INTRODUCTION

In humans, initial sensitivity to a drug of abuse can be used as a marker for liability to drug addiction (Haertzen et al, 1983). Studies have found that genetic factors contribute to the marked variability among humans in their responsiveness to the reinforcing effects of cocaine and influence the vulnerability of individuals to become cocaine abusers (Cadoret et al, 1986; Luthar and Rounsaville, 1993; Merikangas et al, 1998). Furthermore, a family history positive for alcoholism has been associated with increased risk for cocaine addiction (Smith, 1986), suggesting that similar genes underlie the co-morbidity of alcohol and cocaine abuse.

Pharmacogenetic studies in C57 and DBA mice support the above-mentioned hypothesis as these mouse lines exhibit parallel differential sensitivity to the locomotor-stimulant effects of both ethanol and cocaine, with C57 mice exhibiting less locomotion to both drugs (Rocha et al, 1998; Tolliver and Carney, 1995; Womer et al, 1994). However, this is not the case with all mouse lines. For example, FAST and SLOW mice were selected for their higher and lower, respectively, locomotor response to ethanol (Phillips et al, 1992). The replicate line FAST-1 mice also show higher ethanol-, cocaine-, and methamphetamine-induced locomotor activity compared to SLOW-1 mice (Bergstrom et al, 2003). However, no differences in cocaine-stimulated locomotor activity were found between FAST-2 and SLOW-2 mice (Bergstrom et al, 2003). Long-sleep (LS) and short-sleep (SS) mice were selected for their differential initial sensitivity to the sedative effects of a high dose of ethanol (McClearn and Kakihana, 1981). They also exhibit differential sensitivity to the locomotor-stimulant effects of low doses of ethanol, with SS mice exhibiting greater locomotor activation, compared to LS mice (DeFries et al, 1989; Dudek and Phillips, 1990). However, studies determining the sensitivity of these mice to cocaine have yielded disparate results that appear to depend on whether or not the mice were habituated to the testing environment (De Fiebre et al, 1989; George and Ritz, 1990; Jones et al, 1991). George and Ritz (1990) found that without habituation SS mice exhibited more cocaine-induced locomotor activity than LS mice, whereas Jones et al (1991) used a 2-day testing paradigm and found that LS and SS mice that had been injected with saline on day 1 exhibited similar locomotor activation to cocaine on day 2. Together, these studies reinforce the notion that while drugs of abuse may target some common neuronal pathways, independent mechanisms such as environmental influences are also involved in initial responsiveness (Phillips et al, 1996).

Inbred LS (ILS) and SS (ISS) mice, derived from LS and SS sibling matings, also are differentially sensitive to the hypnotic effects of ethanol, with ILS mice exhibiting longer loss of righting reflex than ISS mice (Markel et al, 1995). On the other hand, low doses of ethanol induce greater locomotor activation in ISS mice, compared to ILS mice (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002). However, an effect opposite to ethanol is seen with cocaine: ILS mice exhibit more locomotor activation than ISS mice (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002). In these experiments, the behavioral testing was carried out following a short habituation period (60 min).

Cocaine inhibits monoamine transporters (Ritz et al, 1990) and increases synaptic levels of dopamine (DA), serotonin (5-HT), and norepinephrine (NE). The behavioral effects of cocaine have been most often associated with the mesolimbic and nigrostriatal DA systems (Kuhar et al, 1991; Morse et al, 1995; Miner, 1997). Both nucleus accumbens (NAc) and striatum (ie, dorsal striatum) have been shown to be involved in the locomotor-stimulant effects of cocaine and amphetamine (Delfs et al, 1990; Heusner et al, 2003; Mao and Wang, 2000; Willuhn et al, 2003). Furthermore, cocaine increases extracellular DA levels in both NAc and striatum (Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988; He and Shippenberg, 2000).

Our laboratory has shown that outbred male Sprague–Dawley rats can be profiled as high or low cocaine responders, based on their initial behavioral responsiveness to an acute low dose of cocaine (10 mg/kg) (Gulley et al, 2003; Sabeti et al, 2002). Furthermore, the differential behavioral responsiveness of the high and low responders to cocaine is accompanied by functional differences in DATs in NAc and striatum (Sabeti et al, 2002). It is possible that similar differences in DAT function may also contribute to the differences in cocaine-induced behavior observed in ILS and ISS mice.

In order to further investigate the differential locomotor responsiveness of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine, we first tested whether minimizing the novelty of the behavioral test chamber would alter the differential behavioral cocaine responsiveness from that previously seen (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002). Subsequently, we studied whether differences in striatal levels of cocaine and/or striatal DAT function contribute to the differential behavioral sensitivity of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Adult (80–90 days old), male ILS and ISS mice were obtained from the Institute for Behavioral Genetics (Boulder, CO). The mice were housed in groups of five and exposed to a 12-h light–dark cycle with food and water available ad libitum. Experiments were conducted between 0700 and 1700 hours. All animal use procedures were in strict accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center.

Locomotor Activity

On day 1, drug-naïve ILS and ISS mice were transferred to the testing room, in which the lights were off for the duration of experimental testing. They were allowed to habituate for 60 min in clear acrylic open-field activity chambers (16″ × 16″ × 15″) that were surrounded by an 8 × 8 photobeam frame (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). Following habituation, mice were injected with saline and locomotor activity was monitored for 30 min before a second saline injection and behavioral monitoring for an additional 60 min. On day 2, the same mice were again transferred to the darkened testing room. The habituation and injection procedures were repeated, with one exception: a randomly selected group of ILS and ISS mice were injected with (−)cocaine HCl (10, 15, or 20 mg/kg, i.p.) instead of the second saline injection. Saline and cocaine (dissolved in saline) were administered i.p. at 1 ml/100 g body weight. The purpose of the first saline injection on day 2 was to ensure that both mouse strains exhibited similar baseline activity prior to cocaine treatment. Horizontal and vertical locomotor activities were determined from the number of consecutive photobeam breaks/5 min.

Cocaine Levels

A separate group of ILS and ISS mice underwent the same behavioral testing as described above for day 1. On day 2, cocaine (10, 15, or 20 mg/kg, i.p.) was administered to all of the mice. At 30 min after cocaine injection, mice were killed by cervical dislocation and their striata were dissected and frozen at −80°C. Cocaine levels in striatum were measured using reverse-phase high-performance liquid chromato-graphy coupled with ultraviolet detection as previously described (Gulley et al, 2003).

[3H]WIN 35,428 Binding

Drug-naïve ILS and ISS mice were killed by cervical dislocation. Their striata were dissected, homogenized in ice-cold assay buffer containing 30 mM NaH2PO4, 15 mM Na2HPO4, and 0.32 M sucrose (pH 7.4), and centrifuged (20 000g; 4°C) for 20 min. To generate indirect saturation binding isotherms, the pellets were re-suspended in assay buffer and were incubated for 60 min on ice in tubes containing [3H]WIN 35,428 (4.2 nM) and various concentrations of unlabeled WIN 35,428 (1 × 10−5–3.16 × 10−10 M). Nonspecific binding was determined in the presence of 30 μM benztropine. Following rapid vacuum filtration over GF/B filters (Brandel Inc., Gaithersburg, MD) and three washes with ice-cold assay buffer, the radioactivity retained on the filters was measured by liquid scintillation spectrometry. Proteins were determined by the method of Bradford (1976) using bovine serum albumin as the standard. Affinity values (IC50 and Ki) and the total number of binding sites (Bmax) were determined from nonlinear curve fitting (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA), according to the method of DeBlasi et al (1989).

In Vivo Electrochemistry

Exogenous DA clearance was measured as previously described (Gulley et al, 2003; Zahniser et al, 1999). Briefly, drug-naïve ILS and ISS mice were anesthetized with urethane to a deep surgical level (see Results) and placed in a stereotaxic frame. A small hole was drilled in the skull to allow insertion of an Ag/AgCl reference electrode near the parietal cortex. A second hole was drilled overlying the left striatum (1.2 mm anterior and 1.4 mm lateral to bregma; Franklin and Paxinos, 1997) to allow insertion of an electrode/micropipette assembly (lowered 2.5–4.0 mm ventral to the skull surface). The assembly consisted of a Nafion-coated carbon fiber electrode (30 μm diameter) attached to a single-barrel pipette (tip opening: 10–20 μm) such that tips were parallel and separated by 200–300 μm. Pipettes were filled with 200 μM DA and 100 μM ascorbic acid (in 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline, pH 7.4).

High-speed chronoamperometric measurements were made using an IVEC-10/FAST-12 system (Quanteon, LLC, Lexington, KY), which applied square-wave pulses of 0.00–0.55 V (with respect to reference) at a frequency of 5 Hz. For each recording, a stable background current was established and set to zero prior to pressure-ejecting DA (5–20 psi for 0.1–2.5 s) at calibrated volumes (10–300 nl). Resulting oxidation currents were digitally integrated during the last 80 ms of each 100-ms pulse, and signal changes were converted to DA concentrations based on an in vitro calibration (Gerhardt et al, 1984). Two signal parameters were analyzed from DA oxidation currents: maximal amplitude (Amax) and signal decay time (T80). Amax reflects the maximal extracellular DA concentration detected, whereas T80 is the time for the signal to rise to Amax and decay by 80%. Both parameters are affected by DAT inhibitors (eg, Zahniser et al, 1999) and reflect changes in DA clearance (Cass et al, 1993; May et al, 1988).

For a given assembly and recording location, an ejection volume was chosen that resulted in Amax values of 0.75–3.85 μM. Ejections were made at 5-min intervals, and signals used to determine ‘baseline’ were established when Amax and T80 time varied by 15% for two consecutive applications. At 4 min after the second baseline DA application, mice were injected (i.p.) with either saline or 10 mg/kg cocaine, and subsequent DA ejections were given 1 min later and then every 5 min for the next 30 min. Data were normalized by obtaining a mean value for parameters during the two-point baseline period, setting this value as 100%, and expressing all data (including the two baseline data points) as a percentage of baseline. In one ISS and one ILS mouse, recordings were obtained following both saline and cocaine injections; saline treatment occurred first and recordings were obtained at striatal locations that were separated by at least 200 μm. After experiments were completed, a small current was passed through the recording electrode to produce a marking lesion. The brain was then removed and stored in buffered formalin (4% w/v) for at least 3 days. Subsequently, coronal sections (40 μm) at the level of the striatum were made using a vibratome, mounted to glass slides, and stained with cresyl violet to localize recording sites.

Statistical Analysis

Locomotor activity and rearing data were analyzed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey's post hoc tests. Striatal cocaine levels, correlations between cocaine levels and locomotor activity and [3H]WIN 35,428 binding parameters were analyzed using Student's t-tests. For in vivo electrochemistry, baseline measures of DAT function (eg Amax, T80) were evaluated using unpaired Student's t-test, while strain differences in the effects of drug treatment were evaluated using a mixed, two-factor ANOVA followed by Tukey's post hoc tests. In all cases, p<0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Materials

Chemicals were purchased from either Sigma/RBI (St Louis, MO) or Fisher (Pittsburgh, PA). (−)cocaine HCl and WIN 35,428 were obtained from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (RTI International, Research Triangle Park, NC). [3H]WIN 35,428 was purchased from PerkinElmer Life Sciences Products (Boston, MA).

RESULTS

Effect of Novelty/Habituation on Locomotor Activity of ILS and ISS Mice

The locomotor response of ILS and ISS mice to novelty was tested on day 1 during the first 30 min that the mice were in the open field chambers. As shown in Figure 1, ILS and ISS mice exhibited similar locomotor activity when placed in a novel environment. During this same time period on day 2, both mouse strains showed a significant decrease in locomotor activity relative to day 1 (Figure 1, inset). Between-strain analysis revealed that on day 2 ILS mice exhibited significantly less activity compared to ISS mice, suggesting that ILS mice were more habituated to the testing apparatus (Figure 1, inset).

Figure 1
figure 1

Time course for the effects of novelty/habituation on locomotor activity in ILS and ISS mice tested on two consecutive days. Inset: Locomotor activity (mean±SEM, n=43–44) during 1–30 min, over which the data were averaged for the response to novelty. Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference in the response of ILS and ISS mice to novelty between days 1 and 2 ((F(1,171)=11.07, p<0.05)). Tukey's post hoc analysis found no significant differences in the response of ILS and ISS mice to novelty on day 1 (p>0.05). However, on day 2, ILS mice had significantly lower locomotor activity than ISS mice (#p<0.05).

Effect of Cocaine on Locomotor and Rearing Activities

Following the habituation procedure, we then determined the dose–response relationship for cocaine-induced activation in the two mouse strains. To do this, ILS and ISS mice that had received two saline injections on day 1 were injected on day 2, first with saline and then with either saline or cocaine (10, 15, or 20 mg/kg, i.p.). The time course for cocaine (20 mg/kg)-induced locomotor activity is shown in Figure 2 (inset). Compared to their saline-treated cohorts, cocaine significantly increased locomotor activity in ILS, but not ISS, mice (Figure 2). Between-strain analysis revealed that at all doses of cocaine tested, ILS mice exhibited higher locomotor activity, compared to ISS mice (Figure 2). The locomotor activity of cocaine-treated ISS mice was not significantly different from the saline-treated mice. However a nonsignificant trend toward increased activity was seen at 20 mg/kg cocaine. Strain differences in cocaine-induced locomotor activation were not due to differences in rearing activity, since no significant differences were observed in cocaine-induced rearing between ILS and ISS mice (data not shown). There were also no strain differences in the locomotor activity of ILS and ISS mice during the 30 min period following the first saline injection and prior to cocaine injection (data not shown). Thus, baseline differences in locomotor activity do not contribute to strain differences in cocaine-induced locomotor activity.

Figure 2
figure 2

Dose–response relationships for locomotor activity induced by cocaine in habituated ILS and ISS mice on day 2 of the treatment protocol. Inset: Time–response for locomotor activity induced by injections of saline (first arrow) and cocaine (20 mg/kg, i.p.; second arrow). For the dose–response analysis, locomotor activity was averaged between 91 and 120 min (mean±SEM; 9–13 mice/group). Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant genotype × drug interaction ((F(3,72)=3.94, p<0.05)). Tukey's post hoc analysis indicated that ILS mice had higher locomotor cocaine-induced activity compared to the saline-treated group (#p<0.05) and that cocaine had no significant effect in ISS mice. At all doses of cocaine, ILS mice exhibited significantly higher locomotor activity compared to ISS mice (*p<0.05).

Cocaine Levels in Striatum

To test whether differences in cocaine pharmacokinetics between ILS and ISS mice might account for their differential behavioral sensitivity to the drug, we administered 10, 15, or 20 mg/kg (i.p.) cocaine to a separate group of habituated mice. These mice were killed 30 min after injection in order to measure striatal drug concentrations at the time of maximal behavioral activation. The results of these experiments are summarized in Table 1. No significant differences in striatal cocaine levels were observed between ILS and ISS mice at any of the doses. Furthermore, striatal cocaine levels and cocaine-induced locomotor activity were not significantly correlated within each mouse strain at any of the cocaine doses tested (Table 1).

Table 1 Striatal Cocaine Levels and Correlations between Striatal Cocaine Levels and Cocaine-Stimulated Locomotor Activity in ILS and ISS Mice

[3H]WIN 35,428 Binding in Striatum

The affinity and number of DATs in striatal membranes were determined from indirect [3H]WIN 35,428 saturation curves (Figure 3). Affinities for the radioligand were similar between ILS and ISS mice (Ki values: 9.5±1.1 and 8.8±0.3 nM, respectively). However, ILS mice had a significantly lower number of DATs (24%), compared to ISS mice (Bmax values: 2.9±0.2 and 3.6±0.1 pmol/mg protein, respectively).

Figure 3
figure 3

[3H]WIN 35,428/WIN 35,428 saturation binding isotherms in striatal membranes of drug-naïve ILS and ISS mice. Ki and Bmax values were derived from nonlinear curve fitting. While WIN 35,428 affinities were similar in both strains, ILS mice had significantly lower number of DATs compared to ISS mice (mean±SEM; *p<0.05, n=4 mice/strain).

In Vivo DAT Function in Striatum

In preparation for in vivo chronoamperometric recordings, drug-naïve mice were anesthetized with urethane. Not surprisingly, ISS mice required more urethane to maintain a surgical level of anesthesia (De Fiebre et al, 1992; Wehner et al, 1992). The mean dose used in ISS mice was 2.20±0.70 g/kg, whereas it was 1.67±0.04 g/kg in ILS mice. This difference corresponded to 32% higher dose in ISS mice and was statistically significant (t18=7.18, p<0.001). In our sample of 12 recordings/strain from 10 mice/strain, there were no significant differences in baseline Amax (2 μM) or in the volume of DA needed to attain these signal amplitudes (Table 2). However, there were strain differences in the signal decay time, or T80, of exogenously applied DA. On average, DA was cleared 27% more slowly in ILS mice, compared to ISS mice (Table 2).

Table 2 In Vivo DAT Function during Baseline Recording

As shown in Figure 4a, cocaine (10 mg/kg) had variable effects on Amax in the striatum of both ILS and ISS mice. While there was an overall trend for cocaine to increase Amax in both strains, this effect was not consistent within either group at any time point during the 30-min postinjection period. In contrast, measures of signal decay time were less variable (Figure 4b). In ILS mice, T80 was significantly increased by 28%, compared to baseline, in the 30-min period following injection. Despite the fact that cocaine did not significantly alter T80 in ISS mice, there was a trend for T80 to increase following cocaine injection. Therefore, no significant strain differences were found in the effects of cocaine. In both ILS and ISS mice, saline injection did not significantly alter either Amax or T80 (data not shown).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of cocaine on the in vivo DA clearance parameters Amax (a) and T80 (b) in dorsomedial striatum of ILS and ISS mice. Arrows indicate time of cocaine (10 mg/kg, i.p.) injection. Mean±SEM, n=10 mice/strain. (a) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA found that relative to baseline, cocaine trended to increase Amax in both strains, but this was not statistically significant (F(8,77)=0.11), p>0.05)). (b) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA found no significant between-strain differences in the effects of cocaine on T80 (F(8,77)=0.43), p>0.05)). However, cocaine significantly increased signal decay time by up to 28% over baseline in ILS, but not ISS, mice (*p<0.05). Insets: Representative chronoamperometric recordings of in vivo DA clearance in dorsomedial striatum indicating the parameters analyzed (Amax (a) and T80 (b)) from the electrochemical DA oxidation signals.

DISCUSSION

The results from this study confirm our previous findings and indicate that ILS mice, compared to ISS mice, show consistently greater responsiveness to the locomotor-stimulant effects of cocaine when either short (60 min; Hanania and Zahniser, 2002) or prolonged (two 60-min) habituation sessions are used. In addition, we provide evidence for differences in striatal DAT function and inhibition by cocaine in the two mouse strains, likely reflecting the lower number of striatal DATs in the ILS mice. These differences could help to explain the differential sensitivity of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine-induced locomotor activation.

In rodents, habituation to the testing environment has been shown to influence the locomotor-stimulant effects of various drugs of abuse (Koechling et al, 1991; Mazurski and Beninger, 1987). For example, LS and SS mice tested with saline on day 1 and cocaine on day 2 exhibited similar cocaine-induced locomotor stimulation (Jones et al, 1991). However, George and Ritz (1990) found that without habituation, cocaine induced higher locomotor activity in SS mice than in LS mice, and De Fiebre et al (1989) found that LS mice had higher cocaine-induced arm entries when they were placed in a Y-maze. We have previously found that following a brief habituation period, cocaine enhances locomotor activity of ILS mice to a greater extent than ISS mice (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002). The disparate results (De Fiebre et al, 1989; George and Ritz, 1990; Jones et al, 1991; Hanania and Zahniser, 2002) suggest that environmental factors could be important in the differential sensitivity of LS/SS and ILS/ISS mice to the locomotor-stimulant effects of cocaine. Thus, differences in exploratory behaviors in a novel environment between these mice might explain the contradictory results. However, in the current study, we found that treating ILS and ISS mice with saline in the activity chambers on the first test day did not alter their sensitivity to the locomotor-stimulant effects of either saline (ILS=ISS) or cocaine (ILS>ISS) on the second test day. It is also noteworthy that in a preliminary study testing locomotor activation in four ILS and three ISS mice with no previous experience in the testing environment (ie, no habituation), ILS and ISS mice traveled an average of 5000 and 3000 cm, respectively, during the 20-min period following a 10 mg/kg cocaine injection (T Hanania and NR Zahniser, unpublished observations). Thus, it is unlikely that the disparate results in the literature are due to differences in novelty responses in either the ILS/ISS or LS/SS strains. Conflicting results in inbred vs outbred strains may instead be due to genetic differences that likely resulted from the inbreeding process. In addition, our data support previous studies showing that response to novelty does not influence the response of rats to acute cocaine (Gulley et al, 2003; Sabeti et al, 2002; also see Chefer et al, 2003).

Because we used a multiple-day habituation procedure that included successive saline injections, it is possible that the behavioral responses of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine were influenced by the unexpected outcome of receiving a drug on the second test day, rather than another saline injection. In other words, mice in the two strains might differ in the extent to which they form and/or express learned associations between the environment and expected outcomes. While this issue has not been addressed specifically, conditioned behavioral responses have been assessed in the outbred lines. For example, LS and SS mice exhibit similar learning and subsequent maintenance of fixed-ratio responding for water in an operant-reinforcement paradigm (Elmer and George, 1994). In addition, when ILS and ISS mice are given cocaine after a brief habituation period, ILS mice still exhibit greater cocaine-induced locomotor behavior (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002). Thus, it is unlikely that strain differences in the response to unexpected outcomes can explain the robust differences in behavior we observed previously (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002) and in the current study.

The differential responsiveness of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine-induced locomotion is not influenced by differences in striatal cocaine pharmacokinetics. As shown in Table 1, all doses of cocaine tested resulted in similar striatal cocaine levels in the two mouse strains. Furthermore, there was no significant correlation between striatal cocaine levels and cocaine-stimulated locomotor activity within each mouse strain. Our findings are in agreement with other reports showing no differences in brain cocaine levels in mice or rats, despite their differential sensitivity to cocaine-induced locomotion (Ruth et al, 1988) or to developing cocaine self-administration behavior (Piazza et al, 2000; Rocha et al, 1998). It is possible, however, that there are strain differences in cocaine levels in brain regions other than the striatum that could influence the differential sensitivity of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine-induced locomotion. For example, Wiener and Reith (1990) found a significant correlation between cocaine-induced locomotor activity and cocaine levels in mouse cortex. In addition, we recently found a small (r2=0.18), but significant, correlation between cocaine-induced locomotor activity and cocaine levels in NAc, but not dorsal striatum, of rats (Gulley et al, 2003). However, since we have previously shown that other DAT inhibitors such as amphetamine and GBR 12909 also stimulate higher locomotor activity in ILS mice, compared to ISS mice (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002), it is unlikely that pharmacokinetics alone can explain the differential behavioral sensitivity of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine.

Inhibition of striatal DAT is clearly critical for psychostimulant-induced behaviors. In DAT knockdown mice that express 10% of striatal DAT or in DAT knockout mice, amphetamine and cocaine either attenuate or fail to stimulate locomotor activation (Giros et al, 1996; Sora et al, 1998; Spielewoy et al, 2001; Trinh et al, 2003; Zhuang et al, 2001). Here, we found that ILS mice have 24% fewer striatal DAT binding sites, compared to ISS mice. Based on occupation theory, cocaine would be expected to occupy the same percentage of DATs in ILS and ISS mice since their DATs have the same affinity for cocaine. However, the higher number of DATs in the ISS mice may represent ‘spare transporters’ so that lower doses of cocaine, such as used here, may cause significant increases in extracellular DA and thereby greater locomotor activation only in the ILS mice. Janowsky et al (2001) found that DAT expression in the striatum is genetically correlated with cocaine- and methamphetamine-induced locomotor activity in BXD recombinant inbred mice. Furthermore, the same group reported an inverse correlation between the number of striatal DATs and locomotor activity induced by cocaine (10 mg/kg). These findings are consistent with our results showing higher cocaine-stimulated locomotor activity and lower striatal DATs in ILS mice. However, another plausible explanation would be that striatal extracellular DA concentrations differ between ILS and ISS mice. The modeling results of Wu et al (2001a, 2001b) point to a role for differential DA release, rather than DAT number, being the critical determinant for sensitivity to cocaine. If ILS mice have higher extracellular DA concentrations than ISS mice, then the transporters in ILS mice would be more readily saturated after cocaine inhibition, leading to enhanced drug effects. This possibility remains to be explored.

It was important to test whether the ILS/ISS strain differences in striatal DAT number had functional consequences. We and others have used detailed kinetic analyses to show that in vivo electrochemical measures of DA signal decay time reflect transporter activity (Cass et al, 1993; May et al, 1988; Wu et al, 2001a, 2001b). Here, we used in vivo high-speed chronoamperometry and found that exogenous DA applied in the striatum was cleared at an 27% slower rate in ILS mice, compared to ISS mice. These baseline differences in DAT function are consistent with the differences we observed in DAT number between the two strains. It is noteworthy that in these experiments, ILS mice required 32% less urethane than ISS mice to maintain a surgical level of anesthesia. However, this strain difference in total anesthetic dose, which has been demonstrated previously (De Fiebre et al, 1992; Wehner et al, 1992), is not surprising given that these animals were selectively bred for their sedative response to ethanol (Markel et al, 1995; McClearn and Kakihana, 1981). Furthermore, it is unlikely to account for the differences we observed in DAT activity because urethane anesthesia has no effect on exogenous DA clearance in rats (Sabeti et al, 2003).

Despite the fact that cocaine significantly increased T80 from baseline in striata of ILS mice, no significant effects of cocaine on either Amax or T80 were observed between the two strains. These results are consistent with the results we obtained in in vitro experiments showing no strain difference in cocaine inhibition of striatal [3H]DA uptake (Hanania and Zahniser, 2002). In the present study, we analyzed the effects of cocaine on DAT activity using a dose (10 mg/kg) that produces significant locomotor activation in ILS, but not ISS, mice. It is possible that significant strain differences in cocaine-induced inhibition of DAT function might be observed if higher doses were tested. Regardless, the present analysis of cocaine-induced changes in DAT function, especially when considered along with the between-strain differences we observed in baseline DAT activity, suggests that differences in DAT number and function contribute to, but are not solely responsible for, the differential behavioral effects of cocaine between ILS and ISS mice.

In addition to DAT, cocaine inhibits the NE transporter (NET), and NE can influence cocaine-induced locomotor activity. For example, inhibition of α1-adrenergic receptors attenuates cocaine-stimulated activity in rats (Drouin et al, 2002); at lower doses, the selective DAT inhibitor GBR 12783 stimulates less locomotor activation in α1b knockout mice than in wild-type mice (Villégier et al, 2003). In NET knockout mice, i.p. cocaine injection results in greater locomotor stimulation than in wild-type mice (Xu et al, 2000) whereas i.v. cocaine administration induces less locomotor stimulation, compared to wild-type mice (Mead et al, 2002). The opposing results in these studies could be due to the different routes of cocaine administration, and subsequent differences in drug metabolism and distribution (Mead et al, 2002). Alternatively, the stress caused by an i.p. injection could increase NE levels to a greater extent than an i.v. injection, and this could account for the differential behavioral responses of NET knockout mice to cocaine (Pacak et al, 1995; Mead et al, 2002; Reith et al, 1997). In any case, these results underscore the potential contribution of NET to differential cocaine activation. Studies from our laboratory found that ILS mice have 30% fewer brain NETs than ISS mice (HM Haughey, AL Kaiser, TE Johnson, B Bennett, JM Sikela, NR Zahniser, unpublished observations). If fewer NETs result in supersensitivity to systemic cocaine-induced locomotor activity (Xu et al, 2000), then lower NET number in ILS mice might also contribute to the greater behavioral sensitivity of these mice to the stimulant effects of cocaine.

Cocaine also inhibits the 5-HT transporter (SERT). Interestingly, SERT antagonists can substitute for cocaine in conditioned place preference studies in DAT knockout mice (Mateo et al, 2004) and can modulate the locomotor-stimulant effects of cocaine in rodents (Bubar et al, 2003; Reith et al, 1991). Previously, we found no significant differences in the number of cortical SERTs between ILS and ISS mice (Hanania et al, 2002). However, preliminary data show differential strain modulation of cocaine-induced activity by the SERT inhibitor fluoxetine, as well as by the various 5-HT receptor antagonists (T Hanania, AC McCreary, DO Salaz, AM Lyons, NR Zahniser, unpublished observations). Therefore, strain differences in the 5-HT system could also contribute to the differential behavioral sensitivity of ILS and ISS mice to cocaine.

Although cocaine may increase DA levels and locomotor activity to a greater extent in ILS mice via direct inhibition of DAT, both NE and 5-HT systems interact with the DA system, as well as with each other, to modulate psychostimulant-induced locomotor activity and DA neurotransmission (Auclair et al, 2002; Broderick and Phelix, 1997; Mateo et al, 2004; Munoz et al, 2003; Shi et al, 2000). Thus, cocaine inhibition of NET and SERT indirectly increasing DA levels to a greater extent in ILS mice may also contribute to the differential locomotor-stimulant effects of cocaine in ILS and ISS mice. These possibilities remain to be examined.

In summary, our data suggest that differences in DAT number and function contribute to the differential initial sensitivity of ILS and ISS mice to the locomotor-stimulant effects of cocaine. The opposing responsiveness of these mice to cocaine makes them a valuable model for studying not only DA systems, but also other monoamine systems, that potentially contribute to this phenotype, as well as for elucidating genes that mediate initial sensitivity to cocaine.