Introduction

Phacoemulsification and intraocular lens (IOL) implantation is increasingly being performed under topical anaesthesia. In a series of surveys of members of the American Society of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, Leaming1 showed that the percentage of respondents who reported topical anaesthesia as their primary method of anaesthesia increased from 0.2% in 1993 to 37% in 1998,2 and 61% in 2003.3 Topical anaesthesia has several advantages over regional anaesthesia: unlike injection anaesthetic techniques, there is rapid return of postoperative visual acuity,4 no postoperative ptosis or diplopia,4 and no risk of damage to the globe or orbital contents.5, 6, 7

It is only in recent years that patients' intraoperative visual experience during cataract surgery using different forms of local (topical, peribulbar, retrobulbar, and sub-Tenon's) anaesthesia has been studied in detail.8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14 Several studies have shown that only a minority (0–20%) of patients experience no light perception intraoperatively.8, 9, 10, 12, 13 Most patients perceive at least some light during the surgery. In addition, many patients see a variety of other visual sensations such as movement, flashes, colours, instruments, the surgeon's fingers or hands, the surgeon, or a change in brightness of light.8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 15

To the best of our knowledge, there has been no report on what patients expect to see during cataract surgery and how this compares to their actual intraoperative visual experience. We were also unable to find a reference to such a report in a computerised search utilising PubMed. We conducted a survey to investigate what patients about to undergo phacoemulsification using topical anaesthesia expected to see intraoperatively and what they subsequently saw during the surgery. In addition, we studied if patients' preoperative expectations had any influence on fear resulting from their intraoperative visual experience, and we sought to identify other factors that may be associated with a frightening visual experience.

Materials and methods

Consecutive cataract patients who were scheduled for routine phacoemulsification and IOL implantation under topical anaesthesia in our institution were interviewed face-to-face about 1 h preoperatively by one of two of the authors (CLA and SSGL), using a standardised questionnaire, about their expectations of possible intraoperative visual sensations during the surgery. They were not advised beforehand on the range of possible visual sensations that they might encounter. We asked patients to grade their anxiety levels at the time of the interview based on a visual analogue scale of 0–10, where 0 represents no anxiety and 10 represents maximal anxiety. The same author interviewed the patients again within 1 h postoperatively about the visual sensations they actually experienced in the operated eye during surgery. They were also asked whether they found their visual experience frightening. The study was approved by our Institutional Review Board and informed consent was obtained from the patients before their participating in the study.

Two–three drops of preservative-free amethocaine 1% (Minims®, Chauvin Pharmaceuticals, Essex, England) was administered onto the operated eye about 5 min before the surgery. No patient was given preoperative sedation or direct intracameral anaesthetic injection. Thirty-five patients were given intraoperative sedation, using intravenous propofol (Diprivan®, AstraZeneca, Wilmington, DE, USA) (10–30 mg, 23 patients), midazolam (Dormicum®, Roche, Basel, Switzerland) (0.5–2 mg, 16 patients), fentanyl (Fentanyl Citrate DBL®, David Bull, Melbourne, Australia) (12.5–25 μg, 10 patients), or a combination of these sedatives.

Fisher's exact tests were conducted for univariate analyses. A logistic (multivariate) regression model was built to ascertain the effects of identified factors that may result in a frightening visual experience, with the Hosmer–Lemeshow test applied to check for its goodness of fit. The graph of change in Pearson's χ2 was used to identify influential observations. All statistical tests were conducted at 5% level of significance and the data were entered into STATA 7.0 (Stata Corporation, 4905 Lakeway Drive, College Station, TX, USA) for analysis.

Results

One hundred consecutive patients were interviewed preoperatively. Two patients were excluded from analysis after they declined to be interviewed postoperatively owing to fatigue, leaving a total of 98 patients who completed the study. No patient experienced any intraoperative complication such as posterior capsule rupture. The patient demographics are as shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Patient demographics

Intraoperative sedation was given in 35 patients (35.7%) and omitted in the remaining 63 patients (64.3%) based on the anaesthetist's assessment of the patients' level of anxiety and sympathetic response (heart rate, respiratory rate, blood pressure) during the operation. The mean (±SD) duration of operation was 15.9 (±6.7) min (range, 5–45). Forty-two patients (42.9%) indicated that they were anxious about their operations preoperatively with a mean score (±SD) of 3.7 (±2.2) (range, 2–8) on a visual analogue scale of 0–10.

Thirty-six patients (36.7%) expected to see at least some light intraoperatively, 38 patients (38.8%) expected that they would not perceive any light at all, and the remaining 24 (24.5%) did not know what to expect. Some of the patients who expected to see light also expected that they would see a variety of other visual sensations as summarised in Table 2. Seven patients (7.1%) expected to lose light perception transiently during the surgery.

Table 2 Proportion of patients who expected to see various specific visual sensations and those who experienced these sensations

Of the 55 patients having their first cataract operation, 10 patients (18.2%) expected to see at least some light during the operation, whereas the remaining 45 patients (81.8%) either did not expect to retain light perception (26 patients, 47.3%) or did not know what to expect (19 patients, 34.5%). In contrast, among the 43 patients undergoing their second cataract operation, 26 patients (60.5%) expected to retain light perception, and only 17 patients (39.5%) either did not expect to do so (12 patients, 27.9%) or did not know what to expect (five patients, 11.6%). This difference was statistically significant (Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, P<0.001).

Postoperatively, all patients (100%) reported that they could see at least some light during the surgery. In addition, they reported a variety of other visual sensations (Table 2). Twenty-six patients (26.5%) reported that they lost light perception for short intervals during the surgery. There was no statistically significant association between the use of intraoperative sedation and periods of losing light perception (Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, P=0.999).

Nineteen patients (19.4%) found their visual experience frightening. Those who were anxious (P=0.004), who had a history of cataract surgery in the fellow eye (P=0.018), who experienced an intraoperative increase in clarity (P=0.020), who did not see movement intraoperatively (P=0.022), or who did not know what to expect (P=0.049) were more likely to report that they were frightened, after taking into account a series of other factors using the logistic regression model (Table 3). The model was found to be satisfactory in terms of the Hosmer–Lemeshow test (χ82: 7.34; P-value: 0.50). There were several severe influential observations identified but these warranted no further investigation. There were no statistically significant associations between a frightening visual experience and gender, age, coexisting ocular pathology, preoperative best-corrected visual acuity, duration of surgery, and a difference between patients' preoperative expectations and their actual visual experience (Table 3). Use of intraoperative sedation was not associated with fear from visual experience.

Table 3 Potential risk factors associated with a frightening intraoperative visual experience

Fifty-seven (58.2%) patients reported postoperatively that they felt that appropriate preoperative counselling about possible intraoperative visual sensations would have been useful, whereas 41 (41.8%) did not think so. Among the 19 patients who experienced fear, 73.7% (14 patients) indicated that they would have found preoperative counselling useful compared to 54.4% (43 patients) among the 79 patients who were not frightened. This difference was, however, not statistically significant (Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, P=0.195).

Nine patients (9.2%) indicated that their visual experience affected their satisfaction with the operation. Of these, two (22.2%) reported that their visual experience was unpleasant and had overall decreased satisfaction, whereas seven (77.8%) indicated their overall satisfaction was increased owing to the intraoperative visual pleasantries. A frightening visual experience was statistically more likely to result in decreased satisfaction than increased satisfaction (Fisher's exact test, one-tailed, P=0.028). One of the patients who had found his visual experience pleasant described the ‘fantastic colours’ that he saw.

Discussion

A significant number of patients in our study (63.3%) either believed that they would not see any light at all (38.8%) or did not know what visual sensations to expect (24.5%) during phacoemulsification under topical anaesthesia. Only 36.7% expected to see at least some light. The expectation of seeing during the operation was higher in patients undergoing their second cataract operation (P<0.001). It is possible that intraoperative visual sensations experienced in the first operation may have influenced their beliefs as a previous cataract surgery may be regarded as a practical form of ‘counselling’ on the visual sensations during cataract surgery. Regardless of the patients' expectations, all of them retained light perception during surgery, similar to the studies by Au Eong et al and Newman,9, 13 and many of them also perceived a variety of other visual sensations, which were in excess of their preoperative expectations (Table 2).

In this study, 19.4% of our patients were frightened by their visual sensations, a proportion that is slightly higher than the 15.4% previously reported by Au Eong et al.9 Fear experienced during cataract surgery is of clinical significance as it may cause a sympathetic stress response, which can result in tachycardia, hypertension, hyperventilation, ischaemic strain on the heart, and acute panic attacks.16, 17, 18 In addition, it may cause patients to become less cooperative during the surgery and may increase intraoperative morbidity as most cataract patients are in the geriatric age group and have significant concurrent chronic diseases like hypertension and ischaemic heart disease.

There were several interesting risk factors that correlated with patients' likelihood of being frightened by their intraoperative visual experience. The risk factor with the most significant correlation was preoperative anxiety (P=0.004). Preoperative preparation of patients should thus include a specific aim to allay anxiety and adequate knowledge of what they can expect to see during the surgery may be useful. Another risk factor for fear was patients' preoperative expectations: patients who did not know what to expect were significantly more likely to be frightened compared to those who expected to see some light (P=0.049). In addition, a higher proportion of patients who were frightened indicated that preoperative counselling would have been useful compared to those who were not frightened.

It thus appears from the above results that preoperative counselling may allay anxiety and reduce fear resulting from intraoperative visual experience. In fact, this finding is also suggested by a paper by Tan et al.18 Currently, preoperative counselling about possible visual experience during surgery may still not be widely practised. In our study, no patient received any preoperative counselling about possible intraoperative visual experience. The lack of routine preoperative counselling may be because some ophthalmologists are still unaware of the varied and often vivid visual sensations patients may experience intraoperatively or are unconvinced by the usefulness of preoperative counselling (Leo SW, Lee LKM, Au Eong KG. Visual experience during phacoemulsification and intraocular lens implantation under topical anaesthesia: a nationwide survey on the knowledge and practices of ophthalmologists in Singapore. Presented at the XXIXth International Congress of Ophthalmology, Sydney, April 21–25, 2002).

Of interest is the finding that patients with a history of previous cataract surgery were statistically more likely to be frightened than those without (P=0.018). As noted in Table 1 (demographics), out of the 43 patients who had a history of cataract surgery, 19 patients (44.2%) received topical anaesthesia in their previous operations, 11 (25.6%) received regional anaesthesia, one (2.3%) received general anaesthesia, and 12 (27.9%) could not remember what anaesthesia they received. Also previously noted was that the majority of those undergoing their second cataract operation expected to be able to see during the surgery. At first glance, this may seem to indicate that preoperatively expecting to retain light perception predisposes to a frightening visual experience, contrary to our other finding that patients who did not know what to expect were more likely to be frightened. A possible explanation for this is that the patients who had a more invasive form of anaesthesia (at least 12 in our study) during the first cataract surgery compared to the topical anaesthesia used for the second operation might have experienced a more vivid visual experience the second time and this could have alarmed them. It has been shown in a recent prospective randomised clinical trial that more patients operated on using topical anaesthesia experienced perception of light, colours, and a change in light brightness compared to those operated on using retrobulbar anaesthesia.19 The results therefore suggest that preoperative counselling should target all patients regardless of their preoperative expectations or whether or not they have had a previous cataract surgery.

Another interesting finding from our study is that nine patients (9.2%) reported a change in their satisfaction with the surgery owing to their visual experience, and seven (7.1%) among these nine patients reported their visual experiences to be pleasant, whereas two (2.0%) had unpleasant experiences. This finding is similar to a recent randomised controlled trial conducted in India, which found that visual experiences can be both pleasant and unpleasant.19 Our anecdoctal experience has also shown that some patients do find their visual experiences pleasant owing to the unexpected multitude of colours and shapes, an observation that has been pointed out in recent articles by Zia et al20 and Au Eong et al.21

Newman documented that six out of 102 patients (5.9%) lost light perception at times during phacoemulsification and IOL implantation under topical anaesthesia.13 Transiently raised intraocular pressure could be responsible for this phenomenon. In this study, a higher proportion of patients (26.5%) reported transient loss of light perception during the operation, and this was not associated with the use of intraoperative sedation (P=0.999). Loss of light perception intraoperatively can conceivably be frightening to some patients who may misconstrue this as a complication occurring during the surgery. Preoperative counselling should therefore include a warning about this phenomenon.

In summary, our study shows that a significant proportion of patients undergoing phacoemulsification under topical anaesthesia were either unsure about the possible visual sensations they might experience during the operation or did not expect to see anything. Contrary to their beliefs, all of them experienced at least some light perception, with many perceiving a variety of other visual sensations that were frightening to nearly one in five patients. A majority of patients felt that preoperative counselling about possible intraoperative visual sensations would have been useful and the proportion of patients who felt so was higher among those who were frightened than those who were not. This suggests the importance of preoperative counselling in helping to alleviate the fear experienced by patients.