lever presses showed a significant effect of Cue condition (F1,27=14.3, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Extinction with cue and priming-induced reinstatement

Extinction with cue (left panel). (Figure 4, Exp. 1) The analysis of number of active lever presses showed significant effects of Extinction session (session 1–11) (F10, 270=8.9, p<0.01), but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or interaction between the two factors (p>0.05).

Figure 4
figure 4

Extinction responding with cue and drug priming-induced reinstatement. Left panel: extinction responding with cue over 11 extinction sessions in Exp. 1–3. Right panel: priming-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking (0, 2.5, 5, and 10 mg/kg, IP) in Exp. 1–3. During testing, lever presses led to contingent presentation of the light cue but not cocaine. Data are mean±SEM of active lever presses per 2 h for extinction sessions and per 1 h for priming-induced reinstatement. Exp. 1: n=10–11 per group; Exp. 2: n=8–10 per group; Exp. 3: n=12–14 per group.

PowerPoint slide

Priming-induced reinstatement (right panel). The analysis of number of active lever presses showed significant effects of Cocaine priming dose (F3,81=4.4, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Exp. 2. Wistar Rats: Prior Exposure to Alcohol Vapor Chambers

The timeline of Exp. 2 is provided in Figure 1a. Before testing the rats for acquisition of cocaine self-administration, extinction, and reinstatement of cocaine seeking, we gave the rats intermittent access to air (control condition; n=12) or intoxicating levels of alcohol vapor (162.2±10.6 mg/dl; n=12) for 7 weeks (Figure 1c, Exp. 2). Overall, alcohol pre-exposure had no effect on the different measures of cocaine self-administration, extinction responding, and reinstatement of cocaine seeking.

Acquisition of cocaine self-administration and cocaine dose–response after acquisition

Acquisition (left panel). (Figure 2, Exp. 2) The analysis of the number of cocaine infusions included the between-subjects factor of Pre-exposure condition (air, alcohol vapor), the within-subjects factors of Cocaine dose and Training session. This analysis showed significant effects of Cocaine dose (F3,51=6.4, p<0.01), but no effects of Training session or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05). In addition, there were no significant effects of Pre-exposure condition or interactions between this factor and the other factors (p-values>0.05).

Dose–response after acquisition (right panel). The analysis of number of cocaine infusions showed a significant effect of Cocaine dose (F3,51=20.2, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Extinction without cue and cue-induced reinstatement

Extinction without cue (left panel). (Figure 3, Exp. 2) The analysis of number of active lever presses, which included the between-subjects factor of Pre-exposure condition and the within-subjects factor of Extinction session (session 1–9), showed a significant effect of Extinction session (F8,128=4.6, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Cue-induced reinstatement (right panel). The analysis of number of active lever presses showed a significant effect of Cue condition (F1,16=9.4, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Extinction with cue and priming-induced reinstatement

Extinction with cue (left panel). (Figure 4, Exp. 2) The analysis of number of active lever presses showed a significant effect of Extinction session (session 1–11) (F10,150=2.5, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Priming-induced reinstatement (right panel). The initial ANCOVA analysis (inactive lever as a covariate) of number of active lever presses showed no significant effects of Cocaine priming dose, Pre-exposure condition, or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05). However, a post hoc ANOVA of active lever presses (without the inactive lever responses as the covariate) showed a significant effect of Cocaine priming dose (F3,48=11.1, p<0.01) but no significant effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05). The reason for the differences between the ANCOVA with the inactive lever as the covariate and the ANOVA is not clear because the inactive lever presses were very low across the different conditions (0–3.75 presses per 1 h). However, owing to the very low variability in inactive lever presses, the repeated measures ANOVA of inactive lever presses showed a significant effect of Cocaine priming dose (F3,48=3.3, p<0.05), but no significant effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Exp. 3. Alcohol Preferring P rats: Prior Exposure to Intermittent Access to 20% Alcohol

The timeline of Exp. 3 is provided in Figure 1a. Before testing the rats for acquisition of cocaine self-administration, extinction, and reinstatement of cocaine seeking, we gave the rats intermittent access to water (n=14) or 20% alcohol (mean±SEM of 7.2±0.2 mg/kg/24 h; n=15) in their home cage for 7 weeks (Figure 1c, Exp. 3). Overall, alcohol pre-exposure had no effect on the different measures of cocaine self-administration, extinction responding, and reinstatement of cocaine seeking. In addition, there were no significant correlations between mean alcohol intake (g/kg) and the different cocaine measures of cocaine taking and seeking (Supplementary Table S1, Supplementary Information).

Acquisition of cocaine self-administration and cocaine dose–response after acquisition

Acquisition (left panel). (Figure 2, Exp. 3) The analysis of the number of cocaine infusions included the between-subjects factor of Pre-exposure condition (water, alcohol), and the within-subjects factors of Cocaine dose and Training session. This analysis showed significant effects of Cocaine dose (F3,72=4.7, p<0.01), Training session (F2,48=10.6, p<0.01), and an interaction between the two factors (F6,144=2.8, p<0.05). There were no significant effects of Pre-exposure condition or interactions between this factor and the other factors (p-values>0.05).

Dose–response after acquisition (right panel). The analysis of number of cocaine infusions showed a significant effect of Cocaine dose (F3,72=111.0, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Extinction without cue and cue-induced reinstatement

Extinction without cue (left panel). (Figure 3, Exp. 3) The analysis of number of active lever presses, which included the between-subjects factor of Pre-exposure condition and the within-subjects factor of Extinction session (session 1–9), showed a significant effect of Extinction session (F8,184=8.0, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Cue-induced reinstatement (right panel). The analysis of number of active lever presses showed a significant effect of Cue condition (F1,23=13.6, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Extinction with cue and priming-induced reinstatement

Extinction with cue (left panel). (Figure 4, Exp. 3) The analysis of number of active lever presses showed a significant effect of Extinction session (session 1–11) (F10, 230=2.3, p<0.05) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Priming-induced reinstatement (right panel). The statistical analysis of number of active lever presses showed significant effects of Cocaine priming dose (F3,69=6.8, p<0.01) but no effects of Pre-exposure condition or an interaction between the two factors (p-values>0.05).

Comparison of cocaine-taking and cocaine-seeking behaviors across experiments

(Figure 5) Finally, visual inspection of the data in Figures 2, 3, 4 indicate that independent of the alcohol pre-exposure conditions, the rats in Exp. 1–3 showed some differences in their cocaine-taking and cocaine-seeking behaviors in the different experimental phases. In particular, the rats with vapor exposure (Exp. 2) were slower to initiate cocaine self-administration and the P rats (Exp. 3) showed enhanced responding during the extinction and reinstatement tests (see Discussion). Note: we are confident that the rats in Exp. 2 have learned to lever press for cocaine, as indicated by the clear differences between active and inactive lever presses during acquisition (Supplementary Figure S1) and the observations of an orderly cocaine dose–response after training (Figure 2, Exp. 2).

Figure 5
figure 5

Comparison of cocaine-taking and cocaine-seeking behaviors across experiments. (a) Acquisition of cocaine self-administration with ascending doses of cocaine (3 d per dose). (b) Within-session dose–response curve after acquisition of cocaine self-administration. (c) Extinction responding without the cue over nine extinction sessions. (d) Cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. (e) Extinction responding with cue over 11 extinction sessions. (f) Cocaine priming-induced reinstatement. Data are presented as mean±SEM; comparison between Exp.1–3.

PowerPoint slide

Discussion

We have used established rat models of alcohol drinking and dependence (Simms et al, 2008; Vendruscolo and Roberts, 2014), cocaine self-administration (Schuster and Thompson, 1969), and reinstatement of cocaine seeking (Shaham et al, 2003; Venniro et al, 2016) to determine whether different forms of alcohol exposure during the adolescence and young adult phase (Figure 1) will increase subsequent cocaine taking and seeking in Wistar or alcohol-preferring P rats. We found that prior drinking of moderate or higher amounts of 20% alcohol or exposure to intoxicating levels of alcohol (vapor chambers) had no effect on acquisition of cocaine self-administration, dose–response after acquisition, extinction responding, and cue- and cocaine-priming-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Together, our data from established rat models suggest that alcohol exposure during late adolescence/young adulthood does not increase vulnerability to cocaine self-administration and relapse to cocaine seeking during adulthood.

Methodological and Conceptual Considerations

We provided converging data from the different addiction phases (initiation, maintenance, abstinence, relapse), as they are assessed in rat models, that alcohol pre-exposure does not promote cocaine taking and seeking. These data do not support the gateway hypothesis but as with any data from rodent models of drug addiction and relapse, the generality to the human condition should be made with caution (Epstein et al, 2006; Markou et al, 1993). One general issue is that as with most studies using animal addiction models (Heilig et al, 2016), the rats in our study and other preclinical studies inspired by the gateway hypothesis (see Introduction) were socially isolated throughout the experiment (Exp. 1 and 3) or during the cocaine self-administration phase (Exp. 2). In contrast, in humans the initiation of binge drinking during late adolescence and early adulthood is largely driven by social factors (eg, peer pressure, college drinking parties) (Marzell et al, 2015).

Another issue is that we have used a limited access 2-h/day cocaine self-administration procedure for a relatively short time (12 days). Thus, a question for future research is whether prior alcohol exposure will increase vulnerability to cocaine self-administration in animal models that more closely mimic the human condition. These include the extended daily access escalation model (Ahmed, 2011), the unlimited daily access binge model (Bozarth and Wise, 1985; Tornatzky and Miczek, 2000), the DSM-IV-based model in which rats self-administer cocaine for several months (Deroche-Gamonet and Piazza, 2014; Piazza and Deroche-Gamonet, 2013), or cocaine self-administration models in which rats are given intermittent access to cocaine during extended daily training sessions, leading to binge-like drug intake (Roberts et al, 2007, 2013).

From an addiction perspective, another issue for future research is whether alcohol pre-exposure will increase cocaine taking and seeking in rat models of drug self-administration and relapse in which cocaine self-administration is suppressed by adverse consequences (eg, punishment or electric barrier) (Cooper et al, 2007; Pelloux et al, 2007) or in recent animal models of relapse after punishment-imposed or alternative reward choice-based voluntary abstinence (Caprioli et al, 2015; Krasnova et al, 2014; Marchant et al, 2013; Venniro et al, 2016).

Another issue to consider regarding the generality of the findings to the human condition is the homology between the alcohol pre-exposure developmental period in our study and age of onset of alcohol drinking in humans. In this regard, we started the alcohol exposure in the different experiments between PD 44–64, which corresponds to late adolescence and early adulthood developmental periods of the rat (McCutcheon and Marinelli, 2009; Spear, 2015). These developmental periods correspond to the time of onset of binge drinking in the majority of subjects in epidemiology studies (Davoren et al, 2016; Hingson and Zha, 2009), but do not capture the early- and mid-adolescence periods of humans. Thus, our results may not generalize to early developmental periods in which binge drinking is associated with increased risk to develop alcohol addiction in adulthood (Hingson and Zha, 2009). Another issue to consider regarding the generality of the findings to the human condition is that our rat model likely only captures some neurobiological aspects of the gateway hypothesis, and does not capture social, psychological, and legal aspects that can promote the transition from legal to illegal drugs that are beyond the scope of animal models of addiction.

Another issue that deserves some discussion is the finding that independent of the alcohol pre-exposure conditions, the rats in Exp. 1–3 showed some differences in their cocaine-taking and cocaine-seeking behaviors in the different experimental phases. Specifically, the P rats in Exp. 2. showed enhanced responding during the extinction and reinstatement tests. These findings are consistent with a previous study (Le et al, 2006) that reported higher cocaine seeking in alcohol-naive P rats during extinction (with cue) and cocaine priming-induced reinstatement of drug seeking than in alcohol-naive non-preferring (NP) rats. In another study, Katner et al (2011) showed that alcohol-naive P rats will self-administer more cocaine into nucleus accumbens shell than Wistar rats, suggesting that P rats are more sensitive to some of the behavioral effects of cocaine and cocaine cues than Wistar rats. Together, the results from our study and the two studies mentioned above suggest that genetic factors that influence alcohol taking and seeking can also influence cocaine taking and seeking, and thus provide tentative support for the alternative hypothesis of a shared addiction vulnerability (Agrawal et al, 2004).

Another observation in our study was that the rats in Exp. 2 (vapor exposure) were slower to initiate cocaine self-administration (Figure 5a). A possible explanation for this observation is that the rats in Exp. 2 were group-housed during the alcohol (or air) exposure for 7 weeks, whereas the rats in Exp. 1 and 3 were individually housed during this time period. Indeed, several previous studies have shown that group-housing during adolescence decreases cocaine self-administration (Lu et al, 2003; Neisewander et al, 2012).

Finally, in reference to the general notion of the gateway hypothesis—that early legal drug use increases vulnerability to subsequent illegal drug use—we would like to point out that our results may only be relevant to alcohol and cocaine, but not to other combinations of legal and illegal drugs. We are particularly cautious with regard to the generality of our findings to heroin and prescription opioids, because there are many differences between the neurobiological mechanisms of opioids and psychostimulants, and the risk factors and environmental conditions that control addiction to these drug classes (Badiani, 2013; Badiani et al, 2011; Caprioli et al, 2009). In addition, there is evidence that prior exposure to nicotine during adolescence increases cocaine self-administration (Reed and Izenwasser, 2016) and amphetamine self-administration and reinstatement (Cortright et al, 2012).

Neurobiological Perspective

It is well established that the rewarding effects of cocaine, as assessed in the drug self-administration procedure, are mediated by activation of the dopamine projection from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) to the nucleus accumbens (the mesolimbic dopamine system) (Roberts et al, 1977; Wise, 2004). There is also evidence that alcohol exposure increases dopamine cell firing in VTA and release of dopamine in nucleus accumbens (Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988; Gessa et al, 1985). In addition, pharmacological inhibition of dopamine transmission in VTA or nucleus accumbens decreases alcohol self-administration in rats (Hodge et al, 1993; Samson et al, 1993). These findings and the demonstration that rats will self-administer alcohol directly into the VTA (Gatto et al, 1994) have led to the popular notion that drug-induced activation of the mesolimbic dopamine system is critical for alcohol self-administration and alcohol seeking (Engel and Jerlhag, 2014; Soderpalm and Ericson, 2013; Vengeliene et al, 2008).

From the perspective of common neurobiological mechanisms of alcohol and cocaine reward, the lack of effect of alcohol pre-exposure on cocaine-taking behavior in our study are unexpected. However, our findings are in agreement with results from several studies suggesting dissociable mechanisms of alcohol reward vs cocaine reward. One example is that although the literature on the role of mesolimbic dopamine in cocaine self-administration is highly consistent (Pierce and Kumaresan, 2006; Wise, 1996), this is not the case for alcohol self-administration and intake in rat models. For example, Rassnick et al (1993) showed that 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine system has no effect on alcohol self-administration, and Goodwin et al (1996) showed that systemic administration of the D2-family receptor antagonist pimozide has no effect on alcohol consumption; but see Files et al (1998) and Pfeffer and Samson (1988) for positive results with the D2-family receptor antagonists remoxipride and haloperidol.

Another example for dissociable mechanisms of alcohol vs cocaine taking and seeking is midbrain and striatal glial cell line-derived neurotrophic factor and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Ghitza et al, 2010; Logrip et al, 2015; Ron and Barak, 2016). These growth factors provide trophic support to midbrain dopamine neurons (Hyman et al, 1991; Lin et al, 1993; Lindsay et al, 1994; Tomac et al, 1995), and have been shown to promote cocaine seeking (Graham et al, 2007; Grimm et al, 2003; Lu et al, 2004, 2009) but to suppress alcohol seeking (Barak et al, 2011; Carnicella et al, 2008, 2009; He et al, 2005).

A final example is the mu opioid receptor whose activation is critical for alcohol self-administration and intake (Herz, 1997; Sanchis-Segura et al, 2005; Trigo et al, 2010). In contrast, results from many pharmacological studies indicate that activation of this opioid receptor and more generally the endogenous opioid system has a minimal role in cocaine self-administration (Badiani et al, 2011; Ettenberg et al, 1982; Mello and Negus, 1996).

Conclusions

Using multiple measures and procedures, we found no evidence that prior exposure to moderate, high, or intoxicating levels of alcohol increases cocaine self-administration and relapse to cocaine seeking in rat models. Our data do not support the notion that alcohol is a gateway drug for cocaine and suggest that the high co-morbidity seen in humans is likely due to time-independent shared addiction vulnerability, a notion supported by human epidemiological studies (Agrawal et al, 2004; Degenhardt et al, 2010; Tsuang et al, 1998; Vanyukov et al, 2012).

Funding and disclosure

The research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of NIDA.