INTRODUCTION

Long-term potentiation (LTP) is a form of synaptic plasticity widely accepted as one of the initial events required for memory encoding. Accordingly, LTP is impaired in animal models of aging associated with neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's or Huntington's diseases (Auffret et al, 2010; Rasmussen et al, 1996; Balkowiec and Katz, 2002; Rex et al, 2005; Simmons et al, 2009). However, when healthy aging is concerned, LTP is found to be impaired (Barnes, 1979; Rex et al, 2005), not altered (Costenla et al, 1999; Rex et al, 2005; Kumar et al, 2007), or even increased (Costenla et al, 1999; Kumar and Foster, 2004; Huang and Kandel, 1996). The magnitude of the deficits may depend on the synaptic circuit that is being potentiated (Rex et al, 2005) or on the used stimulation pattern (Costenla et al, 1999). Thus, at a given pathway, as the CA3–CA1 synapses in the hippocampus, LTP induced by strong high-frequency stimulus is not affected by aging (Costenla et al, 1999; Rex et al, 2005), whereas LTP induced by the weaker θ-burst stimulus is enhanced by aging (Costenla et al, 1999; Huang and Kandel, 1996).

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is a neurotrophin that acts as a regulator of synaptic plasticity in the adult brain. It was shown that hippocampal LTP is strongly impaired in BDNF (Korte et al, 1995; Figurov et al, 1996; Patterson et al, 1996) knockout mice, a deficit that could be rescued by increasing BDNF expression (Korte et al, 1996), or even by administration of exogenous BDNF (Patterson et al, 1996). In addition, in BDNF TrkB receptor knockout mice, LTP is also severally affected (Xu et al, 2000; Minichiello et al, 2002). Moreover, upregulation of BDNF rescues synaptic plasticity deficits in an animal model of Huntington's disease (Simmons et al, 2009). These positive actions of BDNF upon synaptic plasticity, being it endogenous (Balkowiec and Katz, 2002) or exogenously added (Fontinha et al, 2008), are particularly favored in weak θ-burst stimuli in the CA1–CA3 synapse. The influence of this fast-acting neurotrophin on synapses requires co-activation of adenosine A2A receptors (A2AR) (Diógenes et al, 2004; Fontinha et al, 2008; Tebano et al, 2008) and this interaction is markedly influenced by aging (Diógenes et al, 2007). Neuromodulation through A2AR is increased upon aging (Rebola et al, 2003). Therefore, higher levels of A2AR seen in aged animals facilitate synaptic effects of BDNF (Diógenes et al, 2007), in spite of lower levels of BDNF receptors in the hippocampus upon aging.

Given that (1) age-related increases in LTP induction (Kumar and Foster, 2004) are mostly evident when LTP is triggered by weak θ-bust stimulus (Costenla et al, 1999, 2) this weaker pattern of LTP induction favors the facilitatory action of BDNF upon LTP (Fontinha et al, 2008), and (3) the effects of BDNF are potentiated or dependent on A2AR activation (Sebastiao and Ribeiro, 2009), which are more relevant in aged subjects, we hypothesized that the enhanced LTP in aged rats could be due to an higher facilitatory influence of BDNF. To test this hypothesis, we evaluated age-related changes in magnitude of LTP induced by weak θ-burst stimuli and tested the influence of endogenous and exogenously added BDNF upon LTP in hippocampal slices taken from infant (4 weeks) to aged (1.7 years) rats. In addition, we also evaluated how the effect of BDNF upon LTP in the different age groups was affected by tonic A2AR activation, and whether the change in LTP upon aging impacts upon alteration in learning and memory.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Experiments were performed in rat (male, Wistar, Harlan Interfauna Iberica, SL, Barcelona, Spain) hippocampi from infant rats (4 weeks old) and from three adult rat groups: young adult (10–15 weeks), age where reproductive behavior is fully established (Havenaar et al, 1993), old adult (36–38 weeks), time where rats get the third part of life expectancy (Havenaar et al, 1993), and aged rats (70–80 weeks old). Animals were handled according to European Community guidelines and Portuguese law on Animal Care and anesthetized with halothane before decapitation. Two hippocampi were isolated, one for electrophysiological recordings and the other one to evaluate BDNF immunocontent.

Ex vivo electrophysiological recordings. The hippocampus was dissected free within ice-cold Krebs’ solution composed of (mM): NaCl 124; KCl 3; NaH2PO4 1.25; NaHCO3 26; MgSO4 1; CaCl2 2; and glucose 10, previously gassed with 95% O2 and 5% CO2, pH 7.4. Slices (400 μm thick) were cut perpendicularly to the long axis of hippocampus with a McIlwain tissue chopper and allowed to recover functionally and energetically for 1 h in a resting chamber, filled with same solution, at room temperature. Slices were transferred to a recording chamber for submerged slices and continuously superfused at 3 ml/min with gassed bathing solution at 32°C; drugs were added to this superfusion solution in an open circuit. When the effect of BDNF scavenger (Trk-Fc) was studied, slices were continuously superfused at same rate, temperature, and oxygenation, but in a closed circuit. Appropriate controls have been performed to ensure that there were no time-dependent changes in field excitatory post-synaptic potentials (fEPSPs) under either the open or the closed circuit conditions. Recordings were obtained with an Axoclamp 2B amplifier and digitized (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Individual responses were monitored, and averages of eight consecutive responses continuously stored on a personal computer with the LTP software (Anderson and Collingridge, 2001).

Basal synaptic transmission. fEPSPs (Figure 1b) were recorded through an extracellular microelectrode (4 M NaCl, 2–6 MΩ resistance) placed in stratum radiatum of CA1 area (Figure 1a). Stimulation (rectangular 0.1 ms pulses, once every 15 s) was delivered through a concentric electrode placed on the Schaffer collateral–commissural fibers, in stratum radiatum near CA3–CA1 border (Figure 1a). The intensity of stimulus (80–200 μA) was initially adjusted to obtain a large fEPSP slope with a minimum population spike contamination. Alteration in synaptic transmission induced by drugs was evaluated as the % change in the average slope of the fEPSP in relation to the average slope of the fEPSP measured during the 10 min that preceded the addition of drugs as described previously (Diógenes et al, 2004).

Figure 1
figure 1

Extracellular recordings in hippocampal slices. Schematic representation of a hippocampal transverse slice preparation showing the recording configuration used to obtain extracellular responses in the CA1 dendritic layer (stratum radiatum) evoked by stimulation of two (a) separate sets of the Schaffer pathway (S1 and S2). (b) Examples of representative trace obtained after stimulation are composed of the stimulus artifact (1), followed by the pre-synaptic volley (2) and the field excitatory post-synaptic potentials (fEPSP) (3). A schematic representation of the stimulation paradigms used in plasticity experiments is represented in (c).

PowerPoint slide

LTP induction and quantification. fEPSPs (Figure 1b) were recorded through extracellular microelectrode (4 M NaCl, 2–6 MΩ resistance) placed in stratum radiatum of CA1 area as represented in Figure 1a. In LTP experiments, stimulation (rectangular 0.1 ms pulses, once every 10 s) was delivered alternatively to two independent pathways through bipolar concentric electrodes placed on Shaffer collateral/commissural fibers in stratum radiatum (Figure 1a). LTP was induced by θ-burst protocol consisting of three trains of 100 Hz, three stimuli, separated by 200 ms as shown in Figure 1c.

The intensity of the stimulus was maintained during the induction protocol. LTP was quantified as the % change in the average slope of the fEPSP taken from 46 to 60 min after LTP induction in relation to the average slope of the fEPSP measured during the 14 min that preceded the induction of LTP. In each individual experiment, the same LTP-inducing paradigm was delivered to each pathway. At 1 h after LTP induction in one of the pathways, BDNF (20 ng/ml) was added to the superfusion solution and LTP was induced in the second pathway, no less than 30 min after BDNF perfusion and only after stability of fEPSP slope values was observed for at least 10 min. The effect of BDNF upon LTP was evaluated by comparing the magnitude of LTP in the first pathway in the absence of BDNF (control pathway), with the magnitude of LTP in the second pathway in the presence of BDNF (test pathway); each pathway was used as control or test on alternate days. To test the modification of the effect of BDNF upon LTP, the modulatory drugs were added to superfusing bath at least 30 min before induction of LTP in the first pathway and remained in bath up to the end of the experiment. BDNF was added, as usual, 60 min after the induction of LTP in the first pathway. Thus, modulatory drugs were present during both LTP-inducing periods, whereas BDNF was only present during the second induction of LTP. This protocol allows the comparison between the effect of BDNF upon LTP under different experimental conditions, keeping the magnitude of LTP under the same drug condition, without BDNF in the same slice as an internal control. When testing the effect of a drug upon LTP (rather than the modulation of the BDNF effect on LTP), the drug was added to bath 30 min before induction of LTP in the second pathway and the magnitude of the resulting LTP was compared with that previously obtained (first pathway) in the absence of the drug (see, Fontinha et al, 2008). In control experiments performed with hippocampal slices taken from any age group, we confirmed that when the θ-burst paradigm was sequencially applied to each of the two pathways in the absence of any drug, the synaptic potentiation obtained in the first pathway was similar to that achieved in the second pathway.

Input–output curve. Input–output curves in slices from different age groups were performed to ensure that modifications in LTP magnitude were not due to changes in basal synaptic transmission. After obtaining a stable baseline for at least 15 min, the stimulus delivered to the slice was decreased until disappearance of the fEPSPs. The stimuli delivered to the slice were successively increased by steps of 20 μA. For each stimulation condition, data from three consecutive averaged fEPSP (each averaged fEPSP is the computerized mean of eight individual fEPSP) were stored. The range of all inputs delivered to the slice was typically from 60 μA to a supra-maximum stimulation amplitude of 300 μA. The input–output curve was plotted as the relationship of fEPSP slope vs stimulus intensity (ie, fiber volley amplitude), which provides a measure of synaptic efficiency.

Western Blotting

Sodium dodecyl sulfate-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis was used to evaluate levels of BDNF immunoreactivity. Hippocampi were disrupted in sucrose-Tris solution with a Teflon pestle. Proteins on hippocampal homogenate were quantified using Bio-Rad assay according to Bradford (1976). A total of 70 μg of protein, for each sample, were separated on 12% polyacrylamide gels, and then transferred onto nitrocellulose membranes. After blocking with 5% milk solution, the membranes were incubated (overnight at 4°C) with primary rabbit polyclonal antibodies recognizing BDNF or α-tubulin (Abcam), and then incubated with anti-rabbit secondary antibody conjugated with horseradish peroxidase for 1 h at room temperature. Proteins of interest were detected using ECL-Plus (Amersham Biosciences). Intensities of bands were quantified using ImageJ program. The BDNF intensity values were divided by correspondent α-tubulin intensities (loading control).

Morris Water Maze

Young adult or aged rats (eight animals in each age group) were first habituated to handling during 5 days before testing began. The protocol used was the classical Morris water maze test, which is sensitive to hippocampal-dependent spatial learning and memory (Morris et al, 1982). The maze consisted of a large circular tank (1.8 m in diameter, 0.6 m in height) of water (temperature, 25±1°C) made opaque with the addition of non-toxic water-based black paint. An escape platform (10 cm in diameter) was submerged 1 cm below the water. Several visual cues were placed on the walls of the testing room, to be used by the animals as spatial references. An automated tracking system (Smart 2.5, PanLab, Barcelona, Spain) monitored all performances in the following parameters: swim path length; escape latency; average speed; and percentage of time spent in each quadrant. All subjects were first given a pre-training habituation session of two trials in which they were placed in the tank and allowed 60 s to swim to the platform located at the center of the tank. Rats were then given spatial (acquisition) training consisting of four trials per day for 5 days, in which the platform was placed at a fixed position in the center of one of the four quadrants of the tank (platform Q, left, right and across). The starting position, at which subjects were placed in the tank facing the wall, was randomly chosen across trials between four possible locations. Inter-trial interval was 15 min, during which animals were towel-dried and placed in a heated incubator (25°C) to prevent hypothermia. The maximum trial duration was 60 s, after which animals were manually guided to the platform if they failed to locate it. Once animals reached the platform, they were allowed to remain there for 20 s. A probe test was given after the last trial on day 5 of the acquisition. In this test, the platform was removed from the tank and animals were allowed to swim freely for 60 s, during which the percentage of time spent on each quadrant was recorded. The results are expressed as mean±SEM from n experiments.

Drugs

BDNF was supplied in a 1.0 mg/ml stock solution in 150 mM NaCl, 10 mM sodium phosphate buffer, and 0.004% Tween-20, provided by Regeneron Pharmaceuticals (Tarrytown, NY). 2-[p-(2-carboxyethyl)phenethylamino]-5-N-ethylcarboxamido adenosine (CGS21680) was purchased from Sigma (St Louis, MO). K252-a and 7-(2-phenylethyl)-5-amino-2-(2-furyl)-pyrazolo-[4,3-e]-1,2,4-triazolo[1,5-c]pyrimidine (SCH58261) were from Tocris Cookson (Ballwin, MO). The recombinant human TrkB/Fc chimera, Trk-Fc, was purchased from R&D systems (Minneapolis, MN). CGS21680 and SCH58261 were made up in a 5 mM stock solution in DMSO. K252-a was made in a 2.5 mM stock solution in DMSO. TrkB-Fc was made up in a 50 μg/ml stock solution in PBS with 0.1% BSA. The concentration of DMSO added to slices (0.001% (v/v)) was below the concentration that influences glutamatergic synaptic transmission (0.02% (v/v); Tsvyetlynska et al, 2005). Aliquots of stock solutions were stored at −20°C until use.

Data Analysis

Values are expressed as mean±SEM from n slices. The significance of differences between the mean values obtained in two different conditions in same slice was evaluated by the Student's t-test. For multiple comparisons, statistical significance was assessed by the one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni's correction. Values of P<0.05 were considered to represent statistically significant differences.

For results obtained in Morris water maze, a two-way ANOVA repeated measures was used to compare acquisition data from the two age groups. A Student's t-test was used to compare time spent in the platform quadrant between groups.

RESULTS

LTP Induced by θ-Burst Stimulus, but not Synaptic Efficiency, is Increased upon Aging

The mild θ-burst stimulation used in this work induced a small, but statistically significant (P<0.05 as compared with baseline), LTP in slices taken from 4-week-old rats (10±3.6% increase in fEPSP slope 46–60 min after θ-burst, n=8; Figure 2a) and 10 to 15-week-old rats (14±1.8% increase in fEPSP slope, n=7; Figure 2b). In contrast, in hippocampal slices taken from older rats, the same θ-burst elicited a robust LTP, so that 46–60 min after θ-burst stimulation, the fEPSP slope was increased by 35±4.6% (36 to 38-week-old rats, n=7; Figure 2c) or 43±4.9% (70 to 80-week-old rats, n=7; Figure 2d). As summarized in Figure 2e, LTP magnitude attained in slices from the two oldest groups was not significantly different, but it was significantly different (P<0.01) from that attained in slices from younger animals (4 weeks or 10–15 weeks). The fEPSP recorded from slices from old animals tended to have more population spikes than in fEPSP recorded in younger animals, and this was particularly evident after LTP induction (Figure 2a–d). Indeed, a higher LTP in these slices increases the probability to detect population spike contamination. A lower GABAergic inhibition in aged rat hippocampus (Billard et al, 1995) might also contribute to higher population spikes.

Figure 2
figure 2

θ-Burst induced long-term potentiation (LTP) but not synaptic efficiency, which is increased upon aging. Left panels in (a–d) show averaged time courses changes in field excitatory post-synaptic potential (fEPSP) slope induced by a θ-burst stimulation in the absence of any drug. The ordinates represent normalized fEPSP slopes, where 0% corresponds to the averaged slopes recorded for 14 min before θ-burst stimulation: −0.61±0.06 mV/ms, n=8 (a), −0.58±0.06 mV/ms, n=7 (b), −0.69±0.07 mV/ms, n=7 (c), and −0.69±0.07 mV/ms, n=7 (d), and the abscissa represents the time of recording. Representative traces from representative experiments are shown in the right panels in (a–d); each trace is the average of eight consecutive responses obtained before (1) and 46–60 min after (2) LTP induction, and is composed of the stimulus artifact, followed by the pre-synaptic volley and the fEPSP. Panel (e) depicts the magnitude of LTP (change in the fEPSP slope at 46–60 min) induced by θ-burst stimulation in relation to pre-θ-burst values (0%) in the absence of any drugs in hippocampal slices taken from the different rat ages as indicated below each column. *P<0.01 and **P<0.001 (one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the Bonferroni's correction). All values are mean±standard error of mean (SEM). In (f) are represented input–output curves obtained in hippocampal slices of young adult rats (10–15 weeks old ()) and aged rats (70–80 weeks old (•)). Input–output curves are displayed as the relationship between fEPSP slope (ordinates) and stimulus intensity (measured as the amplitude of the pre-synaptic volley, in the abscissa) in the two age groups. After obtaining a stable baseline for at least 15 min, the input delivered to the slice was decreased until the slope of the fEPSP was zero. Afterwards, the current delivered to the slice was increased by steps of 20 μA at the time, with three data points collected at each stimulation amplitude (each data point being the average of eight individual fEPSP). The range of all the input delivered to the slice was typically from 60 μA to a supra-maximum intensity amplitude of 300 μA. For each age group, the data are mean±SEM (both of fiber volley and fEPSP slope in each data point) of n=3 slices taken from different animals.

PowerPoint slide

To evaluate if the age-related differences in LTP magnitude could be due to changes in baseline synaptic efficiency, input–output curves in slices taken from 10 to 15- and 70 to 80-week-old rats were performed. Increasing stimulus intensities were delivered to the slices, as described in Materials and methods. For each intensity applied, the slope of the fEPSP (taken as a measure of the strength of the post-synaptic response) was plotted against the fiber volley amplitude (which reflects the level of pre-synaptic activation). No differences were found in input–output curves obtained in slices taken from both age groups (n=3, P>0.05; Figure 2f), indicating that the age-related changes in LTP cannot be accounted by differences in synaptic efficiency.

Aged Rats Show Deficits on Hippocampal-Dependent Learning and Memory, when Compared with Young Adult Rats

To evaluate whether the increased LTP observed in aged rats was accompanied by enhanced skills at hippocampal-dependent learning and memory tasks, we compared the performance of young adult (10–15 weeks old) and aged (70–80 weeks old) rats at the Morris water maze. As shown in Figure 3a, animals of both age groups improved their daily performance during acquisition phase of the Morris water maze task, but aged rats showed a much slower learning profile. ANOVA revealed significant differences in latency throughout the acquisition days (F(4.58)=29.9, P<0.01, n=8; Figure 3a) and an overall effect of age (F(1.58)=12.74, P<0.01, n=8). In probe test, both young and aged rats spent a significantly higher percentage of time in the training quadrant. However, young rats spent significantly more time (56.5±3.7% n=8) in the training quadrant than aged rats (43.4±3.7%, P<0.01, n=8; Figure 3b), which indicates that aged rats have a deficient spatial memory compared with young rats. The repeated measures ANOVA has revealed a consistent pattern across all the four quadrants regardless of the age factor (P>0.05).

Figure 3
figure 3

Comparison of the performance by young and aged rats in the spatial learning protocol of the Morris water maze. (a) Learning curve of the mean escape latencies (±standard error of mean (SEM)) over the 5 days of acquisition training for young () and aged (•) animals (n=8). *P<0.01 between the groups, two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) repeated measures test; (b) percentage of time spent in each quadrant in hidden platform test (probe test), which was performed after the acquisition period. *P>0.01, Student's t-test.

PowerPoint slide

In Aged Rats Tonic Influence of Endogenous BDNF upon LTP is Enhanced

Weak θ-burst stimulation favors the release of BDNF (Balkowiec and Katz, 2002; Gartner and Staiger, 2002) as well as the facilitatory action of this neurotrophin upon LTP (Fontinha et al, 2008). To investigate whether the age-related increase in LTP magnitude could be related to a higher influence of endogenous BDNF, we evaluated the influence of BDNF's scavenger, TrkB-Fc (2 μg/ml), upon LTP in hippocampal slices from 10 to 15 and 70 to 80-week-old rats. TrkB-Fc (2 μg/ml) was added to hippocampal slices 30 min before θ-burst stimulation and remained in the bath up to the end of experiment in a close circuit as described in Materials and methods. Under these conditions and in the absence of the scavenger LTP magnitude in slices taken from young adult rats was 19±14% (Figure 4b and e, n=3, P<0.05 as compared with baseline) and in slices from 70 to 80-week-old rats it was 59±14% (Figure 4d and e, n=3, P<0.05 as compared with baseline), therefore within the same values as that found in the first group of experiments (Figure 2). The scavenger was devoid of effect on hippocampal synaptic transmission in slices taken from either group of animals (Figure 4a and c, n=3). Furthermore, TrkB-Fc (2 μg/ml) did not change LTP magnitude (Figure 4b and e, n=3, P>0.05 as compared with LTP magnitude in the absence of the scavenger) in slices taken from 10 to 15-week-old rats. However, as shown in Figure 4d and e, TrkB-Fc (2 μg/ml) significantly decreased (P<0.05) the magnitude of LTP in slices taken from aged rats. Note, however, that under those conditions, a small but significant LTP (22±5.5%, n=3, P<0.05 as compared with baseline) still occurred in slices from aged rats, the magnitude being similar to that observed in slices from young adults (Figure 4e).

Figure 4
figure 4

The brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) scavenger, TrkB-Fc, decreases the long-term potentiation (LTP) magnitude in slices taken from 70 to 80-week-old rats, but not in slices taken from 10 to 15-week-old rats. Panels (a) and (c) illustrate virtual absence of changes in field excitatory post-synaptic potential (fEPSP) slope when TrkB-Fc (2 μg/ml) was applied to hippocampal slices taken from 10 to 15 (a) or from 70 to 80- (c) week-old rats. Panels (b) and (d) show averaged time course changes in the fEPSP slope induced by the θ-burst stimulation in the absence () or in the presence (•) of TrkB-Fc (2 μg/ml) in slices taken from 10 to 15 (b) or from 70 to 80- (d) week-old rats. Ordinates represent normalized fEPSP slopes, where 0% corresponds to the averaged fEPSP slopes recorded for 10 min immediately before TrkB-Fc, and were as follows: −0.43±0.01 mV/ms (n=3, a) and −0.53±0.10 mV/ms (n=3, c) or corresponds to the averaged slopes recorded for 14 min before θ-burst stimulation: −0.49±0.05 mV/ms, n=4 (b, •), −0.50±0.06 mV/ms, n=4 (b, ), −0.55±0.01 mV/ms, n=3 (d, •), and −0.59±0.02 mV/ms, n=3 (d, ). The abscissas represent the time of every recordings. Representative traces from a representative experiment are shown below panel (d); each trace is the average of eight consecutive responses obtained before (1 and 3) and 46–60 min after (2 and 4) LTP induction, and is composed of the stimulus artifact, followed by the pre-synaptic volley and the fEPSP. Panel (e) depicts the magnitude of LTP (change in the fEPSP slope at 46–60 min) induced by θ-burst stimulation in relation to pre-θ-burst values (0%) in the absence of any drugs (control) and in the presence of TrkB-Fc (2 μg/ml) in slices from 10 to 15 week and 70 to 80-week-old rats. *P<0.05 (paired Student's t-test). All values are mean±standard error of mean (SEM).

PowerPoint slide

To further evaluate the contribution of endogenous BDNF to θ-burst-induced LTP in the two age groups (10 to 15 and 70 to 80-week-old rats), we tested the effect of an inhibitor of Trk phosphorylation, K252a (Knusel and Hefti, 1992). K252a was used at a concentration (200 nM) known to prevent BDNF actions on hippocampal synaptic transmission (eg, Kang and Schuman, 1995; Diógenes et al, 2004) and plasticity (Fontinha et al, 2008), without directly affecting LTP in slices from infant rats (Fontinha et al, 2008). The inhibitor of Trk phosphorylation was added to superfusing bath at least 30 min before induction of LTP in the second pathway, remaining in the bath up to the end of experiment, and was devoid of effect upon synaptic transmission in slices from either young adult (Figure 5a) or aged (Figure 5c) rats. Also, and in accordance with previous observations in infant rats (Fontinha et al, 2008), K252a (200 nM) did not significantly (P>0.05) affect LTP magnitude in slices taken from 10 to 15-week-old rats (Figure 5b and e). In contrast, in slices taken from older (70–80 weeks) rats, K252a (200 nM) significantly decreased LTP (Figure 5d and e, n=3, P<0.05), in agreement with data obtained with BDNF scavenger (Figure 4), which reinforces the conclusion that endogenous BDNF is involved in the enhancement of LTP upon aging. It is worth noting that in the absence of K252a, LTP magnitude in these experiments was statistically significant (P<0.05) either in slices from young adult (20±7.1%, n=3) or aged (48±8.5%, n=3) rats, and that similarly to what occurred in the presence of TrkB-Fc, in the presence of K252a, a small but significant LTP (21±3.0%, n=3, P<0.05 as compared with baseline) was still detected in slices from aged rats (Figure 5d and e).

Figure 5
figure 5

The inhibitor of Trk phosphorylation, K252A, decreases the long-term potentiation (LTP) magnitude in slices taken from 70 to 80-week-old rats, but not in slices taken from 10 to 15-week-old rats. (a) and (c) show averaged time courses changes in field excitatory post-synaptic potential (fEPSP) slope induced by K252a (200 nM) when applied to hippocampal slices taken from 10 to 15 (a) or from 70 to 80- (c) week-old rats. (b) and (d) show averaged time courses changes in fEPSP slope induced by the θ-burst stimulation in the absence () or in the presence (•) of K252a (200 nM) in slices taken from 10 to 15 (b) or 70 to 80- (d) week-old rats. Ordinates represent normalized fEPSP slopes, where 0% represents the averaged fEPSP slopes recorded for 10 min immediately before K252a administration, and were −0.72±0.11 mV/ms, n=4 (a) and −0.72±0.07 mV/ms, n=4 (c) or represent normalized fEPSP slopes, where 0% corresponds to the averaged slopes recorded for 14 min before θ-burst stimulation: −0.74±0.01 mV/ms, n=4 (b, •), −0.62±0.02 mV/ms, n=4 (b, ), −0.55±0.01 mV/ms, n=3 (d, •), and −0.69±0.02 mV/ms, n=3 (d, ). The abscissa represent the time of every recordings. Panel (e) depicts the magnitude of LTP (change in the fEPSP slope at 46–60 min) induced by θ-burst stimulation in relation to pre-θ-burst values (0%) in the absence of any drugs (control) and in the presence of K252a (200 nM) in slices from 10 to 15 and 70 to 80-week-old rats. All values are mean±standard error of mean (SEM). *P<0.05 (paired Student's t-test).

PowerPoint slide

The age-related modifications of endogenous BDNF levels might be confined to synaptic areas as no significant differences (P>0.05, n=5) between BDNF immunoreactivity on hippocampal homogenates from the four age groups were detected (Figure 6).

Figure 6
figure 6

Levels of endogenous brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the different age groups. In (a) are shown the western blots of BDNF (14 kDa) and β-tubulin (55 kDa, loading control), in homogenates of rat hippocampus taken from 4, 10 to 15, 36 to 38, and 70 to 80-week-old rats, as indicated above each lane. In (b) are shown the averaged of BDNF levels (n=4–5). All values are mean±standard error of mean (SEM). The levels of BDNF in infant (4 weeks old) rats were taken as 1. The hippocampus homogenates were taken from the same animals that were used in electrophysiology experiments.

PowerPoint slide

Changes in Effect of Exogenous BDNF upon LTP are Age-Related Dependent

We next evaluated whether any effect of exogenously added BDNF upon LTP was also influenced by age. BDNF (20 ng/ml) was added to slices 60 min after LTP induction in first pathway and 30 min before LTP induction in second pathway, and remained in the bath up to the end of experiments. As illustrated in Figure 7a, in hippocampal slices from young animals (4 weeks old), BDNF caused a marked facilitation of LTP, in accordance with previous reports (Fontinha et al, 2008). In hippocampal slices taken from 10 to 15-week-old rats, the θ-burst stimulus applied in the presence of BDNF also induced a robust LTP (33±3.2% increase in fEPSP slope, n=5; Figure 7b), which was significantly higher (P<0.05; Figure 7e) than that obtained in the other pathway in the absence of BDNF (13±1.8% increase in fEPSP slope).

Figure 7
figure 7

Age-dependent effects of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (20 ng/ml) on θ-burst induced long-term potentiation (LTP). Panels (a)–(d) show averaged time courses changes in field excitatory post-synaptic potential (fEPSP) slope induced by the θ-burst stimulation in the absence () or in the presence (•) of BDNF (20 ng/ml) in hippocampal slices taken from 4 (a), 10 to 15 (b), 36 to 38 (c), and 70 to 80- (d) week-old rats. BDNF (20 ng/ml) was applied 60 min after the induction of LTP in the first pathway () and at least 30 min before induction of LTP in the second pathway (•). The ordinates represents normalized fEPSP slopes, where 0% corresponds to the averaged slopes recorded for 14 min before θ-burst stimulation: −0.61±0.06 mV/ms, n=4 (a, ), −0.52±0.06 mV/ms, n=4 (a, •), −0.58±0.06 mV/ms, n=5 (b, ), −0.57±0.06 mV/ms, n=5 (b, •), −0.69±0.07 mV/ms, n=4 (c, ), −0.68±0.05 mV/ms, n=4 (c, •), −0.69±0.07 mV/ms, n=4 (d, ), and −0.68±0.07 mV/ms, n=4 (d, •), and the abscissa represents the time of every recordings. All values are mean±standard error of mean (SEM). Panel (e) depicts the magnitude of LTP (change in the fEPSP slope at 46–60 min) induced by θ-burst stimulation in relation to pre-θ-burst values (0%) in the absence of any drugs (open bars), or in the presence of BDNF (20 ng/ml, filled bars) alone. Note that BDNF significantly increases LTP magnitude in hippocampal slices taken from 4 (n=4) and 10 to 15- (n=5) week-old rats, but not in slices taken from 36 to 38 (n=4) and 70 to 80- (n=4) week-old rats. **P<0.005 and *P<0.05 (paired Student's t-test) as compared with absence of BDNF in the same experiments (adjacent open column to the left).

PowerPoint slide

In contrast, when applied to hippocampal slices taken from 36 to 38-week-old rats, BDNF did not cause a further increase in LTP (Figure 7c and e). Indeed, θ-burst stimulation induced a 42±7.0% increase in fEPSP slope (n=4) in the presence of BDNF and 37±7.4% increase in fEPSP slope, in the same slices without applying BDNF (P>0.05). Similar results were obtained with the oldest group (70–80 weeks old) of rats, where the presence of BDNF also did not induce any further significant increase in LTP magnitude (37±1.4 or 35±3.5% increase in fEPSP slope 60 min after the θ-burst in the presence or absence of BDNF, respectively, n=4, P>0.05; Figure 7d and e).

It is worth noting that magnitude of LTP in slices taken from 36 to 38 or 70 to 80-week-old rats under control conditions (no BDNF added) is similar to that observed in slices taken from 10 to 15-week-old rats, but in the presence of BDNF.

The effect of BDNF upon LTP, whenever observed (4 weeks and 10 to 15-week-old rats), was prevented by blockade of A2AR with the selective A2AR antagonist, SCH58261 (100 mM; Figure 8), which is in conformity with previously reported influence of A2AR upon the effect of BDNF on synaptic transmission (Diógenes et al, 2004, 2007; PMID: 17421024) and LTP (Fontinha et al, 2008).

Figure 8
figure 8

The antagonist of adenosine A2A receptors, 7-(2-phenylethyl)-5-amino-2-(2-furyl)-pyrazolo-[4,3-e]-1,2,4-triazolo[1,5-c]pyrimidine (SCH58261), prevents the effect of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) upon θ-burst-induced long-term potentiation (LTP). Panels (a) and (c) show averaged time courses changes in field excitatory post-synaptic potential (fEPSP) slope induced by a θ-burst stimulation in the absence () or in the presence (•) of BDNF (20 ng/ml) in experiments where adenosine A2A receptors (A2AR) had been blocked by the selective antagonist, SCH58261 (100 nM). The antagonist was applied 30 min before the LTP induction in the first pathway () and remained in the bath up to the end of the experiment being therefore present during LTP induction in the first and second pathways. BDNF (20 ng/ml) was added (•) 60 min after the induction of LTP in the first pathway () and remained up to the end of the experiment. The ordinates represents normalized fEPSP slopes, where 0% corresponds to the averaged slopes recorded for 14 min before θ-burst stimulation: −0.52±0.03 mV/ms, n=3 (a,), −0.55±0.08 mV/ms, n=3 (a,•), −0.54±0.03 mV/ms, n=4 (c,), and −0.63±0.07 mV/ms, n=4 (c,•), and the abscissa represents the time of recordings. Panels (b) and (d) depict the magnitude of LTP (change in the fEPSP slope at 46–60 min) induced by θ-burst stimulation in relation to pre-θ-burst values (0%) in the absence of any drugs (control), in the presence of BDNF (20 ng/ml) alone, in the presence of SCH 58261 (100 nM) alone, or BDNF (20 ng/ml) together with SCH 58261 (100 nM) as indicated below each column. All values are mean±standard error of mean (SEM). *P<0.05 (one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the Bonferroni's correction).

PowerPoint slide

Influence of A2AR Blockade upon θ-Burst-Induced LTP is Aged-Related Dependent

As the effect of BDNF upon LTP requires co-activation of adenosine A2AR (Figure 8 and Fontinha et al (2008)), blockade of A2AR should markedly reduce the magnitude of LTP in old animals if, as data with the BDNF scavenger and Trk phosphorylation inhibitor (Figures 4 and 5) suggested, the boosted LTP at these ages results from an enhanced action of endogenous BDNF upon LTP. To test directly that hypothesis, we compared the effect of the A2AR antagonist upon LTP in hippocampal slices taken from the different age groups. SCH58261 was used at a concentration (100 nM) selective for rat A2AR (Zocchi et al, 1996), and was added to perfusion solution 60 min after induction of LTP in the first pathway and at least 30 min before LTP induction in the second pathway. As sumarized in Figure 9, the A2AR antagonist significantly (P<0.05) reduced LTP magnitude in slices from 36 to 38 and 70 to 80-week-old rats, but was virtually devoid of effect on LTP in slices from younger animals (4 and 10–15 weeks). Interestingly, in the presence of SCH58261 (100 nM), LTP magnitude in aged rats (36 weeks old) was similar to that observed in younger animals either in the presence or absence of SCH58261 (Figure 9).

Figure 9
figure 9

Age-dependent effects of 7-(2-phenylethyl)-5-amino-2-(2-furyl)-pyrazolo-[4,3-e]-1,2,4-triazolo[1,5-c]pyrimidine (SCH58261) (100 nM) effect on θ-burst-induced long-term potentiation (LTP). Panels (a–d) show averaged time course changes in fEPSP slope induced by the θ-burst stimulation in the absence () or in the presence (•) of SCH58261 (100 nM) in hippocampal slices taken from 4 (a), 10 to 15 (b), 36 to 38 (c), and 70 to 80- (d) week-old rats. SCH58261 (100 nM) was applied (•) 60 min after the induction of LTP in the first pathway. The ordinates represent normalized fEPSP slopes, where 0% corresponds to the averaged slopes recorded for 14 min before θ-burst stimulation: −0.56±0.03 mV/ms, n=4 (a, ), −0.57±0.03 mV/ms, n=4 (a, •), −0.56±0.01 mV/ms, n=3 (b, ), 0.52±0.06 mV/ms, n=3 (b, •), −0.59±0.06 mV/ms, n=3 (c, ), −0.65±0.09 mV/ms, n=3 (c, •), −0.60±0.12 mV/ms, n=3 (d, ), and −0.59±0.05 mV/ms, n=3 (d,•), and the abscissa represents the time of recordings. Panel (e) depicts the magnitude of LTP (change in the fEPSP slope at 46–60 min) induced by θ-burst stimulation in relation to pre-θ-burst values (0%) in the absence of any drugs (control) or in the presence of SCH58261 (100 nM). Note that SCH58261 significantly decreased LTP magnitude in hippocampal slices taken from 36 to 38 (n=3) and 70 to 80- (n=3) week-old rats, but not in slices taken from 4 (n=4) and 10 to 15- (n=3) week-old rats. All values are mean±standard error of mean (SEM). *P<0.05 (paired Student's t-test) as compared with absence of SCH58261 in the same experiments (adjacent open column to the left).

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

The main finding of this work is that the higher LTP observed after θ-burst stimulation upon aging is a consequence of an increase in facilitation of LTP by endogenous BDNF. However, the increased LTP does not translate into improved spatial memory performance of the aged animals.

It is classically established that LTP in the hippocampus is a prototypical experimental model that translates into forms of learning and memory associated with that brain area (Lynch, 2004), including spatial memory as assessed by the Morris water maze (Morris et al, 1982). However, LTP magnitude and memory should not be linearly related, as higher LTP in old animals does not necessarily translate into improved memory upon aging. As pointed out by Huang and Kandel (1996), who observed an age-related increase of LTP induced by a brief 1 Hz paired pulse stimulation, there might be forms of LTP that are negatively coupled with age-related memory loss and that may, therefore, have a negative role in memory storage. On the other hand, changes in LTP can vary in different areas of the dendritic tree as age-related decreases in basal but not apical dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons have been shown (Lynch et al, 2006). It is therefore possible that the now reported age-dependent increase in weak θ-burst-induced LTP of apical dendrites of CA1 neurons reflects, at least in part, LTP failure in some other anatomical areas contributing to the learning and memory paradigms under assay. Clearly, memory impairment in aging is not solely related to a loss of synaptic plasticity (see also, Lynch et al, 2006).

It is known that in old rats there is no loss of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells (Rapp and Gallagher, 1996; Rasmussen et al, 1996), that most biophysical properties of old pyramidal cells do not differ from those in young cells, and that there is no change in spontaneous firing rates of single cells in the hippocampus of freely behaving old rats comparing with young ones (see, Barnes, 2003). With aging there is a decline in the expression of mRNA for different NMDA receptors subunits (Magnusson, 2000; Bai et al, 2004) as well as a decrease in several NMDA subunits (Sonntag et al, 2000; Clayton et al, 2002), which might contribute to a decrease in LTP magnitude upon aging. However, LTP involves many steps besides NMDA receptors activation. We focused on the possibility that the neuromodulatory influence of BDNF on LTP could be affected by aging because the age-related increases in susceptibility to LTP induction (Kumar and Foster, 2004) are more evident when LTP is triggered by weak θ-burst stimuli (Costenla et al, 1999), which favors the release of endogenous BDNF (Gartner and Staiger, 2002; Aicardi et al, 2004) as well as facilitatory actions of exogenous BDNF on LTP (Kramar et al, 2004; Fontinha et al, 2008). Our results clearly show that the BDNF scavenger, Trk-Fc, and the inhibitor of Trk phosphorylation, K252a, caused a marked decrease in LTP in slices from old but not from young rats, compatible with previous observations that early LTP in young animals is not dependent on endogenous BDNF signaling (Fontinha et al, 2008) and suggestive that upon aging there is an enhanced component of LTP that is dependent on endogenous BDNF. Interestingly, in the presence of these inhibitors of endogenous BDNF action, the magnitude of LTP in young or aged animals becomes similar. Furthermore, upon addition of exogenous BDNF, LTP magnitude in slices from young animals is markedly increased becoming comparable to that observed in slices from aged animals.

Taken together, these results strongly suggest that the age-related increases in susceptibility to LTP induction by weak θ-burst stimuli involves an exacerbation of the facilitatory action of endogenous BDNF.

BDNF release can occur through two different pathways: (1) a constitutive pathway, where this molecule is spontaneously secreted or (2) a regulated pathway, where it is secreted in response to depolarization-induced transient changes in the Ca2+ concentration at release sites (see, Lessmann et al, 2003). Therefore, secretion of neurotrophins is regulated by neuronal activity being dependent on the stimulus frequency (Lim et al, 2003) and facilitated by θ-burst stimulation (Gartner and Staiger, 2002; Aicardi et al, 2004). This leads to fast and highly localized changes in synaptic BDNF levels that might not reflect changes in total tissue levels of the neurotrophin. Indeed, we could not detect appreciable age-related changes in the levels of BDNF in hippocampal homogenates. Hence, the higher influence of endogenous BDNF upon LTP in aged animals might be due to an increased activity of the regulated pathway leading to an enhanced release of BDNF at synapses, accompanied by an enhanced release of adenosine, which facilitates the action of BDNF in spite of the age-dependent decline in TrkB receptors (Diógenes et al, 2007). Upon aging, there is a deficit in the regulation of calcium concentration (Landfield and Lynch, 1977), leading to increased levels of intracellular calcium in neurons (Hajieva et al, 2009). Whether this is a cause for an increase in BDNF release through the regulated pathway requires future investigation.

Whether the enhanced action of endogenous BDNF is a compensatory phenomenon physiologically aimed to increase LTP and ameliorate learning and memory deficits or, in alternative, whether the enhanced action of BDNF is itself the source of an age-related increased but dysfunctional LTP remains to be established. Interestingly, in the dentate gyrus, BDNF-induced signaling and plasticity is impaired in aged rats (Gooney et al, 2004), suggesting that deficits in spatial learning could be due to deficits in BDNF/TrkB signaling in other hippocampal areas or even in other brain regions. As discussed above, memory impairment in aging is not solely related to a loss of synaptic plasticity, and surely, spatial learning does not rely exclusively on LTP in the CA1 hippocampal area.

Synaptic plasticity is under tight control by adenosine inhibitory A1 receptors (A1R) and facilitatory A2AR (see eg, Costenla et al, 2010). In has been repeatedly observed that synaptic influences of BDNF are dependent on A2AR receptors activation (Diógenes et al, 2004, 2007; Fontinha et al, 2008; Tebano et al, 2008; Assaife-Lopes et al, 2010), and we could confirm these observations in this work, expanding them to LTP in aged animals. Therefore, the marked LTP inhibition by a selective A2AR antagonist in aged but not young animals may be due, at least in part, to blockade of the component of LTP that is dependent on endogenous BDNF. The density of A2AR (Cunha et al, 1995; Lopes et al, 1999; Diógenes et al, 2007) as well as their coupling to G proteins (Lopes et al, 1999; Rebola et al, 2003; Rodrigues et al, 2008) is increased in the hippocampus of aged animals. Age-induced impact on density and function of A2AR may underlie the preventive effects of caffeine in age-related memory impairments in rodents (Prediger et al, 2005; Costa et al, 2008), as it is mimicked by A2AR antagonists, but not by A1R antagonists (Costa et al, 2008). Here we report that an A2AR antagonist has a marked inhibitory effect on LTP in aged rats where endogenous BDNF actions seems to be also more evident and where impaired hippocampal-dependent memory was detected as assessed by the Morris water maze. One may therefore speculate that the positive actions of A2AR antagonists, including caffeine, upon memory in aged subjects can, at least in part, be related to an inhibition of dysfunctional higher tonic actions of BDNF upon plasticity.

The animals used in this study were undergoing a non-pathological aging process, rather than being a model of neurodegenerative disease, such as Alzheimer's disease where total BDNF levels in the cortex and hippocampus are known to be decreased (Connor et al, 1997; Ferrer et al, 1999). Interestingly, in animal models of Alzheimer's disease, LTP is markedly decreased (Chapman et al, 1999; Oddo et al, 2003; Gureviciene et al, 2004; Trinchese et al, 2004), reinforcing the idea of an important role for a fine-tune regulation of synaptic BDNF levels for proper LTP and, eventually, for memory function. In what concerns influence of BDNF upon LTP, it should be stressed that not only the amount, but also the timing and pattern of BDNF release are crucial for shaping the actions of BDNF at synapses (Ji et al, 2010).

In conclusion, we show for the first time that, in a situation of non-pathological aging, the actions of BDNF are increased upon synaptic plasticity in aged animals. As no changes in total hippocampal levels of this neurotrophin where detected, this enhanced effects are probably due to an increased tonic action of BDNF upon high-frequency neuronal firing.