Main

Although it is widely hypothesized that central nervous system (CNS) serotonergic function varies appreciably among individuals and that such variation may partially underlie differences in trait measures of disinhibition or impulsivity (e.g., Depue and Spoont, 1986; Soubrie 1986; Cloninger, 1987; Zuckerman, 1995), the retest reliability of interindividual variability in serotonergic function (and hence the temporal stability of this putative neurobiologic dimension) remains unclear. Neuropharmacologic challenges are used to index serotonergic function, as reflected in the stimulated release of anterior pituitary hormones into the circulation (Murphy et al. 1990; Yatham and Steiner, 1993). The prolactin (PRL) response to fenfluramine hydrochloride has been used commonly to index central serotonergic responsivity in humans (Murphy et al. 1990). Fenfluramine has been favored over other agents because it potently induces presynaptic release of serotonin, blocks reuptake, and possibly activates postsynaptic receptors (Borroni et al. 1983; Quattrone et al. 1983; Coccaro et al., 1989), thereby providing a measure of net serotonergic responsivity.

With respect to retest reliability, Coccaro et al. (1987) reported that peak ΔPRL response to fenfluramine was moderately correlated (r = 0.75) in a sample of 13 male psychiatric patients over 2 to 12 days. Similarly, Stoff et al. (1992) demonstrated that the peak ΔPRL response to fenfluramine was reproducible (r = 0.80) across 1 week among 10 boys with conduct disorder. Because these two studies examined patient samples (e.g., conduct disorder, major depression) in whom serotonergic function may be chronically dysregulated (e.g., Coccaro et al. 1989; Flory et al. 1998), it remains unclear to what extent normative variability in central serotonergic responsivity also reflects a reproducible dimension of individual differences.

The current study was conducted to examine the retest reliability of the PRL response to fenfluramine in 57 nonpsychiatric patients. Participants were derived from the untreated (placebo) condition of a 6-month clinical trial conducted to examine behavioral sequelae of a cholesterol-lowering medication (Muldoon et al. 2000). A fenfluramine challenge protocol was administered at the beginning and the end of the treatment period.

METHOD

Participants

Men and women, aged 24–60 years, were included in the clinical trial if their low density lipoprotein cholesterol (LDL-C) levels exceeded 160 mg/dl. All participants were of good general health, and none were taking medications known to affect serotonergic responsivity or plasma prolactin concentrations, including psychotropic or glucocorticoid medications. Four participants were taking antihypertensive medications and six were using endogenous hormones (i.e., hormone replacement therapy or oral contraceptives) throughout the 6-month clinical trial. Medical exclusions from the clinical trial included cancer treatment within the past year, stroke, diabetes requiring pharmacological treatment, chronic kidney or liver disease, untreated hypertension, schizophrenia, or delusional disorder. Pregnant or lactating women were also excluded from participating in the study.

Fenfluramine challenge data were available on 57 individuals, including 26 women and 31 men with an average age of 46 years (SEM = 1.0). Thirty-six percent of the participants had a baccalaureate or higher degree, and 90 percent of the sample was employed. Fifty-one participants were Caucasian, five were African American, and one was Asian American. Nine individuals (16%) met criteria for a current DSM-III-R Axis I diagnosis of mild to moderate severity at one or both assessments based on structured clinical interview (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R: Non-Patient Edition [Spitzer et al., 1990]), but none were receiving treatment. Diagnoses included social phobia (n = 1), simple phobia (n = 1), alcohol dependence (n = 3), a nonpsychotic mood disorder (n = 3), and adjustment disorder (n = 1). All statistical analyses reported below retained significance on reanalysis excluding these nine individuals. Participants provided written, informed consent, and the protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Pittsburgh.

Procedure

Fenfluramine Challenge

The protocol for the challenge has been described in detail (Manuck et al. 1998). Briefly, participants reported to the laboratory in the morning after a 12-h fast. A 20-gauge, heparin-locked catheter was introduced aseptically into a vein in the antecubital fossa. After a 30-min adaptation period, a blood sample to determine baseline (i.e., pre-administration) plasma PRL concentration was drawn, after which dl-Fenfluramine hydrochloride was administered orally as a single dose of 30–60 mg in 10-mg increments to best approximate a weight relative administration of 0.55–0.65 mg fenfluramine/kg body weight. Blood samples were drawn 60, 120, 150, 180, and 210 min later. Plasma PRL concentration was determined from each sample, and fenfluramine and norfenfluramine concentrations during the challenge were determined from samples drawn at 150 and 210 min. All blood samples were centrifuged, separated, and frozen at −70°C until analysis. An identical protocol was repeated after 6 months (average days = 181; SEM = 2.7]).

PRL Assay

Plasma PRL levels were determined in duplicate using Immunocorp's (Montreal, Canada) solid phase two-site immunoradiometric method. The lower limit of sensitivity of the PRL assay is 0.5 ng/ml, and the interassay coefficient of variation is 6–9%. The lower detection limit of the fenfluramine/norfenfluramine assays is 2.0 ng/ml, and the coefficient of variation is 5.2% at 5.0 ng/ml.

Statistical Analyses

Peak PRL response (ΔPRL[fen]) was expressed as the arithmetic difference between the highest PRL concentration obtained after drug administration and pre-administration PRL concentration. The area under the response curve (AUC PRL[fen]) was calculated using trapezoidal integration. These two indices of serotonergic responsivity were then adjusted by regression for any covariation with pre-administration PRL concentration, sex, age, or fenfluramine and norfenfluramine concentrations during the challenge. The temporal stability of individual differences in these indices of serotonergic responsivity was examined with Pearson correlations, computed across the 6-month interval. Among women, the challenges were performed without control for menstrual cycle phase, and, because PRL response to fenfluramine is known to vary appreciably across the menstrual cycle (O'Keane et al. 1991), the correlations were repeated excluding premenopausal women and postmenopausal women taking hormone replacement therapy according to a cyclic schedule (n = 15).

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Baseline PRL levels, unadjusted ΔPRL[fen], and AUC PRL[fen] levels at both assessments are presented in Table 1, for men and women separately. Because of practical constraints, we shortened the standard 5-h protocol to 3.5 h, which may account for adjusted ΔPRL[fen] values that are somewhat lower than previous reports (e.g., Coccaro et al. 1987; Stoff et al., 1992)). As we have reported previously (Manuck et al., 1998), however, peak PRL concentrations during abbreviated and standard protocols correlated 0.91 in a separate sample of 42 individuals who completed the 5-h protocol, indicating high concordance for peak PRL concentrations across the two sampling intervals.

Table 1 Sample Characteristics and Plasma Prolactin Levels (Mean ± SEM.)*

Retest correlations for baseline PRL levels and the two indices of serotonergic responsivity are presented in Table 2 Baseline, or preadministration, PRL levels were highly stable over 6 months, with a correlation of 0.88 across all participants. Adjusted Δ PRL[fen] concentrations and AUC PRL[fen] were moderately reproducible across time in the total sample (r = 0.50 and 0.57, respectively; p < .0001). When cycling women were excluded from the analyses, the retest correlations were comparable, increasing to 0.54 (p < .0001) for adjusted ΔPRL[fen] and 0.59 (p < .0001) for adjusted AUC PRL[fen]. These results demonstrate moderate retest reliability for the fenfluramine challenge when used as an index of CNS serotonergic responsivity in adults. That the results of the current study were not stronger may be due to the interval between measurements, which was considerably longer than that of previous reports, that is, 6 months compared with 1–2 weeks in the studies reported by Coccaro et al. (1987) and Stoff et al. (1992).

Table 2

The results of the current study are consistent with previous reports showing good stability of the prolactin response to fenfluramine in psychiatric patients (Coccaro et al., 1987; Stoff et al., 1992). Participants in the current study were generally healthy, and none were receiving psychopharmacological treatment. However, all had elevated LDL-C concentrations. In the current sample, we are unable to evaluate hypotheses linking (low) serum cholesterol to psychiatric disorders and behaviors associated with dysregulated 5-HT responsivity (e.g., depression, impulsive aggression, suicide). It might be noted that within the restricted range of lipid concentrations present in our sample, neither the adjusted ΔPRL[fen] concentrations nor the AUC PRL[fen] were associated with variability in total cholesterol, LDL-C, or high density lipoprotein cholesterol levels at either occasion of testing (r = −0.18–0.14). Thus, to the extent that cholesterol varied in this sample, it was unrelated to measures of CNS serotonergic responsivity.

Hormonal responses to fenfluramine are thought to represent net CNS serotonergic responsivity, because fenfluramine both stimulates presynaptic release of serotonin and blocks reuptake. It is not clear, however, whether hormonal responses to agents that preferentially target pre- or postsynaptic events or specific serotonin receptors are temporally stable. For example, intravenous infusion of clomipramine, which blocks serotonin reuptake, was associated with comparable peak prolactin responses across 4 weeks in healthy men but not across 2 weeks in a separate group of healthy men (Gilmore et al., 1993). The reproducibility of hormonal responses to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors awaits further confirmation.

To conclude, the findings of this study are consistent with speculation that interindividual variability on indices of serotonergic function reflects a stable neurobiologic dimension of individual differences that may in turn underlie variation in serotonin-associated traits of personality or temperament.