INTRODUCTION

Methamphetamine (MA) is a highly addictive substance (Woolverton et al, 1984) and MA abuse has been increasing worldwide (Shaw, 1999). It has been reported to have a long-term neurotoxic effect on dopaminergic neurons (Preston et al, 1985; Wilson et al, 1996). Frontal and subcortical areas have been suggested as more vulnerable areas to the neurotoxic effects of MA, as these regions are closely associated with the dopaminergic system (Sekine et al, 2003; Volkow et al, 2001b).

Prior 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) studies have reported changes of relative regional cerebral glucose metabolism (rCMRglc) in MA users. Volkow et al (2001c) have reported higher cortical and lower subcortical rCMRglc in abstinent MA users. London et al (2004) have reported abnormal rCMRglc in limbic and paralimbic areas in recently detoxified MA users.

Other prior brain imaging studies have reported that MA users have various kinds of abnormalities in frontal areas (Chang et al, 2002; Ernst et al, 2000a; Nordahl et al, 2002; Paulus et al, 2003, 2002; Sekine et al, 2003; Thompson et al, 2004; Volkow et al, 2001a). In addition, our recently completed studies have shown that MA users have significantly lower prefrontal gray matter density (Chung et al, 2004) and decreased regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in anterior cingulate cortex compared to healthy comparison subjects (Hwang et al, 2004).

Impairment of neuropsychological function has also been reported in MA users (Kalechstein et al, 2003; Vorhees et al, 1994). Paulus et al (2002, 2003) have reported decision-making dysfunctions in MA users. They have also reported a failure of activation in prefrontal cortices during decision-making tests in MA users, as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). However, the detailed association of frontal executive functions and rCMRglc in frontal lobes of MA users has not been reported.

Potential gender difference has been suggested in neurotoxic effects of MA (Hall and Broderick, 1991). Males have been reported to be more susceptible to neurotoxic effects of MA in animal studies (Hirata et al, 1996; Wagner et al, 1993). Recently, Chang et al (2002) have reported that male MA users had decreased rCBF and female MA users had increased rCBF in occipital and midline structures, relative to gender-matched comparison subjects (Chang et al, 2002). They have also reported that the decrease of frontal rCBF was more prominent in male MA users. However, to the best of our knowledge, there have been no prior studies investigating gender differences on the cerebral glucose metabolism of MA users.

Based on prior imaging and neuropsychological studies, we hypothesized that abstinent MA users would have decreased frontal rCMRglc, as measured by FDG-PET, and impaired frontal executive function, as assessed by the Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST), relative to healthy comparison subjects. We also hypothesized that, in accord with previous studies reporting potential gender difference in MA neurotoxicity, decreased frontal rCMRglc and poor frontal executive function would be more pronounced in male MA users than in female MA users.

PATIENTS AND METHODS

Subjects

Inclusion criteria were (1) age 19–49 years, (2) DSM-IV MA abuse or dependence as determined by Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV), as administered by experienced psychiatrists, (3) abstinence for at least 4 weeks, and (4) cumulative intravenous MA use over 50.0 g. Exclusion criteria were (1) current or past serious medical or neurological illness, (2) current or past history of Axis I psychiatric disorder: schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and other psychotic disorders, and any current Axis I disorder requiring psychotropic medications, as identified by SCID-IV, (3) antisocial or borderline personality disorders, as identified by the Personality Disorder Questionnaire-4, (4) lifetime exposure to any other DSM-IV dependence- or abuse-related drugs, except nicotine, caffeine, alcohol drinking, and prescribed medications, and (5) contraindications to magnetic resonance (MR) scanning. Study subjects were instructed to abstain from alcohol, any prescribed medications, or herbal medications for at least 2 weeks before the PET scan. Smoking was prohibited for 2 h before the PET scan, as a prolonged cessation of smoking may induce nicotine withdrawal, which can confound PET findings in smokers.

In all, 35 MA users (28 males and seven females) and 21 healthy comparison subjects (15 males and six females) were recruited through advertisements at local newspapers and at the Korean Association against Drug Abuse. To investigate potential gender effects on the MA use, we compared rCMRglc and frontal executive functions between male MA users (n=28) and male comparison subjects (n=15), and between female MA users (n=7) and female comparison subjects (n=6). Study procedures including PET and the WCST were conducted in Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, South Korea. Study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Seoul National University Hospital and McLean Hospital, Massachusetts, USA. After providing a complete description of the study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

Information regarding clinical and demographic variables was derived from the MA users and healthy comparison subjects by research psychologists and research psychiatrists. The Addiction Severity Index (ASI) (McLellan et al, 1980) was administered to 30 MA users.

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

A research neuropsychologist conducted WCST to examine impairment in frontal executive function for 33 MA users and 21 healthy comparison subjects. Two MA users (37- and 42-year-old males) who had participated in the PET study did not elect to participate in the neuropsychological test. The WCST assesses the executive function, the ability to form abstract concepts, to shift and maintain set, and to utilize feedback (Heaton et al, 1993; Yoo et al, 2000). Numbers of perseveration errors, nonperseveration errors, and total errors (perseveration errors+ nonperseveration errors) were used for statistical analysis.

Acquisition of Positron Emission Tomography

Subjects were scanned using an ECAT EXACT 47 scanner (Siemens-CTI, Knoxville, TN, USA), which had an intrinsic resolution of 5.2 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) and simultaneously imaged 47 contiguous transverse planes with a thickness of 3.4 mm for a longitudinal field view of 16.2 cm. Before injection of the tracer, a 15-min transmission scan was performed using triple GE-68 rod sources to correct attenuation. Emission scanning started after intravenous injection of 370 MBq (10 mCi) of 18F-FDG and continued for 30 min without earplugs or eye pads. All 18F-FDG PET scans were performed in a dimly lit room with minimal auditory stimulation during both injection and PET scanning, with the subject in a supine position with eyes closed so as to minimize confounding effects of any activities. At the beginning of PET scanning, subjects were instructed not to think about or do anything in particular during the scanning. Gathered data were reconstructed in a 128 × 128 × 47 matrix with a pixel size of 2.1 × 2.1 × 3.4 mm by means of a filtered back-projection algorithm employing a Shepp-Logan filter with a cutoff frequency of 0.3 cycles/pixel.

Imaging Data Analysis

Spatial preprocessing and statistical analysis were performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM 99) (Institute of Neurology, University College of London, UK) implemented in Matlab (Mathworks Inc., USA) (Friston et al, 1995a). All reconstructed images were spatially normalized into the MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, CA) standard template to remove the intersubject anatomical variability (Friston et al, 1995b; Tarairach and Tournoux, 1988). Affine transformation was performed to determine the 12 optimal parameters to register the brain on the template. Subtle differences between the transformed image and the template were removed by the nonlinear registration method using the weighted sum of the predefined smooth basis functions used in discrete cosine transformation.

Spatially normalized images were smoothed by convolution with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with 16 mm FWHM to increase the signal-to-noise ratio and to accommodate the variations in subtle anatomical structures. Normalization was performed to remove between-subject variability of global metabolism (proportional scaling in SPM). After spatial and count normalization, significant differences of rCMRglc between MA users and healthy comparison subjects were estimated at every voxel using t-statistics. Parameters to define regions of significant differences were conservatively set. These include p-value=0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons), height threshold t=4.88, and the extent threshold=50 voxels. For volume of interest (VOI) count analysis on the significantly different voxel cluster, normalized counts were calculated using VOI module of SPM. However, for the thoroughness of report, differences at the uncorrected p level of 0.001 were also presented.

Statistical Analysis

Group differences in variables involving continuous data were computed using independent t-tests. Between-group comparisons involving categorical data were assessed using Fisher's exact test for 2 × k table. Associations between continuous variables were calculated using Pearson correlation analysis. Statistical significance was defined at 0.05 and two-tailed. Stata 7.0 for Windows was used for computations.

First, MA users and healthy comparison subjects were compared. In the subanalysis, comparisons between male MA users and male comparison subjects and between female MA users and female comparison subjects were conducted to explore potential gender effects.

RESULTS

Demographic and Clinical Data

There were no significant differences in age, sex composition, prevalence of social alcohol drinking, or handedness between 35 abstinent MA users (28 men and seven women, 35.5±6.4 years) and 21 healthy comparison subjects (15 men and six women, 33.2±6.4 years). Prevalence of current cigarette smoking was greater in MA users, relative to healthy comparison subjects (Fisher's exact test, p=0.02). There was a significant difference in educational level between MA users and healthy comparison subjects (independent t-test, df=54, t=10.1, p<0.01). It was practically impossible to recruit healthy comparison subjects with educational levels comparable to those in MA users. Instead, we matched the socioeconomic status of parents between groups as in prior drug studies (Pope et al, 2001). There were no significant differences in the pack years of cigarette smoking between the smokers in the MA user group (n=21) and the smokers in the healthy comparison group (n=6). All MA users were intravenous users. Detailed drug-related variables of abstinent MA users are described in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of MA Users and Healthy Comparison Subjects

There were no significant differences in total cumulative dose, average daily dose, mean abstinence period, ASI scores, handedness, or prevalence of social alcohol drinking and current cigarette smoking between male and female MA users. Male MA users were significantly older than female MA users (independent t-test; df=33, t=3.82, p<0.01).

There were no significant differences in age, handedness, or prevalence of social alcohol drinking and current cigarette smoking between 28 male MA users (37.2±5.1 years) and 15 male comparison subjects (36.1±4.8 years).

There were no significant differences in age, handedness, or prevalence of social alcohol drinking between seven female MA users (28.5±6.8 years) and six female comparison subjects (26.1±3.8 years). Prevalence of cigarette smoking was larger in female MA users, relative to female comparison subjects (Fisher's exact test, p=0.03).

Comparison between Methamphetamine Users and Healthy Comparison Subjects

There were significant decreases in rCMRglc in the right superior frontal white matter (Talairach coordinates (x, y, z): 20, 34, 30; 6.1% decrease) in MA users relative to the healthy comparison subjects (corrected p<0.05, t>4.88) (Figure 1). The nearest gray matter region was the right middle frontal cortex (Brodmann area, BA, 9). When uncorrected p<0.001 was used for defining significant differences, there were significant decreases in rCMRglc in the left medial frontal gray matter (BA 8) and bilateral subgyral white matters in MA users relative to healthy comparison subjects.

Figure 1
figure 1

Region of decreased rCMRglc in MA users relative to healthy comparison subjects. Relative rCMRglc decrease in MA users (n=35) relative to healthy comparison subjects (n=21) is depicted in glass brain projection (left panel), slice view (middle panel), and the brain of one healthy volunteer (right panel). Reduction of rCMRglc in right superior frontal white matter (Talairach coordinates (x, y, z): 20, 34, 30) in MA users relative to healthy comparison subjects is shown. SPM map (t>4.88, extent threshold=50 voxels, p<0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons) is provided. MA: methamphetamine; rCMRglc: regional cerebral glucose metabolism; SPM: statistical parametric mapping.

MA users had significantly more total errors, perseveration errors, and nonperseveration errors in the WCST relative to the healthy comparison subjects (independent t-test; t=3.29, df=52, p<0.01; t=2.91, df=52, p<0.01; t=2.81, df=52, p<0.01, respectively).

The number of total errors, perseveration errors, and nonperseveration errors in the WCST negatively correlated with the rCMRglc value in the area of significant difference (ie right middle frontal white matter) in MA users (Pearson's correlation, r=−0.45, n=33, p<0.01; r=−0.54, n=33, p<0.01; r=−0.36, n=33, p=0.04, respectively) (Figure 2). Age had no significant correlations with the rCMRglc value in the area of significant difference or total errors, perseveration errors, or nonperseveration errors in WCST in MA users.

Figure 2
figure 2

Correlations of number of perseveration and nonperseveration errors in WCST with rCMRglc in right frontal white matter (region with significant rCMRglc difference between MA users (n=35) and healthy comparison subjects (n=21) (Talairach coordinates (x, y, z): 20, 34, 30)) of MA users (n=33). Negative correlation between rCMRglc in right frontal white matter and number of perseveration errors in MA users (n=33) (upper panel) (Pearson's correlation, r=−0.54, n=33, p<0.01), and also between rCMRglc in right frontal white matter and number of nonperseveration errors in MA users (n=33) (lower panel) (Pearson's correlation, r=−0.36, n=33, p=0.04) is presented. MA: methamphetamine; rCMRglc: regional cerebral glucose metabolism; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

There were no significant differences in rCMRglc in the area of significant difference or the number of errors in the WCST between MA users who were smokers (n=21) and those who were not (n=14). In addition, there were no significant differences in rCMRglc in the area of significant difference or the number of errors in the WCST between smokers (n=6) and nonsmokers (n=15) in the healthy comparison group.

We conducted multiple regression analyses (dependent variables: WCST and PET raw scores; independent variables: diagnosis, education). Similar results were produced when educational levels were covaried. In addition, there were no within-group correlations between education levels and number of errors in the WCST or rGMRglc value in the area of significant difference in MA users and in healthy volunteers.

Findings in Male Methamphetamine Users

There were significant decreases in the rCMRglc value in the area of significant difference in male MA users relative to male comparison subjects (independent t-test, t=6.43, df=41, p<0.01) (Figure 3). Male MA users had significantly more total errors, perseveration errors, and nonperseveration errors in WCST relative to male comparison subjects (independent t-test, t=2.90, df=39, p<0.01; t=2.59, df=39, p=0.01; t=2.76, df=39, p<0.01, respectively) (Figure 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Relative rCMRglc in right frontal white matter (region with significant rCMRglc difference between MA users (n=35) and healthy comparison subjects (n=21) (Talairach coordinates (x, y, z): 20, 34, 30)) and the number of total errors in WCST in male comparison subjects and male MA users. Significant differences in rCMRglc in right frontal white matter between male MA users (n=28) and male comparison subjects (n=15) (left panel) (independent t-test, t=6.43, df=41, p<0.01), and also in number of total error in WCST between male MA users (n=26) and male comparison subjects (n=15) (right panel) (independent t-test, t=2.90, df=39, p<0.01) are presented. MA: methamphetamine; rCMRglc: regional cerebral glucose metabolism; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

The number of total errors and perseveration errors in WCST negatively correlated with rCMRglc values in the area of significant difference in male MA users (Pearson's correlation, r=−0.47, n=26, p=0.02; r=−0.54, n=33, p<0.01, respectively). The number of nonperseveration errors in the WCST was not significantly correlated with rCMRglc values in the area of significant difference in male MA users (Pearson's correlation, r=−0.31, n=26, p=0.12).

Findings in Female Methamphetamine Users

There were no significant differences in rCMRglc values in the area of significant difference between female MA users and female comparison subjects (independent t-test, t=0.63, df=11, p=0.54). In addition, there were no significant differences in total errors, perseveration errors, and nonperseveration errors in the WCST between female MA users and female comparison subjects (independent t-test, t=1.34, df=11, p=0.21; t=1.24, df=11, p=0.24; t=0.54, df=11, p=0.60, respectively).

There were no significant correlations of the number of errors (total, perseveration, and nonperseveration errors) in the WCST with rCMRglc values in the area of significant difference in female MA users (Pearson's correlation, r=−0.13, n=7, p=0.78; r=−0.10, n=7, p=0.83; r=−0.19, n=7, p=0.68, respectively).

DISCUSSION

We report decreased rCMRglc in the frontal white matter and impairment in frontal executive function in MA users. The levels of these impairments correlated with each other. In subanalysis to investigate potential gender differences, our findings of decreased frontal rCMRglc and impaired frontal executive function were found only in male MA users, but not in female MA users.

To the best of our knowledge, the current report is the first study in MA users investigating the gender–MA interactions on the rCMRglc. Strengths of our study include (1) all MA subjects were without comorbid axis I or II disorders for which structural brain deficits have been reported and (2) all MA subjects were not only without current or lifetime diagnosis of other substance abuse/dependence but also without any lifetime exposure to any other DSM-IV dependence- or abuse-related drugs, except nicotine, caffeine, alcohol, and prescribed medications.

In accord with our first hypothesis, the MA user had decreased rCMRglc in frontal white matter. Prior imaging studies other than FDG-PET in MA users have reported various kinds of frontal abnormalities; decreased dopamine transporter level (Sekine et al, 2003), lesser task-related activation (Paulus et al, 2002, 2003), decreased rCBF (Chang et al, 2002; Hwang et al, 2004), decreased relative N-acetyl aspartate (NAA) level (Ernst et al, 2000a; Nordahl et al, 2002), and gray matter deficits (Thompson et al, 2004; Chung et al, 2004). Our finding of hypometabolism in the frontal white matter provides additional evidence for frontal abnormality in MA users.

Prior FDG-PET studies have also reported that MA users had altered rCMRglc (London et al, 2004; Volkow et al, 2001c). Volkow et al (2001c) have reported that MA abusers had relative glucose hypometabolism in the striatum and thalamus and hypermetabolism in the parietal cortex. London et al (2004) have reported abnormal glucose metabolism in the limbic and paralimbic areas in MA users. In the current study, we report decreased rCMRglc in the frontal white matter.

Locations of the reported hypometabolism do not coincide exactly from one study to another. Differences in the abstinence period of MA subjects in each study might be the most important reason for different locations of altered metabolism among studies, as rCMRglc in MA users has been reported to be affected by the length of abstinence periods (Wang et al, 2004). While most MA subjects were abstinent for 2 weeks to 5 months in Volkow's study and for 4–7 days in London's study, MA subjects in the present study were abstinent for much longer periods (mean abstinence period=19.1 months). Discrepancies of the locations may be also attributable to the different clinical characteristics of MA users, as MA users in the current study were all intravenous MA users and without lifetime exposure to other major street drugs. Furthermore, brain areas with significant hypometabolism in the different studies may have some close functional connections, as shown in a study reporting correlations between decreased dopamine D2 receptor levels in striatum and orbitofrontal rCMRglc in MA users (Volkow et al, 2001a).

Our findings of rCMRglc decrease in the frontal white matter, but not in the gray matter, may seem unusual, as rCMRglc of white matter is generally lower compared to that of gray matter. However, we found decreased rCMRglc in other frontal regions including gray matter of MA users when a less conservative criterion, that is, uncorrected p level of 0.001, was applied. In accordance with our findings, a decreased level of NAA, a marker of neuronal viability, in frontal white matter has also been reported in MA users (Ernst et al, 2000a). Therefore, our findings suggest that impairments in the frontal lobe of MA users are more profound in the white matter than in the gray matter.

In addition to hypometabolism of the frontal white matter, we also report the impairment of frontal executive function in MA users. The closest gray matter areas to the areas with significantly decreased rCMRglc in our study corresponded with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which is associated with frontal executive function. Inefficient decision-making strategy in MA users, which is indicated by more errors in the WCST in the present study, is consistent with prior studies reporting impairment of the frontal executive function in MA users (Kalechstein et al, 2003; Paulus et al, 2002, 2003).

Poor performances of MA users in the WCST in the current study correlate with decreased rCMRglc in frontal white matter. These correlations between frontal executive function and frontal metabolism in our study are in accord with prior fMRI studies reporting the failure of normal prefrontal activation during decision-making tasks in MA users (Paulus et al, 2002, 2003). Therefore, the current finding of decreased rCMRglc in frontal white matter may underlie the impairment in frontal executive function.

In line with our second hypothesis, the decreased rCMRglc in the frontal white matter and the impairment in the frontal executive function were found only in male MA users. Our finding of gender difference in rCMRglc in frontal white matter in MA users is consistent with a recent study reporting more pronounced hypoperfusion of frontal regions in male MA users (Chang et al, 2002). Gender differences in cerebral perfusion and metabolites of frontal white matter have also been reported in those who abuse cocaine, an addictive psychostimulant like MA. Male cocaine users have decreased rCBF and NAA/creatine ratio, while female cocaine users had increased rCBF and higher myoinositol/creatine ratio in frontal white matter, compared to their gender-matched comparison subjects (Chang et al, 1999; Ernst et al, 2000b; Tucker et al, 2004).

Estrogen, a female sex hormone, may underlie the gender–MA interaction observed in the current study. Estrogen has been reported to have neuroprotective effects against MA exposure (Culmsee et al, 1999; Gao and Dluzen, 2001). Neuroprotective effects of estrogen can be mediated by a number of factors, including cardiovascular effects (Nabulsi et al, 1993), antioxidant effects (Sawada et al, 1998), inhibiting Ca2+ channels in striatal neurons (Mermelstein et al, 1996), inhibiting dopamine transporter function (Wirz-Justice et al, 1974), or reduction of MA-induced hyperthermia (Ali et al, 1996; Dluzen and McDermott, 2002).

However, only females, not males, have been reported to benefit from neuroprotective function of estrogen (Dluzen et al, 2002; Dluzen and McDermott, 2002). Therefore, other mediating factors, associated or nonassociated with estrogen, may also play important roles in gender–MA interaction. As glial response is associated with hypermetabolism (Roh et al, 1998), the absence of frontal hypometabolism in female MA users in the current study may be associated with enhanced glial repair response to MA in females rather than males. After MA exposure, female mice have been reported to express augmented mRNA of glial fibrillary acidic protein, which is associated with glial repair response to brain injury (Dluzen et al, 2003; Garcia-Segura et al, 1999).

Readers should be cautious in interpreting the comparison results between female MA users and female comparison subjects due to the small statistical power. However, consistent gender effects both in frontal metabolism and frontal executive functions in our study suggest the existence of gender differences in resistance against MA neurotoxicity in frontal lobes. Another limitation of our study was significant age difference between female and male MA users, as gender–MA interaction in the present study may be due to confounding age–MA interaction. However, there were no correlations of age with frontal metabolism or frontal executive function in the current study. In addition, there were no significant differences of age between male/female MA users and their gender-matched comparison subjects. Therefore, it was unlikely for our gender–MA interaction to be confounded by potential age–MA interaction.

Educational levels may have confounded our findings of decreased rGMRglc and increased errors in the WCST in MA users relative to healthy comparison subjects since there was a difference in educational levels between groups. However, results were similar when analyses were covaried by educational levels. Consequently, our findings in MA users are likely to stem from the MA use rather than the difference in educational levels.

In addition, an HIV test was not conducted due to ethical and legal issues. As the risk of HIV infection in illegal drug users is high (Farabee et al, 2002), the effects of HIV infection on the findings cannot be totally excluded. However, the prevalence of HIV infection in the general population in Korea is very low relative to other countries. Furthermore, as Korean drug users can easily obtain syringes in pharmacies, only 1.1% of HIV infection is due to i.v. drug use in Korea (Kim et al, 2003). Additionally, we could not find any differences of brain volume between MA subjects and healthy comparison subjects, while patients with HIV infection have been reported to have smaller brain volumes, even in an asymptomatic state (Stout et al, 1998).

In conclusion, we report that abstinent MA users have decreased rCMRglc in frontal white matter and impaired frontal executive functions, and men are more vulnerable to these frontal neurotoxicities caused by MA use than women.