Introduction

Next-generation sequencing has decreased the cost of and time required for genome-scale testing and has increased the potential for sequencing any child’s genome. As both the predictive validity and the diagnostic validity of genome-scale testing technologies have increased, discussion about their application—for example, newborn screening—has increased in pediatrics1 and public health contexts.2 These approaches produce information about a broad range of conditions potentially relevant to the child’s and their family’s health. Consequently, genome-scale testing may result in a corresponding increase in the amount of secondary genetic information potentially available to parents and clinicians, requiring stakeholders to make difficult decisions about what types of information should be disclosed.

Research suggests that a majority of parents are interested in the hypothetical possibility of obtaining genomic sequencing results for their newborns3, 4, 5 or young children,5 but preferences for research results decline in number when characterized on dimensions related to actionability, severity, age of onset, or likelihood.6 What remains unclear from previous research is if and how preferences for this information vary when jointly characterized along these dimensions. Understanding how parents consider this information could help clinicians support parents or could be used to inform educational and decision support strategies when genome-scale testing is used in clinical practice or research.7

To address this gap, we elicited parental preferences for learning health-related information about a young child through genome-scale testing. Specifically, we examined parents’ interest in receiving their child’s test results related to non-medically actionable conditions. Information about the health conditions that could be revealed by the results was characterized along multiple dimensions, such as age of onset, level of disability the condition could cause, and the likelihood that the condition would develop given a true-positive test result. The choice of dimensions was informed by models that describe systems for categorizing incidental findings8 and by factors relevant to child and family health, such as disability or quality of life. A guiding assumption of our approach is that the value parents place on learning genome-scale test results is influenced by multiple characteristics of the health conditions for which a child may be found to be at increased risk. We also examined whether subgroup differences exist based on gender or race, as these could signal the importance of communicating differently with parental subgroups to support decision making.9

We conducted a discrete-choice experiment (DCE) to quantify how parental preferences for learning a child’s genome-scale test results are affected by differences in health condition characteristics. DCE is a type of conjoint analysis that assumes that individuals value “products” (in this case, test results) based on their features. A recent review article found 179 health-related DCEs published between 2009 and 2012.10 This method is being used more frequently to study complex health decision making, and has been applied in studying adult preferences for learning incidental genomic information.11, 12

Materials and methods

The study protocol received institutional review board review and approval from the RTI International's Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects. Study design and analyses were conducted following current guidance for conducting DCEs in health contexts.13, 14, 15, 16

Study sample

Respondents were eligible to participate in the study if they lived in the United States, were English-speaking adults aged 18 to 40, self-identified as either white or black, and had at least one child aged 5 years old or younger in the home. Qualtrics—an online data collection company based in Provo, UT—hosted the survey and recruited the sample from their proprietary online subject panel. The recruitment procedures were not intended to draw a probability-based sample from the full population of adults meeting the eligibility criteria.

Because sample size calculations can present a challenge in choice experiments, minimum sample size depends on multiple criteria, including the question format, the complexity of the choice task, the desired precision of the results, and the need to conduct subgroup analyses.17 Most published choice experiments have a sample size between 100 and 300 respondents.18 The sample size for this survey was set at 1,200, with the goal of recruiting a sample split between women and men and black and white respondents.

Survey instrument

The primary goal was to elicit parental preferences for health-related information about a child that could be learned through genome-scale testing. Specifically, we examined parents’ interest in learning their children’s test results that would indicate a significant risk for genetic health conditions that were not medically actionable, defined in the survey as conditions leading to mental and/or physical disabilities for which effective preventive treatments are not currently available, although there may be interventions to improve quality of life over the course of the condition or to relieve symptoms.

Based on a literature review19 and input from clinicians, genetic counselors, and geneticists at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC), we developed our DCE using an initial set of attributes and attribute levels. These attributes and levels were then refined using input from ten cognitive interviews conducted with parents of children under the age of 5 years. The final survey allowed us to measure the relative importance of seven attributes: (i) penetrance, (ii) typical age of onset of the health condition, (iii) level of mental disability that the health condition may cause, (iv) level of physical disability that the health condition may cause, (v) rate of progression (the speed with which the health condition may worsen), (vi) availability of options to improve quality of life, and (vii) impact on life span (Table 1). Each choice question comprised two generically labeled profiles—profile A and profile B—and participants chose the profile that would be more important for them to know (see Figure 1 for a sample choice question).

Table 1 List of attributes and attribute levels used in constructing the discrete-choice experiment
Figure 1
figure 1

Example of a choice question from the discrete-choice experiment.

Because the total number of all possible pairwise combinations of the attribute levels is large, an experimental design program was used to create a smaller set of profiles and choice questions. The simplified design provides enough information to accommodate both main-effects and interaction-effects models; this approach has been shown to optimize the design when it is unclear if interaction effects exist between attributes in the model.20 In this study, a variation of the Fedorov21 algorithm was used to construct an orthogonal fractional factorial experimental design with 1,147 treatment profiles. Respondents were randomly assigned to any of 99 test blocks, each composed of eight distinct unordered profile pairs. Consequently, each respondent completed a series of eight choice questions. This design was statistically efficient in maximizing the amount of information gathered from respondents.

The final survey was designed for participants who may have limited genetic knowledge. Respondents were first asked to read background information defining several basic genetic concepts used throughout the survey. The survey then described the attributes and levels that were included in the DCE. Using Likert-type scales ranging from 1 (least distressing) to 7 (most distressing), respondents were asked to rate how distressing it would be for them to learn information for each level of the seven attributes. For the attribute describing the chance that the condition will develop, the chance (or risk) was presented both numerically and graphically. The probability levels were presented graphically using a 100-square grid, in which each square represented one person with a gene identifying a genetic disorder. Squares presented in color represented people for whom the condition will develop. Research suggests that pictograms such as risk grids are an effective method of communicating risk.21

Respondents reviewed an example risk grid and completed a comprehension question. Respondents who answered incorrectly were presented with the correct answer and an explanation before proceeding with the survey, whereas respondents who answered correctly were presented with the correct answer to reinforce their understanding.

The attribute levels were presented in a scale array with one level per row (Figure 1). The study started with two practice trials involving two and four attributes, respectively. The survey also collected basic demographic information and respondents’ personal experience with genetic disorders and testing, their mistrust in the health-care system,22 objective numeracy,23 and intolerance of uncertainty.24 We included the last three of these measures as potential moderators in exploratory analyses, but did not retain them in the final models because we found no evidence that they influenced preferences. The median time respondents took to complete the survey was 15 min.

Statistical analysis

We analyzed the data using a conditional logistic regression model,25 using Stata 13.0.26 We ran a main-effect random parameters logit model27 for comparison, and the results in terms of significance and ranking of attribute levels were identical to the conditional logit. The dependent variable was coded “1” if respondents selected a test-result profile and “0” if they did not for each of the test-result profiles in the eight choice questions. The independent variables corresponded to the attribute levels for the test-result profiles. The attribute levels were coded as categorical variables using effect coding. With effect coding, zero indicates the mean effect across all attribute levels, rather than the omitted level, as in dummy coding.28 This procedure produces a parameter estimate for all level categories, where the omitted-category parameter is the negative sum of the included-category parameters. Conditional logit regression uses choice information to estimate the relative contributions of different attribute levels to participant preferences. In a choice set comprising two alternative test-result profiles, respondents are assumed to choose the option they view as providing greater utility. In turn, the choices respondents make are functions of differences in the attributes of alternative test-result profiles. When they are aggregated over many choices, we are able to measure respondents’ relative preferences for the various attribute levels.

The regression coefficients from a conditional logit model are statistical estimates of the average relative contribution across the sample of each attribute level to the likelihood of choosing one test-result profile over another, with larger coefficients indicating greater relative preference for that level. We also calculated the relative importance of each attribute over the range of levels by subtracting the coefficient associated with the least preferred level of each attribute from that associated with the most preferred level, dividing by the sum of ranges over all attributes and multiplying by 100. Changes in attributes displaying a wider range from the most to least preferred level are more important in predicting parents’ choices and have a greater impact on determining the kinds of test results parents consider important to know. We estimated a main-effects model of preferences for the attribute levels of the genome-scale test results for the full sample. Interaction terms were included to test for subgroup interactions testing attribute preferences by the gender and race of participants. In these models, gender and race variables were dummy coded so that women and white participants served as the reference category.

We conducted a descriptive analysis of the distress items using SPSS 21.0.29 We used one-way repeated measures ANOVAs (analyses of variance) with a Greenhouse-Geisser correction to determine if the mean level of distress varied across levels within an attribute. We also conducted post-hoc tests with the Bonferroni correction for pairwise comparisons of levels. We conducted paired t tests for the two attributes that had only two levels.

Results

The survey was conducted online from 24 August 2014, through 8 September 2014. Qualtrics screened a total of 3,032 respondents, 2,021 of whom met the study eligibility criteria. Of these, 671 did not complete the survey or were excluded after screening because their relevant subgroup quota had already been met. To ensure that only respondents who read and answered survey materials in the expected time allotted were included in the analyses, we removed an additional 55 respondents who completed the survey in less than 6 min. Finally, six respondents were excluded from the analyses because they completed fewer than eight discrete-choice tasks.

The final sample included 1,289 adult parents with young children living in the United States, for a minimum cooperation rate of 62.0%. By design, approximately half the respondents were women (n = 643, 49.9%) and half were black (n = 638, 49.5%). The majority of respondents were married (n = 762, 59.1%), non-Hispanic (n = 1,132, 87.8%), and had completed at least some college (n = 984, 76.4%). The median age was 30 years (M = 29.8, standard deviation [SD] = 5.6). About half the respondents (n = 648, 50.3%) had two children under the age of 18 years. Among the sample, 61% (n = 783) had never personally had a genetic test, nor did they know of a family member or someone close who had had a genetic test. The demographic characteristics of the study sample are shown in Table 2. When compared to 2010 Census population estimates filtered by age and stratified by race, respondents in our sample had attained a higher level of education on average, were more likely to be married, were more likely to be enrolled in Medicare or Medicaid, and were less likely to have privately purchased health insurance. White respondents were also more likely than the white population at large to have employer-provided insurance and had lower average household income. Black respondents in our sample had a higher average household income than the black population at large.

Table 2 Demographic characteristics of the study sample

Discrete-choice experiment analysis

The main-effects model is presented in Table 3. The model fit statistics provide evidence that health-condition characteristics, as defined by the experiment’s attribute levels, influence parental choices about the kinds of test results that are important to know, χ2(15) = 1,407.80, P < 0.001, Pseudo R2 = 0.10. The relative importance percentages indicate how much difference each attribute as a whole across the range of levels contributed to parental decisions about learning their children’s results. Because respondent characteristics are not taken into account, estimates can be interpreted as the average contribution of attribute levels on relative preferences for the sample as a whole. When deciding what kinds of genetic test results are most important to know, parents showed preference for conditions with a high degree of penetrance (38.5%), those that begin earlier in life (12.9%), those that progress rapidly (9.8%), those that have severe mental (11.8%) and/or physical (12.4%) symptoms, and those that are characterized by a shortened life span (13.6%). However, knowing whether there were limited options versus effective options to improve quality of life did not have a significant effect on the choice of test results (B = 0.02; standard error [SE] = 0.01; 95% confidence interval [CI]: −0.01 to 0.04, z = 1.07, P = 0.284). Except for options to increase quality of life, all attributes significantly contributed to respondents’ choices.

Table 3 Conditional logistic regression analysis results for each attribute and level predicting parental interest in learning child’s sequencing information: main-effects model (N = 1,289)

Differences in relative importance by each attribute’s levels are shown in Figure 2. All attribute level preference weights were significantly different from one another, with a few exceptions. For instance, on average, differences in age of onset from less than 1 year (B = 0.15; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: 0.10−0.21) to 1 to 5 years (B = 0.14; SE = 0.03, 95% CI: 0.09−0.19) did not affect which profile was chosen. Also, preference rates among three of four rate-of-progression levels were not statistically distinct. Profiles with a stable rate of progression (B = −0.10; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: −0.15 to −0.05) were just as likely to be selected as important to know as those with a slow rate of progression (B = −0.12; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: −0.17 to −0.07), which in turn were not significantly different from profiles with a moderate rate of progression (B = 0.04; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: −0.01 to 0.09).

Figure 2
figure 2

Plot showing preference weights (line graphs) and mean distress ratings (bar charts) by attribute level. Mean distress ratings within an attribute are all significantly different at P < 0.001, except for the difference between conditions with an age of onset at 6 to 12 years versus 13 to 18 years, t(1,249) = −3.08, P = 0.013, and a nonsignificant pairwise comparison between conditions with an age of onset less than 1 year versus 1 to 5 years, t(1,249) = 1.56, P = 0.707.

Adding the parent gender interaction terms to the main-effects model significantly improved model fit, Δχ2 (15) = 37.30, P = 0.001. Although the interaction analysis revealed differences between mothers’ and fathers’ preferences for select attribute levels, these were all differences of magnitude and not of direction. Consistent with the main-effects model for the full sample, mothers and fathers both showed greater interest in knowing information about health conditions with more severe or pressing implications. Fathers were less interested than mothers in conditions that had only a 10% chance of happening given a true-positive genetic test result (B = −0.12; SE = 0.05; 95% CI: −0.22 to −0.02; z = −2.25; P = 0.025), were likely to decrease a child’s life span (B = −0.09; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: −0.15 to −0.04; z = −3.14, P = 0.002), or were characterized by lasting and severe physical consequences (B = −0.11; SE = 0.04; 95% CI: −0.19 to −0.03; z = −2.59, P = 0.010). Fathers also showed greater relative preference for genetic test results pertaining to conditions that were not associated with any physical disability (B = 0.09; SE = 0.04; 95% CI: 0.01−0.17; z = 2.15, P = 0.031). No other statistical differences by gender were observed.

We also tested for differences in attribute-level relative preferences between white and black respondents; adding these interaction terms to the main-effects model significantly improved model fit, Δχ2(15) = 38.57, P = 0.001. As with the observed moderation effects by gender, differences by race were reflected in the relative weights of preferences for specific attribute levels but did not alter the direction of these effects. The interaction estimates representing differences in coefficients by race showed that black respondents were more likely than white respondents to show interest in learning about conditions that had a 10% chance of developing given a true-positive genetic test result (B = 0.14; SE = 0.05; 95% CI: 0.03−0.24, z = 2.56; P = 0.010), were unlikely to impact a child’s life span (B = 0.09; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: 0.03−0.15; z = 3.00, P = 0.003), or did not pose a threat of mental disability (B = 0.15; SE = 0.04; 95% CI: 0.06−0.23; z = 3.44; P = 0.001). Black respondents also showed less relative preference than white respondents for test results about conditions associated with severe mental disability (B = −0.12; SE = 0.04; 95% CI: −0.20 to −0.03, z = −2.72, P = 0.007). Overall, however, black respondents placed greater value on knowing test results for conditions characterized by severe mental disability than on results that had no threat of mental disability. The interaction analysis did not reveal any other statistically significant differences by race.

Distress questions

Respondents rated how distressing they found each level of the seven attributes. The mean distress ratings are grouped by attribute and presented in Figure 2. Notably, the pattern of distress ratings mirrors respondent preferences found in our conditional logistic regression analysis. Within any given attribute, respondents rated attribute levels corresponding to more severe manifestations as more distressing. There were statistically significant differences in mean distress ratings across levels of all seven attributes, with F tests ranging from F(1.48, 1,852.66) = 18.72, P < 0.001 for age of onset to F(1.38, 1,731.99) = 2,136.80, P < 0.001 for level of mental disability. All but one pairwise difference between levels within attributes were statistically significant (Ps < 0.001). Specifically, parents did not find the prospect of a condition with an age of onset under 1 year any more or less distressing than one with an age of onset of 1 to 5 years, t(1,249) = 1.57, P = 0.707.

Discussion

The level of parental interest in genomic sequencing information is high,5 at least when hypothetical scenarios are proposed. This study revealed that for childhood-onset non-medically actionable genetic disorders leading to mental and/or physical disabilities for which effective preventive treatments are not currently available, parents are relatively more interested in knowing about highly penetrant conditions. Further, the levels of each dimension of information parents most wanted to know were the most severe manifestations, such as greater penetrance, earlier age of onset, greater level of mental and physical disability, rapid rate of progression, and greater impact on life span. These were also rated as more distressing. This may be because the information, although perceived as valuable, was also recognized as being likely to cause distress. Options only to enhance quality of life were not significant and did not match the overall pattern of results.

Based on previous research that shows blacks have greater distrust of the health system,30 we might have expected differences in parental interest in certain attributes associated with non-medically actionable genetic conditions. In general, we did not find meaningful differences between black and white parents on most of the attributes studied. We did find some statistically significant differences along some attributes, such as desire to know information that was unlikely to impact a child’s life span, but the overall direction and magnitude of the findings across groups were quite similar. Past studies demonstrating mistrust focused on the personal receipt of health care31 or on being involved in health research.32 It is possible that in the context of receiving information about one’s child, these differences are diminished. Another possibility is that differences in mistrust of the health-care system by race among participants in this study were less pronounced than those observed in prior research. Although we measured mistrust in the health-care system for this study, disentangling the complex associations among mistrust, race, and preferences is beyond the scope of this paper. Future studies should continue to examine racial and ethnic differences in preferences and responses to disclosure of genomic sequencing information, given that recent research shows that blacks are likely to view genetic testing and research less favorably than are whites.33

We found relatively few meaningful differences between mothers and fathers. Once again, statistically significant differences were revealed, but these differences were quite small. Previous research studying parental preferences for their child’s genomic sequencing has not typically reported differences by gender.34 Given the limited research to date examining parental preferences in this context, future research should nonetheless continue to examine group differences by gender, to extend these findings. Continued research is warranted because parental preferences may vary. Some of these preferences could call for additional counseling, support, or interventions to achieve informed consent. This examination could also inform decision support strategies if future differences emerge, and it could be used to develop support resources for family adaptation as technological advances in screening become more pervasive.35

Consistent results showing parental preferences to learn about conditions with more severe manifestations, which are also more distressing, may reflect the perceived value of knowing such information when making hypothetical decisions. However, it is important to recognize that in actual experience, learning more severe or distressing information could disrupt family functioning. Previous research shows that parents value the personal utility of genomic information in health care36 and research contexts.37 However, the long-term individual, familial, or societal effects of learning genetic information about conditions that are not medically actionable are not known.

The strengths of our study include an experimental design, use of a large sample balanced across gender and two racial categories, and the examination of both the full sample results and subgroup differences for a variety of dimensions that could characterize genetic disorders in childhood that are not medically actionable. Our findings can facilitate an evidence-based approach to informed consent, interpretation and analysis, and disclosure of results, because we were able to obtain parental input using a structured framework that could guide decisions about which genomic variants should be interpreted and reported back to clinicians and families based on parental preferences. The results suggest that parents find utility in learning information that is not medically actionable and that could be distressing.

Despite these strengths, there are some limitations. Our approach focused on examining parental preferences for this one category of childhood-onset genetic disorders. Future research should examine other categories of information, such as those that are medically actionable in childhood or adulthood or not medically actionable in adulthood, to understand the full spectrum of information that could be made available through genome-scale testing. The potential bias of using an online convenience sample cannot be quantified, although research using online panels has shown that this approach has comparable validity and reliability to those of more traditional survey methods.38, 39 Additionally, the hypothetical nature of the choice experiment may not accurately reflect decisions made by parents when they are presented with such decisions in real-life situations. We did not include a “no test” option for parents who were not in the market for test results, and it is possible that these parents might have had more trouble answering the choice questions, and that this contributed to higher variance in the responses.

Despite these potential limitations, this study contributes to the growing body of research illuminating parental preferences for genomic sequencing information for their children and the return of incidental or optional findings, whether in a clinical or public health context. As genome-scale testing becomes more widely available, understanding what type of information should be disclosed to parents and why parents value certain types of information will help both clinicians and parents during clinical and research encounters.