INTRODUCTION

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is one of several brain regions reported to be associated with mood disorders. The PFC plays an important role in information processing and integration. As a function of this region, both cognitive and emotional stimuli are evaluated, facilitating flexibility in decision-making and allowing the individual to respond adaptively to environmental cues and changes. Neuroimaging studies have linked the development of mood disorders with specific structures within the PFC, including the dorsolateral PFC, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the orbital frontal cortex (OFC). The volume of the OFC has been shown to be smaller in geriatric depression (Lai et al, 2000), and a smaller OFC volume was associated with functional impairment in depressives and controls (Taylor et al, 2003).

Better understanding of OFC function is thus likely to inform us about depression, particularly in the area of late-life depression. Previous studies in the neuroscience literature have implicated the left PFC in relational processing, which can be thought of as the binding of different pieces of information (Badgaiyan et al, 2002). Several experiments found increased activation in the left PFC during relational retrieval tasks (Mottaghy et al, 1999; Rugg et al, 1999; Simons et al, 2001). A recent study reported that the left inferior PFC was associated with retrieval on both the individual items that are necessary for relational retrieval and the link between these items (Badgaiyan et al, 2002). Ventromedial frontal lobe damage has been shown to increase false recognition and intrusion errors (Parkin et al, 1996). Damage to the dorsolateral PFC does not produce these errors (Schacter et al, 1998).

Based on these studies, we sought to characterize the relationship between OFC volume and cognitive function in depressed and nondepressed elders. We specifically examined performance on the Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT) (Benton, 1974), a widely used instrument that assesses visual perception, visual memory, and visuoconstructive abilities. As it measures perception of spatial relations and memory for newly learned material, the BVRT requires intact memory and relational processing. Subjects with impairment in these areas tend to make perseverative errors on the BVRT. We hypothesized that, among depressives, smaller left OFC volumes would be associated with greater numbers of perseverative errors on the BVRT and negatively associated with the BVRT total correct score.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Sample

All subjects were participants in the National Institute of Mental Health—sponsored Duke University Mental Health Clinical Research Center (MHCRC) for the Study of Depression in Later Life. The patient group was restricted to subjects 60 years or older with a diagnosis of major depressive disorder. Exclusion criteria included (1) another major psychiatric illness; (2) current or past alcohol or drug dependence; (3) primary neurologic illness, including dementia; (4) medications or medical illness that would prevent the participants from completing neuropsychological testing (eg sedation from narcotic medications, severe visual impairment); (5) physical disability that precluded cognitive testing; and (6) metal in the body that precluded magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).

Control subjects were recruited from a listing of over 1900 community dwelling elders from the Aging Center Subject Registry at Duke University; these individuals have expressed a willingness to participate in the Duke Aging Center Research. Eligible control subjects had a nonfocal neurological examination, no self-report of neurological or depressive illness, and no evidence of a depression diagnosis based on the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS; Robins et al, 1981) portion of the Duke Depression Evaluation Schedule (Landerman et al, 1989).

The Duke University Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved this study. The study's purpose and procedures were explained to all subjects; those providing written informed consent were enrolled.

Assessment Procedures

At baseline, a study geriatric psychiatrist administered a standardized clinical assessment to each subject. The assessment included the Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale (Montgomery and Asberg, 1979) and the Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al, 1975). A trained interviewer administered the DIS to all subjects to assess symptoms of major depression.

Cognitive Assessment

Subjects were administered the BVRT (Benton, 1974) as part of a larger assessment battery that included the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) neuropsychological battery (Morris et al, 1989) and digit span forward and backward. The BVRT consists of 10 cards, each consisting of one or more simple geometric designs. The card is exposed for 10 s, and the subject must draw what he saw immediately after its removal. The test requires spatial conceptualization, immediate recall, and visuomotor reproduction. We chose to examine the BVRT because it was one test in our battery in which we identified and counted perseverative errors.

MRI Variables

MRI acquisition

All subjects were screened for the presence of cardiac pacemakers, neurostimulators, metallic implants, metal in the orbit, aneurysm clips, or any other condition for which MRI was contraindicated. Subjects were imaged with a 1.5-T whole-body Signa MRI system (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI) using the standard head (volumetric) radiofrequency coil. Padding was used to immobilize the head without causing discomfort. The scanner alignment light was used to adjust the head tilt and rotation so that the axial plane lights passed across the cantho-meatal line, and the sagittal lights were aligned with the center of the nose. A rapid sagittal localizer scan was acquired to confirm the alignment. A dual-echo, fast spin-echo acquisition was obtained in the axial plane for morphometry of cerebral structures. The pulse sequence parameters were repetition time=4000 ms, echo time=30, 135 ms, 32 kHz (±16 kHz) full imaging bandwidth, echo train length=16, a 256 × 256 matrix, 3-mm section thickness, one excitation and a 20-cm field of view. The images were acquired in two separate acquisitions, with a 3-mm gap between sections for each acquisition. The second acquisition was offset by 3 mm from the first so that the resulting data set consisted of contiguous sections.

MRI processing

Images were archived as a normal procedure on magneto-optical disks in the MRI center and transferred to the Duke Neuropsychiatric Imaging Research Laboratory (NIRL) for processing on SUN workstations. Volume measurements used an NIRL-modified version of MrX Software, which was created by GE Corporate Research and Development (Schnectady, NY) and originally modified by Brigham and Women's Hospital (Boston, MA) for image segmentation.

The basic segmentation protocol is a supervised, semiautomated method that has been described previously (Byrum et al, 1996; Kikinis et al, 1992). Once the brain was segmented into tissue types and the nonbrain tissue stripped away through a masking procedure, the total hemispheric gray and white matter volumes were calculated. The total left and right gray and white matter volume was defined as the total brain volume.

Next, specific regions of interest (ROIs) were assessed using tracing and connectivity functions. The medial OFC was traced bilaterally, and a mask was created and applied to the segmented brain. The neuroanatomic borders of the OFC used for this study have been previously described (Lai et al, 2000). To summarize the method briefly, the inferior border begins with the first appearance of the frontal lobe tissue as one moves superiorly from the base of the brain on consecutive axial slices. As one moves superiorly, the limen insula (temporal stem) appears connecting the orbital frontal gyrus (OFG) and temporal lobes. The posterior boundary of OFG was determined by the anterior appearance of the circular insular sulcus. If the cistern of the cerebral vallecula was greater than or equal to 20 pixels across, then lines were drawn from the circular insular sulcus to the lateral end of the cistern of the cerebral vallecula on each hemisphere. If the cistern was less than 20 pixels across, then a line was drawn joining the circular insular sulci of each hemisphere. The entire lateral extent of the frontal lobe was included until the superior border was reached. To determine the most superior slice to include in the OFG, each hemisphere was assessed separately. If the cerebrospinal fluid of the olfactory sulcus plus the gray matter extending posteriorly from the sulcus was at least 3/4 of the OFG length (anterior to posterior), the OFG was included in that hemisphere.

The final step was to run a summarizing program that calculated the volume within the specific OFC ROI defined by the analyst.

All technicians received extensive training by experienced volumetric analysts. Reliability was established by repeated measurements on multiple MRI scans before raters were approved to process the study data. Intraclass correlation coefficients were as follows: total brain=0.998, left medial OFC=0.9, and right medial OFC=0.9.

Analytic Strategy

Bivariate analyses were performed on variables comparing means and proportions of patients to those of controls from the study. Group differences for categorical variables were analyzed using the χ2 test for sex and Fisher's exact test for race. For all continuous variables a t-test was used to compare means between groups.

A series of linear regression models were constructed to test the relationship between BVRT perseverative errors and the proportion of OFC (left, right, and total) volume to the total brain volume (ie normalized OFC volumes). We decided a priori to control for the effects of age and years of education in all regression models because these factors are known to independently affect performance on the BVRT. While sex differences have also been shown for some neuropsychological tests, sex may not affect BVRT performance in the elderly (Coman et al, 1999), so we decided not to automatically include it a priori in our models.

Similar models were constructed with number correct on BVRT as the independent variable.

RESULTS

The sample consisted of 30 depressed and 40 nondepressed control subjects. As shown in Table 1, the mean age for both groups was about 70 years. The control group had significantly more female subjects than the depressed group. The two groups did not differ significantly by race, education, or MMSE score. The mean MADRS score for the depressed group was 26.67 (±5.66).

Table 1 Sample Characteristics and Between-Group Comparisons

Table 1 also shows performance on BVRT and MRI brain volumes. The control group had a higher total correct score on the BVRT than the depressed group, a statistically significant difference. There was no difference in the number of perseverative errors between groups. The control group had larger volumes of left, right, and total OFC, although the difference for right OFC was not statistically significant. There was no difference in the total brain volume between the two groups.

We performed a series of regression models to examine the associations between BVRT performance and left, right, and total OFC volume normalized for (divided by) the total brain volume. Included in the models were total correct on BVRT, with age and education also included as covariates. We examined depressives, controls as well as both groups combined (N=70). We then did the same for BVRT perseverative errors, also controlling for age and education.

In none of the models was BVRT performance (correct or perseverations) significantly associated with either total or right normalized OFC volume. We present the results examining the relationship between BVRT performance and left normalized OFC volume.

In a regression model (see Table 2) controlling for the effects of age and education, left OFC volume (normalized for total brain volume) was associated with total correct on BVRT for the entire sample. In separate models of normalized left OFC volume (not shown), when we examined the groups separately, BVRT total correct was not significant (p<0.172 for depressed and p<0.556 for controls).

Table 2 Linear Regression Models of the Left Orbital Frontal Cortex Volume, Normalized for Total Brain Volume for the Entire Sample

In another regression model (see Table 3) involving the depressed patients only, when we controlled for the effects of age and education, left OFC volume (normalized for total brain volume) was associated with total perseverative errors on BVRT. In a separate model (not shown), when the depressed and control groups were combined, perseverative errors were not significant (p<0.564).

Table 3 Linear Regression Models of the Left Orbital Frontal Cortex Volume, Normalized for Total Brain Volume for Depressed Patients Only

As our regression models were significant only for left normalized hippocampal volume, we present a summary of post hoc bivariate analyses examining MMSE, MADRS, and sex effects on left normalized hippocampal volume. For the entire group, the total MMSE score was associated with total BVRT correct (p<0.0001), but not with the number of perseverative errors (p<0.2400) or left normalized OFC volume (p<0.6380). Sex was not associated with BVRT total correct (p<0.8040), perseverative errors (p<0.9816), or with left normalized OFC volume (p<0.8101). For the depressed group, the total MADRS score was not associated with total correct on BVRT (p<0.1598), BVRT perseverative errors (p<0.4476), or with left normalized OFC volume (p<0.7689). Among depressives, the MMSE score was associated with BVRT total correct (p<0.001), but it was not associated with perseverative errors (p<0.6704) or with left normalized OFC volume (p<0.2860). Sex was associated with left normalized OFC volume (p<0.0083), but it was not associated with BVRT total correct (p<0.1250) or perseverative errors (p<0.0988).

We then constructed subsequent models controlling for sex effects. For the entire sample, the BVRT number correct remained significantly associated with left normalized OFC volume after controlling for age, sex, and education (p<0.0376). Among patients, when sex was added as a covariate along with age and education, the association between number of perseverative errors on BVRT and normalized left OFC volume fell short of significance (p<0.0746).

DISCUSSION

The major finding of this study was that the left OFC volume was associated with performance on the BVRT, both total score and number of perseverative errors. The results remained statistically significant after controlling for the effects of age and education. Total correct on BVRT was significant for the entire sample, and, although it did not reach statistical significance (p<0.172), the trend was similar for the 30 depressed patients. Conversely, among the depressed patients, perseverative errors were significantly associated with left OFC volumes controlling for age and education.

These results are consistent with findings in the neuroscience literature linking the left PFC with relational processing, in particular with retrieval of relevant information during episodic memory tasks (Badgaiyan et al, 2002). Activation of the left inferior PFC has been reported during retrieval experiments (Mottaghy et al, 1999; Rugg et al, 1999; Simons et al, 2001) and during encoding tasks (Dolan and Fletcher, 1997; Mottaghy et al, 1999). A recent functional MRI study found activity in the left PFC when participants thought about a just-seen item and in the right PFC when participants noted whether an item had been presented previously (Johnson et al, 2003).

Several neuroscience studies have focused specifically on the OFC. One study reported that the processing of stimuli that deviate from expectations involves the OFC selectively (Petrides et al, 2002). In contrast, they found that when subjects make explicit decisions on the contents of memory (eg judgments of relative stimulus familiarity), the mid-ventrolateral PFC is involved, and that the mid-dorsolateral PFC is engaged in tasks requiring monitoring of information within working memory. Another functional study using positron emission tomography showed that the left OFC was activated during recall of both novel and practiced unrelated words, while the right OFC was relatively more active during recall of the practiced but not the novel word list (Paradiso et al, 1997). These authors suggested that the left OFC might have a specific role in the retrieval of unstructured verbal information.

The present study also adds to the literature linking geriatric depression with the OFC. As neuroscience studies improve understanding of this brain structure, new findings will better inform research in late-life depression and its cognitive consequences. Clinical correlation of relational processing associated with OFC dysfunction may include many of the previously reported problems associated with executive dysfunction in geriatric depression such as loss of interest in activities and naming difficulties (Alexopoulos et al, 2002). These cognitive changes may be associated with functional impairment seen in late-life depression (Kiosses et al, 2001; Steffens et al, 1999).

Other studies have examined type of error on neuropsychological testing in depression and dementia. Demented patients, compared with controls, have been reported to have greater perseverations and omissions (Eslinger et al, 1988). Omissions are reported as most discriminating of depression when compared to dementia (La Rue et al, 1986). Our findings indicate that, in addition to dementia, perseverative errors are important in late-life depression as well. Future studies would need to determine if the left orbital frontal volume is reduced in both demented and depressed elderly subjects who commit perseverative and other types of errors.

Future research should also include larger samples of depressed patients. It is likely that our sample of 30 depressed patients was not large enough to detect a significant association between total correct on the BVRT and normalized left OFC volume among the depressed group only. Our study focuses on one test in a relatively small sample, so our results should be viewed with a cautious eye. Future research will need to include other clinical neuropsychological measures that assess memory and relational processing. Our study focused on structural imaging; future studies on late-life depression should also include a functional imaging component.