INTRODUCTION

Disturbances in both emotional and cognitive functions are diagnostic features of major depressive disorder (MDD). Emotional valence and cognitive processes, and their circuitry, are also domains in the research diagnostic classification system for defining core constructs of psychopathology such as depression (Insel et al, 2010). Emotional and cognitive tasks have been found in common to elicit dysregulation of prefrontal circuitry in MDD (Fitzgerald et al, 2006; Steele et al, 2007; Koenigs and Grafman, 2009). Some studies suggest dysregulation of prefrontal circuitry may be ameloriated following response and remission to treatment, and may have a role in the prediction of treatment outcomes (Brassen et al, 2008; Fales et al, 2009; Mayberg, 2009).

Meta-analyses of functional imaging studies in depression have identified the dorsolateral prefrontal (dLPFC) and anterior cingulate (ACC) part of the dorsomedial (dmPFC) regions of the prefrontal cortex affected in depression (Fitzgerald et al, 2006; Koenigs and Grafman, 2009; Hamilton et al, 2012). Despite the convergent evidence of both these prefrontal circuitry dysregulation in MDD, there is no consensus about the specific direction of the changes seen (hypo- or hyperreactivity), and whether the dysfunction reflects a common disruption across both emotional and cognitive tasks, or a more specific pattern that is task dependent. As an example, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies using emotion tasks have reported dLPFC hypoactivation in MDD compared with healthy controls for processing of positive and negative emotion words (Siegle et al, 2002), dLPFC hyperactivation when emotion is induced by happy and sad words (Elliott et al, 2002), and a null effect when emotion is induced by film clips (Beauregard et al, 1998). A similar lack of consistency in the direction of effect has been observed across cognitively demanding tasks; dLPFC hypoactivation during executive functioning (Elliott et al, 1997) and dLPFC hyperactivation during mental arithmetic (Hugdahl et al, 2004). A similarly inconsistent pattern has been observed for the ACC activation: robust ACC hyperactivation for the emotion tasks, but both hypo- and hyperactivation of the ACC during cognitive tasks of executive function (Pizzagalli, 2011). Moreover, understanding the directionality profile of both the dLPFC and ACC regions in relation to each other is critical to understand how emotion and cognition functions are integrated in MDD. Preliminary evidence suggests that this interplay may be critical in clinical efficacy of treatment in MDD (Pizzagalli, 2011; Fox et al, 2012).

Studies to date have used different samples and imaging, task and analysis protocols, making direct comparison of findings difficult. A majority of prior studies have looked at either emotion or cognition functions in separate studies even in the situation they were acquired from the same patient cohort. Meta-analyses suggest that inconsistency in findings is attributable to a lack of standardization in these sample and methodological factors (Fitzgerald et al, 2006). Identifying the specific and common signatures of prefrontal dysregulation across tasks using a standardized approach is needed in order to identify candidate biomarkers for treatment prediction that take the neurobiological expression of MDD into account. Knowing which emotional and/or cognitive function is related to which aspect of prefrontal dysregulation is also needed for the development of novel treatments, and for tailoring them to both clinical subtype and genetic factors.

Our study used fMRI data gathered during a set of five activation tasks designed to engage emotional and cognitive functions. These tasks assess constructs based on an established evidence base and are applicable to theories of depression and treatment (Gordon and Williams, 2010). For example, there is convergent evidence from our group and others that the auditory oddball paradigm captures fundamental functions of selective attention, and recruits prefrontal components of attention circuitry, including ACC and dLPFC (Linden et al, 1999; Williams et al, 2007). The oddball paradigm has also been shown to tap into the attention/concentration features of depression (Kemp et al, 2009) and into treatment response (Kalayam and Alexopoulos, 1999).

Our subjects are drawn from the first 15% of MDD participants as part of the International Study to Predict Optimized Treatment in Depression (iSPOT-D) (Williams et al, 2011). Protocols for imaging acquisition and analysis were implemented in an identical way across tasks and in the same subject sample, and we use the term ‘standardization’ to describe this approach. For example, each functional task sequence was designed to run for 5 min, 8 s, and the pre- and post-processing pipeline was applied in the same way to all task data. Using this standardized approach, which removes the contribution of task protocol, processing and sample characteristic variations, we sought to identify what aspects of prefrontal hypo- and hyperactivation are consistent for MDD relative to controls across domains of cognitive and emotion processing. This approach is analogous to the standardized battery of fMRI protocols used for presurgical mapping (for example, identifying the language network, sensorimotor strip, etc) (Deblaere et al, 2002). In presurgical mapping, multiple functional paradigms with overlapping results are seen as adding confidence in assessing neurosurgical risk by convincingly mapping the brain’s functional architecture. Complementary efforts have been applied towards developing a task battery in other psychiatric applications such as schizophrenia. For example, in schizophrenia, a cognitive neuroscience context has been used in the ‘CNTRICS’ initiative to identify behavioral paradigms that implicate core functions and neural circuits for use with neuroimaging (Barch et al, 2009; see http://cntrics.ucdavis.edu). These paradigms are selected to be suitable for neuroimaging, for elucidating the brain circuit biomarkers of impaired cognition in the disorder. This study is a first application of using this approach in understanding depression and has potential implications in identifying predictors of treatment response and development of novel targets for treatment.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Thirty MDD participants who provided fMRI data at Westmead Hospital (a University of Sydney Medical School hospital) as baseline data for the iSPOT-D study were included in this analysis. This cohort represents the first 15% of all MDD participants estimated to undergo testing as part of the iSPOT-D study at this site (a total 200 MDD patients, ages 18–65 years old are estimated to be tested). Data for 30 healthy participants matched for sociodemographics were also included in the analysis for comparison.

A complete description of the study assessments, inclusion/exclusion criteria and diagnostic procedures is available in Williams et al, 2011. In short, the primary diagnosis of nonpsychotic MDD was confirmed using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (Sheehan et al, 1998), according to DSM-IV criteria, and a score ⩾16 on the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD17) (Hamilton, 1960). All MDD participants were either antidepressant medication-naive or, if previously prescribed an antidepressant medication, had undergone a wash-out period of at least five half-lives. Healthy control participants were extensively screened for the absence of Axis I disorders (using the MINI) and for an HRSD17 score ⩽7, and they could not meet the DSM-IV criteria for recurrent or non-recurrent MDD or other psychiatric disorders.

This study was approved by the institutional review board and was conducted according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki 2008. After study procedures were fully explained in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the institutional review board, participants provided written informed consent.

Activation Tasks

During magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), all participants performed activation tasks that assessed cognitive (selective attention using oddball task; sustained attention and working memory using continuous performance task; and impulsivity and inhibition processes using Go-NoGo task) and emotional processes (emotion processing at and below level of conscious awareness). Following the goal of standardization, these tasks were designed to be equivalent to the cognitive thinking and emotion tasks undertaken with event-related potential recording in the iSPOT-D protocol (Williams et al, 2011). The total scan time for each activation task was standardized to 5 minutes and 8 seconds.

Details of the cognitive tasks (oddball task, continuous performance task, Go-NoGo task) and emotion tasks (unmasked conscious emotion processing task, masked nonconscious emotion processing task) are presented in Table 1 and Supplementary Figure 1.

Table 1 Details of the fMRI Task Protocol

Image Acquisition

MRI was performed using a 3.0 Tesla GE Signa HDx scanner (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, Wisconsin). Acquisition was performed using an 8-channel head coil. MR images for each functional task were acquired using echo planar imaging (EPI) MR sequence with the following parameters: TR=2500 ms, TE=27.5 ms, matrix=64 × 64, FOV=24 cm, flip angle=90°. Forty contiguous axial/oblique slices with a slice thickness of 3.5 mm were acquired to cover the whole brain in each volume. For each activation task, 120 volumes were collected with a total scan time of 5 minutes and 8 seconds. Three dummy scans were acquired at the start of every acquisition. Structural MRI 3D T1-weighted images were acquired in the sagittal plane using a 3D spoiled gradient echo (SPGR) sequence (TR=8.3 ms; TE=3.2 ms; flip angle=11°; TI=500 ms; NEX=1; ASSET=1.5; Frequency direction: S/I). A total of 180 contiguous 1 mm slices were acquired covering the whole brain with a 256 × 256 matrix with an in-plane resolution of 1 × 1 mm, resulting in 1 mm3 isotropic voxels. The 3D SPGR sequence was collected for use in normalization of the fMRI data to standard space.

Functional MRI Data Analysis

The fMRI data was preprocessed and analyzed using SPM8 software (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Motion correction was performed by realigning and unwarping the fMRI images to the first image of each task run. A mean image for the fMRI time series was generated and was normalized to the T1-weighted structural scan using boundary-based registration technique (Greve and Fischl, 2009). This registration technique has been shown to be more accurate and robust for registering EPI fMRI data in comparison to existing normalized mutual information and correlation ratio registration techniques. The T1-weighted data was also normalized to standard space using the FMRIB non-linear registration tool (FNIRT) (Andersson et al, 2007a, 2007b). Normalization warps from these two steps were stored for use in functional to standard space transformations. Global signal was estimated using a mask within the ventricles and white matter and was removed from the motion-corrected fMRI time series. fMRI data was smoothed using an 8 mm Gaussian kernel and high-pass filtered using a cutoff period of 128 s. For the cognitive tasks, a canonical hemodynamic response function convolved event-related model was used to model the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) responses: oddball (target and nontarget trials), continuous performance (working memory, 1-back and baseline trials) and Go-NoGo tasks (Go and NoGo trials). For the emotion tasks, a hemodynamic response convolved box car function was used to model the BOLD response for each of the emotion blocks.

In the first-level fixed-effect analysis, the following contrast images were derived. Cognitive tasks: oddball—targets vs nontarget (to assess selective attention function); continuous performance—working memory vs baseline (to assess working memory function); (Our primary condition of interest was working memory. Subsequent papers will report on this task in more detail, including the sustained attention vs baseline contrast. This latter contrast may be considered equivalent to the target vs nontarget contrast in the oddball task, and is not of primary interest in this study.); Go-NoGo—NoGo vs baseline (to assess response inhibition) (For this task, the primary interest in this study was the inhibition impulsivity construct (assessed by noGo), not the automatic motor responding (Go).). Emotion tasks: negative emotion (anger, fear, sad, disgust) vs neutral (to assess emotion reactivity to negative stimuli), and positive emotion (happy) vs neutral (to assess response to positive/reward stimuli). Individual contrast images were normalized to standard space using the normalization warps estimated in the preprocessing steps above. The standard space contrast maps were then entered for all second-level random effects analyses.

To test our hypothesis of prefrontal dysfunction with MDD, we used a region of interest (ROI) analysis approach. Five ROIs were selected and comprised of 5 mm radius spheres: three in the dLPFC centered at (34, 46, 32), at (43, 41, 14) and at (−37, 33, 23); and two in the dmPFC in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) centered at (−6, 30, 12) and at (3, 22, 20). The center coordinates were selected based on two separate previous meta-analysis findings of most consistent MDD dysfunction in comparison to controls (Fitzgerald et al, 2006; Hamilton et al, 2012). A P value of 0.05 (FWE corrected for multiple comparisons) was used to assess significant findings. For the significant ROIs, summary β values for the whole ROI were also extracted and were tested for correlations with symptom severity (HRSD17 scores), behavioral (reaction time) data and between tasks for both MDD and control groups. To test for functional differences between MDD and control groups in other brain regions, voxel-wise whole brain analyses were also performed and are reported in the Supplementary section.

RESULTS

Sociodemographic information for the MDD and control groups is summarized in Table 2.

Table 2 Characteristics of the Samples; Sociodemographic, Diagnostic and Clinical

Behavioral Data Analysis

Behavioral data (reaction time and accuracy of performance on task) were available only for the cognitive tasks (All participants completed the task inside and outside the scanner session. For participants whose data was not successfully logged during the scanning session, we used their data from outside the session. The same task was used and there were no differences in mean responses for the group according to inside or outside scanner.). The emotion tasks required active attention but not concurrent behavioral responses, given that one condition involved nonconscious processing of stimuli.

For both groups, accuracy was ⩾97% on each task, with the only significant difference between the MDD and control groups for the oddball task (P=0.045, MDD with more errors than controls). In contrast, MDD participants were significantly slower than controls on each of the three tasks (oddball, P=0.003: MDD=394±73 ms, controls=346±42 ms; continuous performance, P<0.001: MDD=604±142 ms, controls=479±87 ms; Go-NoGo, P=0.002: MDD=312±62 ms, Controls=258±67 ms).

For the MDD group, reaction times were not significantly correlated with the severity of depressive symptoms on the HRSD17. For accuracy on tasks, the number of errors on the continuous performance task were significantly correlated with HRSD17 (r=0.478, P=0.007). Scatter plots for both these measures with severity of depressive symptoms are provided in Supplementary Figure 2.

fMRI Data Analysis

MDD participants were found to have significant functional differences in the prefrontal ROIs across cognitive and emotion processing tasks (see Figure 1 and Table 3). For cognitive tasks, MDD participants showed a significant hyperactivation in the ACC during working memory updating in the continuous performance task and for response inhibition on the Go-NoGo task. We also observed hypoactivation in the right dLPFC during working memory updating. For emotion tasks, MDD participants also showed hypoactivation in the right dLPFC for explicit viewing of negative emotion. For explicit viewing of positive emotion, hypoactivation was observed in the dorsal ACC. MDD patients did not differ from controls in prefrontal activation for selective attention in the oddball task.

Figure 1
figure 1

Prefrontal regions (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (a and b); dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (c and d)) with significant differences between the MDD and control group. Figure shows location of each region of interest and the corresponding plots show the contrast estimates (mean±SE) for each group. R-dLPFC, right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; ACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex.

PowerPoint slide

Table 3 Prefrontal Functional Differences between MDD and Control Participants

Correlation of fMRI With Symptom Severity, Behavioral Data and Across Tasks

The profiles of dLPFC hypoactivation across tasks, dmPFC hyperactivation for cognitive tasks and dmPFC hypoactivation for positive emotion in MDD were not found to correlate significantly with current clinical symptom severity (HRSD17) or reaction time performance on the cognitive tasks.

There was a between task correlation in activation, in which greater activation in the dmPFC during working memory was significantly correlated with less activation in the dLPFC during explicit viewing of negative emotion (r=−0.502, P=0.005) specifically for the MDD group (Figure 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Scatter plot showing correlation of dmPFC (ACC) activation during working memory (Y-axis) with the right dLPFC activation during conscious processing of negative emotion (X-axis). R-dLPFC, right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; ACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

In this study, we assessed the use of a standardized fMRI battery to define which prefrontal circuits contribute in common to cognitive and emotion-related dysfunctions in MDD, and which reflect specific contributions in direction and location of effect. In MDD patients, the right dLPFC showed a common profile of hypoactivation across cognitive tasks assessing working memory, and across emotion tasks for both explicit viewing of negative facial emotions. By contrast, the medial prefrontal circuit in MDD was differentiated by type of task, with hyperactivation during cognitive tasks, but hypoactivation during explicit viewing of positive emotion.

Prefrontal circuits are key components of the functional network involved in regulating emotional reactivity and appraising salient emotional input (Davidson, 2002). Our findings are consistent with impairments in both regulation and appraisal of emotion in MDD, mediated by the ACC and dLPFC (Hamilton et al, 2012). Within the emotion regulation and appraisal networks, the amygdala and hippocampus together have the role of processing and maintenance of the emotional aspects of incoming information, and the prefrontal cortex has a predominantly regulatory role over these limbic regions. The ACC has direct connections to the amygdala (Johansen-Berg et al, 2008), whereas the dLPFC exerts a control over contextual processing of salient limbic amygdala-hippocampal inputs via the orbitofrontal regions (Davidson, 2003).

Our finding of dLPFC hypoactivation in MDD for explicit viewing of negative emotion suggests a reduction in top down appraisal of these negative emotional inputs, consistent with meta-analysis (Hamilton et al, 2012). Hypoactivation of the dLPFC may arise from a failure in relaying sensory information for the negative stimulus from the brain salience networks to the dLPFC for contextual appraisal—resulting in an insufficient neural response from this region. On the other hand, ACC hypoactivation for positive emotion suggests that a loss of ACC-mediated regulation of reward related inputs, which may contribute to anhedonic features of depression (Phillips et al, 2003).

Prefrontal circuits are also key to the performance of day-to-day executive functions like attention and memory recall (Miller and Cohen, 2001). The dLPFC dysfunction has been previously shown linked to impaired cognitive processing in MDD (Fitzgerald et al, 2006; Koenigs and Grafman, 2009). Our finding of dLPFC hypoactivation during working memory updating in MDD is consistent with this previous evidence. Here, dLPFC hypoactivation was observed specifically in the right hemisphere. We note that MDD patients did not show significant dLPFC dysfunction during attention in the oddball task or for response inhibition in the Go-NoGo task. In MDD, working memory processing demands may be more suited to identify deficits in cognitive control mechanisms relying on the lateral prefrontal circuitry in comparison to the attention or motor inhibition tasks. Previous studies using both tasks, but including an affect ‘distractor’ component, have successfully elicited functional differences in both these types of cognitive processing for MDD in this region (Wagner et al, 2006; Wang et al, 2008). Task performance during cognitive processing is also suggested to be a factor influencing the activation profile of the dLPFC (Thomas and Elliot, 2009). In this regard, enhanced cortical function is necessary for normal cognitive performance, whereas impaired performance is associated with reduced function. Although we see both reduced dLPFC function and impaired performance on cognitive task (seen as longer response times) for the MDD participants as a group, dLPFC function was not found significantly correlated to performance.

The ACC on the other hand is suggested to be the key region for integrating cognitive and emotional functions. Our findings of ACC hyperactivation in MDD for both working memory and response inhibition tasks replicates previous reports of corresponding hyperactivation during working memory (Fitzgerald et al, 2008; Schöning et al, 2009) and tasks assessing similar inhibitory control functions (Wagner et al, 2006; Langenecker et al, 2007). Increased recruitment of the ACC may reflect an increased load on depressed patients for cognitive processing. Hyperactivity of the ACC may also reflect the inability of MDD participants to deactivate the default mode network and successfully engage the cognitive control task positive network (dLPFC region)—hence poor performance on cognitive tasks. This is supported by evidence of medial prefrontal suppression linked to successful cognitive performance (Anticevic et al, 2010). Previous work suggests that the increased self-referential processes with depression are core in the failure to downregulate the default mode activity (Sheline et al, 2009). The BOLD responses for both the working memory and response inhibition tasks in our study show that controls were successfully able to disengage from the default mode state (seen as deactivation or negative response for the ACC—Figure 1). ACC functional activation during working memory was also found significantly correlated with the dLPFC activation during emotion processing, suggesting that the functional link between these two prefrontal cortices (in addition to those with the limbic circuitry) may be important to understand the cognitive–emotion interaction underlying the symptomology of MDD.

A limitation of this study is our focus on the prefrontal cortex as a first step in comparing across a standardized suite of tasks. This means that implications for limbic circuitry are speculative and require further specific study. The current study is also limited by the sample size used for analyses and warrants caution in the interpretation of our findings. Also our methodological approach was constrained to search regions of interest. Replication in more empirically driven analyses using a larger MDD cohort is needed. However, this analysis approach shows that previous meta-analytical findings of areas of prefrontal dysfunction in MDD are extendable to our participant cohort. Primarily, the convergent results across all tasks highlight the potential benefits of using suites of standardized protocols (crafted using existing theoretical frameworks) to identify and evaluate the robustness of results in psychiatric disorders.

Future Directions

For the iSPOT-D cohort, data will subsequently be available to test hypotheses regarding connectivity, including those that use other types of imaging data such as diffusion tensor imaging. This work will build on work that used multiple tasks to show common deficits in brain activation that underlie cognitive dysfunction in MDD (Fitzgerald et al, 2008; Halari et al, 2009) and reduced frontal–limbic relationships in MDD (Siegle et al, 2007). iSPOT-D will also provide a platform to replicate the present findings in a larger patient cohort, examine a priori subtypes of MDD and integrate the brain imaging data with genotypes. Post-treatment data will also be available, allowing us to test whether response to antidepressant medication normalizes prefrontal dysfunction in MDD. Previous studies of single tasks suggest that normalization would occur (Brassen et al, 2008; Fales et al, 2009). The present findings provide a sound basis to test whether hypoactivation vs hyperactivation of the prefrontal cortex contributes to the prediction of treatment response.